scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Institution

Northwestern University

EducationEvanston, Illinois, United States
About: Northwestern University is a education organization based out in Evanston, Illinois, United States. It is known for research contribution in the topics: Population & Transplantation. The organization has 75430 authors who have published 188857 publications receiving 9463252 citations. The organization is also known as: Northwestern & NU.


Papers
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
17 Jul 2009-Small
TL;DR: This Review provides a critical examination of the various interparticle forces (van der Waals, electrostatic, magnetic, molecular, and entropic) that can be used in nanoscale self-assembly.
Abstract: The ability to assemble nanoscopic components into larger structures and materials depends crucially on the ability to understand in quantitative detail and subsequently "engineer" the interparticle interactions. This Review provides a critical examination of the various interparticle forces (van der Waals, electrostatic, magnetic, molecular, and entropic) that can be used in nanoscale self-assembly. For each type of interaction, the magnitude and the length scale are discussed, as well as the scaling with particle size and interparticle distance. In all cases, the discussion emphasizes characteristics unique to the nanoscale. These theoretical considerations are accompanied by examples of recent experimental systems, in which specific interaction types were used to drive nanoscopic self-assembly. Overall, this Review aims to provide a comprehensive yet easily accessible resource of nanoscale-specific interparticle forces that can be implemented in models or simulations of self-assembly processes at this scale.

1,344 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In patients with transthyretin amyloid cardiomyopathy, tafamidis was associated with reductions in all‐cause mortality and cardiovascular‐related hospitalizations and reduced the decline in functional capacity and quality of life as compared with placebo.
Abstract: Background Transthyretin amyloid cardiomyopathy is caused by the deposition of transthyretin amyloid fibrils in the myocardium. The deposition occurs when wild-type or variant transthyretin becomes unstable and misfolds. Tafamidis binds to transthyretin, preventing tetramer dissociation and amyloidogenesis. Methods In a multicenter, international, double-blind, placebo-controlled, phase 3 trial, we randomly assigned 441 patients with transthyretin amyloid cardiomyopathy in a 2:1:2 ratio to receive 80 mg of tafamidis, 20 mg of tafamidis, or placebo for 30 months. In the primary analysis, we hierarchically assessed all-cause mortality, followed by frequency of cardiovascular-related hospitalizations according to the Finkelstein–Schoenfeld method. Key secondary end points were the change from baseline to month 30 for the 6-minute walk test and the score on the Kansas City Cardiomyopathy Questionnaire–Overall Summary (KCCQ-OS), in which higher scores indicate better health status. Results In the prim...

