scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Author

Robert Soderberg

Bio: Robert Soderberg is an academic researcher from University of Alabama at Birmingham. The author has contributed to research in topics: Poison control. The author has an hindex of 1, co-authored 1 publications receiving 156 citations.

Papers
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Greater involvement in car crashes and less use of car restraints explains the 64% higher rate of injury for 3 year olds than for infants, and it is time to target the toddlers.
Abstract: Objective. To determine the effect of car restraints on motor vehicle injury rates for children aged 0 to 14 years. Methods. A probability sample of all police-reported car crashes in the United States in 1990 and 1991 was analyzed for injury rates of passengers aged less than 15 years in relation to restraint usage, age, and seating position. Results. Optimal restraint usage (defined as car seats for children 0 to 4 years old and lap shoulder belts for children 5 to 14 years old) was 40%. The use of the car seat was 76% for infants (0 to 12 months old) and 41% for toddlers (1 to 4 year olds). The non use of a restraint was highest for 10 to 14 year olds (43%). The rate of involvement in car crashes for all children was 21.4 (per 1000/yr). The highest rate was the 14 year olds with 29.6 followed by 2 year olds with 26.5. Injury rates were 4.76 (per 1000/yr) for all children. The lowest rate was 2.91 for infants but increased to 4.78 for 3 year olds. The single strongest risk factor for injury was the non use of a restraint. (Adjusted odds ratio 2.7; 95% CI 2.4 to 3.0.) The risk factor for injury for the front seat was 1.5 (95% CI 1.4 to 1.7). Use of the car seat reduced injuries by 60% for 0 to 4 year olds, whereas the lap shoulder harness was only 38% effective for 5 to 14 year olds (P ≤ .001) Injury rates of unrestrained 0 to 4 and 5 to 14 year olds were similar. Conclusions. Greater involvement in car crashes and less use of car restraints explains the 64% higher rate of injury for 3 year olds than for infants. It is time to target the toddlers. Restraints designed for adults are not as effective for the school age child as car seats are for the preschool child. A better restraint for the school age child should be designed and evaluated. Meanwhile, increased usage of current restraints must be encouraged, as they substantially reduce injuries.

157 citations


Cited by
More filters
25 Aug 2004
TL;DR: The Principles of Readability gives a brief introduction to the literacy studies in the U.S. and the research on readability and the readability formulas.
Abstract: The Principles of Readability gives a brief introduction to the literacy studies in the U.S. and the research on readability and the readability formulas.

752 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
11 May 2002-BMJ
TL;DR: Driving while feeling sleepy, driving after five hours or less of sleep, and driving between 2 am and 5 am were associated with a substantial increase in the risk of a car crash resulting in serious injury or death reduction in the prevalence of these three behaviours may reduce the incidence of injury crashes.
Abstract: Objectives: To estimate the contribution of driver sleepiness to the causes of car crash injuries. Design: Population based case control study. Setting: Auckland region of New Zealand, April 1998 to July 1999. Participants: 571 car drivers involved in crashes where at least one occupant was admitted to hospital or killed (“injury crash”); 588 car drivers recruited while driving on public roads (controls), representative of all time spent driving in the study region during the study period. Main outcome measures: Relative risk for injury crash associated with driver characteristics related to sleep, and the population attributable risk for driver sleepiness. Results: There was a strong association between measures of acute sleepiness and the risk of an injury crash. After adjustment for major confounders significantly increased risk was associated with drivers who identified themselves as sleepy (Stanford sleepiness score 4-7 v 1-3; odds ratio 8.2, 95% confidence interval 3.4 to 19.7); with drivers who reported five hours or less of sleep in the previous 24 hours compared with more than five hours (2.7, 1.4 to 5.4); and with driving between 2 am and 5 am compared with other times of day (5.6, 1.4 to 22.7). No increase in risk was associated with measures of chronic sleepiness. The population attributable risk for driving with one or more of the acute sleepiness risk factors was 19% (15% to 25%). Conclusions: Acute sleepiness in car drivers significantly increases the risk of a crash in which a car occupant is injured or killed. Reductions in road traffic injuries may be achieved if fewer people drive when they are sleepy or have been deprived of sleep or drive between 2 am and 5 am. What is already known on this topic Driver sleepiness is considered a potentially important risk factor for car crashes and related injuries but the association has not been reliably quantified Published estimates of the proportion of car crashes attributable to driver sleepiness vary from about 3% to 30% What this study adds Driving while feeling sleepy, driving after five hours or less of sleep, and driving between 2 am and 5 am were associated with a substantial increase in the risk of a car crash resulting in serious injury or death Reduction in the prevalence of these three behaviours may reduce the incidence of injury crashes by up to 19%

741 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This review has tried to delineate behavioral factors that collectively represent the principal cause of three out of five RTCs and contribute to the causation of most of the remaining.
Abstract: Road traffic crashes (RTCs) are responsible for a substantial fraction of morbidity and mortality and are responsible for more years of life lost than most of human diseases. In this review, we have tried to delineate behavioral factors that collectively represent the principal cause of three out of five RTCs and contribute to the causation of most of the remaining. Although sharp distinctions are not always possible, a classification of behavioral factors is both necessary and feasible. Thus, behavioral factors can be distinguished as (i) those that reduce capability on a long-term basis (inexperience, aging, disease and disability, alcoholism, drug abuse), (ii) those that reduce capability on a short-term basis (drowsiness, fatigue, acute alcohol intoxication, short term drug effects, binge eating, acute psychological stress, temporary distraction), (iii) those that promote risk taking behavior with long-term impact (overestimation of capabilities, macho attitude, habitual speeding, habitual disregard of traffic regulations, indecent driving behavior, non-use of seat belt or helmet, inappropriate sitting while driving, accident proneness) and (iv) those that promote risk taking behavior with short-term impact (moderate ethanol intake, psychotropic drugs, motor vehicle crime, suicidal behavior, compulsive acts). The classification aims to assist in the conceptualization of the problem that may also contribute to behavior modification-based efforts.

379 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Children with facial fractures require long-term follow-up to monitor potential growth abnormalities and primary bone grafting is preferred over secondary reconstruction and alloplastic materials should be avoided when possible.

350 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: These data, representing the largest series to date, confirm that blunt C-spine injuries in children are rare and major neurologic sequelae in survivors is uncommon, does not correlate well with cord level, and rarely is complete.

281 citations