scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
JournalISSN: 0196-0202

Ear and Hearing 

Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
About: Ear and Hearing is an academic journal published by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. The journal publishes majorly in the area(s): Hearing loss & Hearing aid. It has an ISSN identifier of 0196-0202. Over the lifetime, 3910 publications have been published receiving 177647 citations. The journal is also known as: Official journal of the American Auditory Society.


Papers
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A model of auditory word recognition, the neighborhood activation model, was proposed, which describes the effects of similarity neighborhood structure on the process of discriminating among the acoustic‐phonetic representations of words in memory.
Abstract: Since the publication of Oldfield's (1966) seminal article, “Things, Words and the Brain,” a great deal of attention has been devoted to the structural organization of words in the mental lexicon. Most of this research, however, has focused on the structure of higher level aspects of lexical representations, namely the semantic and conceptual organization of lexical items in memory (e.g., Miller & Johnson-Laird, 1976; Smith, 1978). As a consequence, little attention has been directed to the structural organization of the representations of sensory and perceptual information used to gain access to these higher level sources of information. The goal of the present investigation was to explore in detail this structure and its implications for perception of spoken words by normal and hearing-impaired listeners. In the present set of studies, structure will be defined specifically in terms of similarity relations among the sound patterns of words. Similarity will serve as the primary means by which the organization of acoustic-phonetic representations in memory will be investigated. We assume that similarity relations among the sound patterns of spoken words represent one of the earliest stages at which the structural organization of the lexicon comes into play. The precise aim of the present investigation was to gain a detailed understanding of the lower level relations between stimulus input, activation of phonetic representations, and, subsequently, recognition of spoken words. The identification of structure with similarity relations among sound patterns of words raises the difficult problem of defining similarity. Similarity, although crucial to the present investigation, is an ill-defined concept in research on speech perception and spoken word recognition, and one that deserves considerably more work in these areas of research (see Mermelstein, Reference Note 7). However, similarity can be approximated by both computational and behavioral predictors of confusion, the approach taken here. Thus, similarity will be defined in terms of a specific computational metric for predicting confusions among phonetic patterns as well as a behavioral, or operational, metric based on the results of a series of perceptual experiments. Having defined structure as the similarity relations among the sound patterns of words, the question arises: Should the structural organization of representations in memory have consequences for spoken word recognition? Consider a content-addressable memory system in which there is no noise either in the signal or the listener (Kohonan, 1980). In such a system, encoding the acoustic-phonetic information in the stimulus word is tantamount to locating the word in memory. In this case, the structural organization of acoustic-phonetic representations in memory would have no consequences for word identification. Instead, the task of spoken word recognition would be identical to phonetic perception, and one need only study phonetic perception to understand how words are recognized. (By phonetic perception we are referring to the perception of individual segments such as consonants and vowels.) It is undeniable that phonetic perception is important in spoken word recognition. It is not undeniable, however, that the human word recognition system operates as a noiseless content-addressable system or that the acoustic-phonetic signal itself is devoid of noise. To begin, the signal is very often less than ideal for the purposes of the listener. Words are typically perceived against a background of considerable ambient noise, reverberation, and the voices of other talkers. In addition, coarticulatory effects and segmental reductions and deletions substantially restructure the phonetic information in a myriad of ways (Luce & Pisoni, 1987). Although such effects may indeed be useful to the listener (Church, Reference Note 3; Elman & McClelland, 1986), they also undoubtedly produce considerable ambiguities in the speech signal, making a strictly content-addressable word recognition system based on phonetic encoding unrealistic. In short, both the noise inherent in the signal as well as the noise against which the signal is perceived make it unlikely that word recognition is accomplished by direct access, based solely on phonetic encoding, to acoustic-phonetic representations in memory. Not only is the speech signal noisy, so too is the recognition system of the listener. Although the human is clearly well adapted for the perception of spoken language, the system by which language is perceived is by no means a perfect one. In normal listeners, encoding, attentional, and memory demands frequently result in the distortion, degradation, or loss of important acoustic-phonetic information. The data on misperceptions alone attest to the fact that spoken word recognition is less than perfect (Bond & Garnes, 1980). In hearing-impaired listeners, the problems faced by normal listeners are exacerbated by impoverished input representations. Thus, again, a strictly content-addressable system does not suffice as a model of human spoken word recognition. The alternative to a noiseless content-addressable system is one in which the stimulus input activates a number of similar acoustic-phonetic representations or candidates in memory, among which the system must choose (Marslen-Wilson, 1989; Marslen-Wilson & Welsh, 1978; Triesman, 1978a, b). In this system, a considerable amount of processing activity involves discriminating among the lexical items activated in memory. Indeed, many current models of word recognition subscribe to the view that word recognition is to a great degree a process of discriminating among competing lexical items (Forster, 1979; Luce, Pisoni, & Goldinger, 1990; Marslen-Wilson, 1989; Marslen-Wilson & Welsh, 1978; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981; McQueen, Cutler, Briscoe, & Norris, 1995; Morton, 1979; Norris, 1994). Given that one of the primary tasks of the word recognition system involves discrimination among lexical items, the study of the structural organization of words in memory takes on considerable importance, especially if structural relations can be shown to influence the ease or difficulty of lexical discrimination, and, subsequently, word recognition and lexical access. By the same token, under the assumption that word recognition involves discrimination and selection among competing lexical items, variations in the ease or difficulty of discriminating among items in memory can enlighten us as to the structural organization of the sound patterns of words. In short, lexical discrimination and structure are so inextricably tied together that the study of one leads to a further understanding of the other. Assuming, then, that structural relations among words should influence spoken word recognition via the process of discrimination, it is important to determine that structural differences among words actually exist. Previous research by Landauer and Streeter (1973) has demonstrated that words vary substantially not only in the number of words to which they are similar, but also in the frequencies of these similar words. These findings suggest that both structural and frequency relations among words may mediate lexical discrimination. Investigation of the behavioral effects of these relations should help us to understand further not only the process of lexical discrimination, but also the organization of the sound patterns of spoken words in memory. The issue of word frequency takes on an important role in the investigation of the structural organization of the sound patterns of words. Numerous studies over the years (Howes, 1954, 1957; Newbigging, 1961; Savin, 1963; Soloman & Postman, 1952) have demonstrated that the ease with which spoken words are recognized is monotonically related to experienced frequency, as measured by some objective count of words in the language. However, little work has been devoted to detailing the interaction of word frequency and structural relations among words (see, however, Triesman, 1978a, b). If word frequency influences the perceptibility of the stimulus word, it should also influence the degree of activation of similar words in memory. Frequency is important, then, in further specifying the relative competition among activated items that are to be discriminated among. The goal of the present investigation was, therefore, to examine the effects of the number and nature of words activated in memory on auditory word recognition. Throughout the ensuing discussion, the term similarity neighborhood will be employed. A similarity neighborhood is defined here as a collection of words that are phonetically similar to a given stimulus word. (The term stimulus word will be used to refer to the word for which a neighborhood is computed.) Similarity neighborhood structure refers to two factors: 1) the number and degree of confusability of words in the neighborhood, and 2) the frequencies of the neighbors. This first factor will be referred to as neighborhood density or neighborhood confusability; the second factor will be called neighborhood frequency. In addition to neighborhood structure, the frequency of the stimulus word itself will be of interest.

