scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Author

Sarah L. Walsh

Bio: Sarah L. Walsh is an academic researcher from University of Western Australia. The author has contributed to research in topics: Alarm signal & Predator. The author has an hindex of 2, co-authored 3 publications receiving 15 citations.

Papers
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Many animals produce antipredator calls in response to the presence of a predator, which, in some species, can provide information about the distance to predator as discussed by the authors. But, although previous research has sho...
Abstract: Many animals produce antipredator calls in response to the presence of a predator, which, in some species, can provide information about the distance to predator. Although previous research has sho...

18 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jun 2019-Ethology
TL;DR: Acoustic analyses indicate that Western Australian magpies structurally combine generic alarm calls with acoustically distinct alert calls to produce an alarm alert sequence, providing further evidence for a structural basis of potential syntactic‐like call combinations in the vocal communication system of a group‐living songbird.
Abstract: The ability for humans to create seemingly infinite meaning from a finite set of sounds has likely been a critical component in our success as a species, allowing the unbounded communication of information. Syntax, the combining of meaningful sounds into phrases, is one of the primary features of language that enables this extensive expressivity. The evolutionary history of syntax, however, remains largely debated, and it is only very recently that comparative data for syntax in animals have been revealed. Here, we provide further evidence for a structural basis of potential syntactic‐like call combinations in the vocal communication system of a group‐living songbird. Acoustic analyses indicate that Western Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis) structurally combine generic alarm calls with acoustically distinct alert calls to produce an alarm alert sequence. These results are distinct from previous examples of call combinations as, to our knowledge, evidence for this capacity is yet to be demonstrated in the natural communication of a non‐human species that is capable of vocal learning throughout life. These findings offer prospects for experimental investigation into the presence and function of magpie call combinations, extending our understanding of animal vocal complexity.

9 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The authors used a nonlinear dimensionality reduction analysis and sequential transition analysis to quantitatively describe the non-song combinatorial repertoire of the Western Australian magpie (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis).
Abstract: Comparative studies conducted over the past few decades have provided important insights into the capacity for animals to combine vocal segments at either one of two levels: within- or between-calls. There remains, however, a distinct gap in knowledge as to whether animal combinatoriality can extend beyond one level. Investigating this requires a comprehensive analysis of the combinatorial features characterizing a species' vocal system. Here, we used a nonlinear dimensionality reduction analysis and sequential transition analysis to quantitatively describe the non-song combinatorial repertoire of the Western Australian magpie (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis). We found that (i) magpies recombine four distinct acoustic segments to create a larger number of calls, and (ii) the resultant calls are further combined into larger call combinations. Our work demonstrates two levels in the combining of magpie vocal units. These results are incongruous with the notion that a capacity for multi-level combinatoriality is unique to human language, wherein the combining of meaningless sounds and meaningful words interactively occurs across different combinatorial levels. Our study thus provides novel insights into the combinatorial capacities of a non-human species, adding to the growing evidence of analogues of language-specific traits present in the animal kingdom.

1 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper , the authors found that Western Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis) produce alarm calls containing fewer notes compared to those produced when predators are closer.
Abstract: Many animals provide information about predator proximity in their alarm calls. In response to predators further away, Western Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis) produce alarm calls containing fewer notes compared to those produced when predators are closer. Since the ability to make fine-scale adjustments to antipredator responses by being sensitive to the level of urgency in calls may be beneficial, receivers are expected to be able to appropriately decipher and respond to this information. We conducted playbacks to test whether magpies can respond to urgency information in conspecific alarm calls. Magpies were exposed to low-urgency calls (calls with one note), high-urgency calls (calls with four notes), and one- and four-note control calls. Receivers showed greater levels of responsiveness following playbacks of high-urgency calls compared to playbacks of low-urgency and control calls, providing evidence that magpies can respond to information about the urgency of a predator threat from conspecific alarm calls.

Cited by
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Many animals produce antipredator calls in response to the presence of a predator, which, in some species, can provide information about the distance to predator as discussed by the authors. But, although previous research has sho...
Abstract: Many animals produce antipredator calls in response to the presence of a predator, which, in some species, can provide information about the distance to predator. Although previous research has sho...