1,340 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, a diffusion model for two-choice real-time decisions is applied to four psychophysical tasks and the model reveals how stimulus information guides decisions and shows how the information is processed through time to yield sometimes correct and sometimes incorrect decisions.
Abstract: The diffusion model for two-choice real-time decisions is applied to four psychophysical tasks. The model reveals how stimulus information guides decisions and shows how the information is processed through time to yield sometimes correct and sometimes incorrect decisions. Rapid two-choice decisions yield multiple empir- ical measures: response times for correct and error responses, the probabilities of correct and error responses, and a variety of interac- tions between accuracy and response time that depend on instructions and task difficulty. The diffusion model can explain all these aspects of the data for the four experiments we present. The model correctly accounts for error response times, something previous models have failed to do. Variability within the decision process explains how errors are made, and variability across trials correctly predicts when errors are faster than correct responses and when they are slower. Making decisions is a ubiquitous part of everyday life. In psychol- ogy, besides being an object of study in its own right, decision making plays a central role in the tasks used to study basic cognitive functions such as memory, perception, and language comprehension. Frequently, the decisions required in these tasks are rapid two-choice decisions, decisions that are based on information that can be described as vary- ing along a single dimension. Two key features of these decisions are that they occur over time—decisions are never reached instantaneous- ly—and that they are error prone. In this article, we present a model to explain this class of decision processes. The goal is to understand what information drives the decision and how the decision process evolves over time to reach correct and incorrect decisions. The problem is dif- ficult because potential models are constrained to explain multiple empirical measures that interact in complex ways. The measures include mean response times for correct and error responses, the shapes of the distributions of the response times, and the probabilities of cor- rect and error responses. The relation between response time and accu- racy is not fixed; it varies according to whether speed or accuracy of performance is emphasized and according to whether one or the other of the responses is more probable or weighted more heavily. In addi- tion, the relation between probability of an error and error response time is not fixed but varies across levels of overall accuracy. Because of these complexities, no previous model has been completely successful. Often, models have dealt with only one measure—accuracy but not response time, or response time but not accuracy. Models that have dealt with response time have usually tried to explain only mean response times for correct responses, not the shapes of response time distributions or response times for errors. Modeling speed-accuracy relationships has usually not been attempted. In this article, we show how the diffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978, 1981, 1985, 1988; Ratcliff, Van Zandt, & McKoon, 1998) can explain all of these aspects of the data for two-choice perceptual decisions. For the first time, the model provides an integrated account of both the information that drives decisions and how that information is processed over time to produce correct and error responses. The main domain of application is to tasks on which response time is typically under a second. The model may apply when response time is greater than 1 s, but at much longer times, decisions are probably based on multiple decision attempts in which the first decision attempted was sometimes not made or made with too little confidence for the response to be based on that decision (with perhaps different informa- tion or different response criteria used in each successive attempt). A major weakness of all of the models for reaction time is the fail- ure to account for error reaction times. Luce (1986) reviewed data and theory for error reaction times and concluded that there are few sys- tematic studies of error reaction times that can be used to produce comprehensive empirical generalizations, nor is there a comprehen- sive theoretical account of error reaction times. Empirically, the rela- tionship between correct and error reaction times varies: Sometimes errors are faster than correct responses (mainly when the task is easy and speed is emphasized); sometimes errors are slower than correct responses (mainly when the task is hard and accuracy is emphasized; see Luce, 1986; Swensson, 1972). Ratcliff et al. (1998, see also Smith & Vickers, 1988) presented data showing individual subjects had a crossover, with error responses faster than correct responses at high accuracy, and error responses slower than correct responses at low accuracy. This pattern is very difficult for models to produce; models predict slow errors or fast errors (e.g., Link & Heath, 1975), but most cannot predict both or predict crossovers. In this article, we show that the diffusion model can explain the relationship between correct and error responses across a range of experimental paradigms while at the same time fitting all the other response time and response probability aspects of the data. The key to the model's success is variability in the decision process: We show this in experiments with perceptual stimuli, but the model is more general than this application; it can potentially have equal success for the two- choice cognitive tasks to which it has been applied previously. These tasks include short- and long-term recognition memory tasks, same/different letter-string matching, lexical decision tasks, numeros- ity judgments, and visual-scanning tasks (Ratcliff, 1978, 1981; Rat- cliff et al., 1998; Strayer & Kramer, 1994).

1,339 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors performed a prospective trial involving 10,273 women with hormone-receptor-positive, human epidermal growth factor receptor 2 (HER2)-negative, axillary node-negative breast cancer.
Abstract: Background The recurrence score based on the 21-gene breast cancer assay predicts chemotherapy benefit if it is high and a low risk of recurrence in the absence of chemotherapy if it is low; however, there is uncertainty about the benefit of chemotherapy for most patients, who have a midrange score. Methods We performed a prospective trial involving 10,273 women with hormone-receptor–positive, human epidermal growth factor receptor 2 (HER2)–negative, axillary node–negative breast cancer. Of the 9719 eligible patients with follow-up information, 6711 (69%) had a midrange recurrence score of 11 to 25 and were randomly assigned to receive either chemoendocrine therapy or endocrine therapy alone. The trial was designed to show noninferiority of endocrine therapy alone for invasive disease–free survival (defined as freedom from invasive disease recurrence, second primary cancer, or death). Results Endocrine therapy was noninferior to chemoendocrine therapy in the analysis of invasive disease–free surv...

1,337 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, an economic model of defensive marketing strategy is developed for complaint management based on Hirschman's exit-voice theory, which is used to reduce the number of customer com...
Abstract: On the basis of Hirschman's exit-voice theory, an economic model of defensive marketing strategy is developed for complaint management. Though many firms strive to reduce the number of customer com...

1,336 citations


Authors

Showing all 76189 results

NameH-indexPapersCitations
George M. Whitesides2401739269833
Ralph B. D'Agostino2261287229636
Daniel Levy212933194778
David Miller2032573204840
Ronald M. Evans199708166722
Michael Marmot1931147170338
Robert C. Nichol187851162994
Scott M. Grundy187841231821
Stuart H. Orkin186715112182
Michael A. Strauss1851688208506
Ralph Weissleder1841160142508
Patrick O. Brown183755200985
Aaron R. Folsom1811118134044
Valentin Fuster1791462185164
Ronald C. Petersen1781091153067
Network Information
Related Institutions (5)
University of Pennsylvania
257.6K papers, 14.1M citations

96% related

Columbia University
224K papers, 12.8M citations

96% related

Yale University
220.6K papers, 12.8M citations

95% related

Harvard University
530.3K papers, 38.1M citations

95% related

Stanford University
320.3K papers, 21.8M citations

95% related

Performance
Metrics
No. of papers from the Institution in previous years
YearPapers
2023275
20221,183
202110,513
202010,260
20199,331
20188,301