1,704 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the impact of certain audit committee characteristics identified by the Blue Ribbon Committee on Improving the Effectiveness of Corporate Audit Committees (BRC) on the likelihood of financial restatement was examined.
Abstract: This study addresses the impact of certain audit committee characteristics identified by the Blue Ribbon Committee on Improving the Effectiveness of Corporate Audit Committees (BRC) on the likelihood of financial restatement. We examine 88 restatements of annual results (without allegations of fraud) in the period 1991–1999, together with a matched pairs control group of firms of similar size, exchange listing, industry and auditor type. We find that the independence and activity level (our proxy for audit committee diligence) of the audit committee exhibit a significant and negative association with the occurrence of restatement. We also document a significant negative association between an audit committee that includes at least one member with financial expertise and restatement. To test the robustness of the results we also consider a sample of 44 fraud and no‐fraud firms and arrive at largely similar findings. Our results underscore the importance of the BRC's recommendations as a means of strengthen...

1,485 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors investigate if the use of a Big 6 auditor is increasing the propensity of a firm's endogenous propensity to generate accruals and find that high-accrual firms have greater scope for aggressive and/or opportunistic strategies.
Abstract: This study investigates if the use of a Big 6 auditor is increasing in the firm's endogenous propensity to generate accruals. High‐accrual firms have greater scope for aggressive and/or opportunist...

1,429 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the authors examined the relationship between audit committee characteristics and the extent of corporate earnings management as measured by the level of income-increasing and income-decreasing abnormal accruals and found that aggressive earnings management is negatively associated with the financial and governance expertise of audit committee members, with indicators of independence, and with the presence of a clear mandate defining the responsibilities of the committee.
Abstract: This study investigates whether the expertise, independence, and activities of a firm's audit committee have an effect on the quality of its publicly released financial information. In particular, we examine the relationship between audit committee characteristics and the extent of corporate earnings management as measured by the level of income‐increasing and income‐decreasing abnormal accruals. Using two groups of U.S. firms, one with relatively high and one with relatively low levels of abnormal accruals in the year 1996, we find a significant association between earnings management and audit committee governance practices. We find that aggressive earnings management is negatively associated with the financial and governance expertise of audit committee members, with indicators of independence, and with the presence of a clear mandate defining the responsibilities of the committee. The association is similar for both income‐increasing and income‐decreasing earnings management, suggesting that audit com...

1,285 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The reliability and validity of the HHIE as well as its brevity, simplicity, and ease of administration and interpretation all recommend its use in assessing hearing handicap in the elderly.
Abstract: This report describes the development and standardization of the Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly (HHIE). This self-assessment tool is designed to assess the effects of hearing impairment on the emotional and social adjustment of elderly people. The inventory is comprised of two subscales: a 13-item subscale explores the emotional consequences of hearing impairment; a 12-item subscale explores both social and situational effects. The inventory was administered to 100 elderly subjects (mean age = 75 years) with hearing threshold levels in the better ear ranging from normal to severe. The reliability of the HHIE was evaluated by assessing its internal consistency through the computation of Chronbach's alpha. Alpha values ranged from 0.88 (social/situational subscale) to 0.95 for the entire inventory. Split-half reliabilities were equally high. The validity of the HHIE was not directly evaluated. Certain aspects of the data, however, support the construct validity of the instrument, while analysis of the questions themselves appears to attest to its content validity. Possible uses of the inventory were described and suggestions were made regarding future research on the instrument. The reliability and validity of the HHIE as well as its brevity, simplicity, and ease of administration and interpretation all recommend its use in assessing hearing handicap in the elderly.

998 citations

Performance
Metrics
No. of papers from the Journal in previous years
YearPapers
2023135
2022237
2021168
2020180
2019138
201851