18 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
26 Aug 2019-PeerJ
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors used playback experiments to determine whether Western Australian magpies (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis) respond to predator information associated with alarm calls and found that receivers showed significantly greater levels of responsiveness to signallers that previously gave alarm calls in the appropriate context.
Abstract: Alarm calls are a widespread form of antipredator defence and being alerted to the presence of predators by the alarm calls of conspecifics is considered one of the benefits of group living. However, while social information can allow an individual to gain additional information, it can also at times be inaccurate or irrelevant. Such variation in the accuracy of social information is predicted to select for receivers to discriminate between sources of social information. In this study, we used playback experiments to determine whether Western Australian magpies (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis) respond to the predator information associated with alarm calls. Magpies were exposed to the alarm calls of two group members that differed in the threat associated with the alarm call: one call was played in the presence of a predator model while the other was not-in order to establish differences in the predator information provided by each caller. We then played back the alarm calls of the same group members in the absence of the predator model to determine whether magpies responded differently to signallers in response to the previous association between the alarm call and a predator threat. We found that receivers showed significantly greater levels of responsiveness to signallers that previously gave alarm calls in the appropriate context. Thus, the accuracy of threat-based information influenced subsequent receiver response.

13 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is found that females sing at the same amplitude as males, but that male and female carols exhibit differences in frequency, which highlights the importance of studying female song.

12 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is demonstrated that great tit responses to mobbing calls were affected by the number of individuals calling, suggesting that numerical competence plays a central role during mobbing and that great tits reduce uncertainty of information by collating information from several individuals.
Abstract: Numerical competence—the ability to represent, discriminate, and process numerical quantity information—is a widespread cognitive ability in animals that influences survival and reproductive success. Little is known about the role of numerical competence during predator mobbing—when a prey moves toward and harasses a predator. Since being in a larger group dilutes the risk of injury or death during a mobbing event and large groups are more efficient than small groups at repelling predators, the capacity to evaluate the number of mobbers before joining the mobbing flock may be highly beneficial for individuals. We tested whether the strength of the mobbing response of great tits (Parus major), a songbird that frequently mobs predators, is related to the number of callers. The minimum distance to the loudspeaker tended to be lower, and the number of calls produced by great tits was higher during playbacks simulating several callers than during the playbacks of one caller. These results suggest that numerical competence plays a central role during mobbing and that great tits reduce uncertainty of information by collating information from several individuals. We suggest further studies testing whether birds use individual vocal discrimination to assess the number of heterospecifics during mobbing. Can animals count? Although historically the ability to count has distinguished humans apart from the rest of the animal kingdom, studies in the last decades have shown that numerical competence, the ability to represent, discriminate, and process numerical quantity information, is a widespread cognitive ability in animals. While this competence influences an individual’s survival success, little is known about the role of numerical competence during predator mobbing. Using a field-based playback experiment on a population of wild great tits (Parus major), we demonstrate that great tit responses to mobbing calls were affected by the number of individuals calling. The minimum distance to the loudspeaker tended to be lower and the number of calls produced by great tits tended to be higher during playbacks simulating multiple callers than during the playbacks of one caller. Thus, numerical assessments are used to decide whether or not to participate in mobbing responses.

12 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Investigating the vigilance behaviour of free-range naked neck chickens when confronted with alarm calls and songs of a commonly occurring wild passerine found that subjects exhibited an increased vigilance to alarm calls compared to songs, showing that chickens respond to heterospecific signals as wild birds do.
Abstract: The ability to use heterospecific alarm calls is adaptive in the wild, as it provides an opportunity to avoid predators. We now know that several species are able to respond to alarm calls intended for others. However, this capacity has never been investigated in domestic animals. The capacity to use heterospecific alarm calls may be relevant for free-range domestic species, especially when they share predators with wild signallers. Using playback experiments, we investigated the vigilance behaviour of free-range naked neck chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) when confronted with alarm calls (test playbacks) and songs (control playbacks) of a commonly occurring wild passerine, the great tit (Parus major). We found that subjects exhibited an increased vigilance to alarm calls compared to songs, therefore, showing that chickens respond to heterospecific signals as wild birds do. Recently, there has been an increased interest for free-range poultry production, notably because of the benefits of this farming method for chicken welfare. Although future studies are required to address this question, mortality due to predation may be reduced through the implementation of structures in areas frequented by wild heterospecific signallers.

8 citations