scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Posted ContentDOI

A behavioral logic underlying aggression in an African cichlid fish

22 Jul 2020-bioRxiv (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory)-
TL;DR: This research found that the relative SL of intruders played an important role in determining behavioral performance, and suggested that A. burtoni males amplify non-physical aggression to settle territorial disputes in response to differences in relative SL that were not previously considered to be behaviorally relevant.
Abstract: Social rank in a hierarchy determines which individuals have access to important resources such as food, shelter, and mates. In the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni, rank is under social control, such that larger males are more likely than smaller males to be dominant in rank. Although it is well known that the relative size of A. burtoni males is critical in controlling social rank, the specific behavioral strategies underlying responses to males of different sizes are not well understood. In this research, our goal was to characterize these responses by performing resident-intruder assays, in which aggressive behaviors were measured in territorial males in response to the introduction of unfamiliar males that differed in relative standard length (SL). We found that the relative SL of intruders played an important role in determining behavioral performance. Resident males exposed to larger (>5% larger in SL) or matched (between 0 and 5% larger or smaller in SL) intruder males performed more lateral displays, a type of non-physical aggression, compared to resident males exposed to smaller (>5% smaller in SL) intruder males. However, physical aggression, such as chases and bites, did not differ as a function of relative SL. Our results suggest that A. burtoni males amplify non-physical aggression to settle territorial disputes in response to differences in relative SL that were not previously considered to be behaviorally relevant. Highlights Relative size determines social rank in the African cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni Resident male A. burtoni respond differently to small size differences in intruder males Residents perform more non-physical aggression against larger intruders Residents do not alter physical aggression as a function of differently sized intruders Distinct behavioral strategies are used against different intruders

Summary (3 min read)

Ethical Note

  • The protocols and procedures used here were approved by the Stanford University Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care (protocol number: APLAC_9882) and followed the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the use of animals in research.
  • The authors were able to monitor the behaviours of all fish throughout each day of the study (see below).
  • Throughout the whole assay, each tank was monitored in real time through a Wi-Fi-enabled camcorder remotely connected to a tablet (iPad).
  • Fish in all other tanks were monitored three times daily by visual inspection, to ensure they experienced no physical harm.
  • No fish were physically harmed at any point during the assay.

General approach

  • The authors took several steps to control for social experience and age of both the resident and intruder, since these factors have been found to influence behavior in A. burtoni (Alcazar et al., 2014; Solomon-Lane & Hofmann, 2019) .
  • To control for previous social experience, the resident and intruder were unrelated and had had no visual, physical, or chemical interaction at any point prior to the assay.
  • Another step the authors took to control for social experience was to socially suppress all males before they were given the social opportunity to ascend to dominance and were provided a territory.
  • The authors also physically isolated socially ascending fish from other fish, to further control for the role of social experience on behavioral responses to the intruder.
  • Finally, all resident-intruder pairs were age-matched, to control for effects of age on behavior (Alcazar et al., 2014) .

Social suppression

  • Two 121-liter social suppression tanks (see Fig. 2a for illustrated example) were each filled with 20 related, small suppressed males, as well as 3 large, unrelated dominant males and .
  • CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license available under a was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
  • The two tanks contained broods of the same age from different parents.
  • Smaller suppressed males were housed in these conditions for at least 45 days before being transferred to a dominance inducing tank (see below).
  • (d) After the assay, male residents are weighed for body and gonad mass and behavior videos are scored and analyzed.

Dominance inducing tank setup

  • Thirty-liter dominance inducing tanks (see Fig. 2b for illustrated example) were set up for the isolation of previously suppressed males to allow for controlled social ascent to dominance.
  • Each tank contained a shelter (half terra cotta pot) and faced a 121-liter tank filled with 10 unrelated females with which the male could interact visually but not physically or chemically.
  • CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license available under a was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
  • The copyright holder for this preprint (which this version posted July 22, 2020.
  • Males were transferred from the suppression tank and isolated in a dominance-inducing tank for 2-4 weeks before entering the assay tank.

Dissections

  • Immediately following the completion of the resident-intruder assay, the resident male was removed, weighed, and euthanized via rapid cervical transection (see Fig. 2d ).
  • An incision was made anterior to the vent to the caudal fin and the gonads were removed and weighed.

Scoring behavior

  • Based on previous work, multiple types of behavior were quantified ( Fernald & Hirata, 1977 ; see Fig. 3 for illustrated examples of behaviors): fleeing from male; physical aggression (chase male and bite male); non-physical aggression (lateral display and flexing); and pot entry, a territorial behavior.
  • Chase was defined as a rapid swim directed towards a fish.
  • Lateral displays were defined as aggressive displays classified as presentations of the side of the body to another fish with erect fins, flared opercula, and trembling of the body.
  • Pot entry was defined as as any time a male entered the half terra cotta pot.

Aggressive behaviors

  • Chase Bite Lateral display (a) (b) (c) Matched, and Larger.
  • The "Smaller" group contained residents that were exposed to intruders 5% or more larger in SL; the "Matched" group included residents that were within ±5% of the size of the intruder; and the "Larger" group included residents that were exposed to intruders 5% or more smaller in SL.

Statistical analysis

  • All statistical tests were performed in Prism 8.0.
  • The authors used Kruskal-Wallis ANOVAs followed by Dunn's post-hoc tests for comparisons of physiological and behavioral measures across groups.
  • When comparing only two groups, the authors used Mann-Whitney tests.
  • Raster plots were generated using custom software packages in R (available at https://github. com/FernaldLab).
  • Correlational analyses were conducted using Pearson's r. Effects were considered significant at p≤0.05.

Qualitative analysis of behavior as a function of relative SL

  • The authors first visualized behavioral output for all fish in raster plots (Fig. 4 ).
  • These plots showed that regardless of relative SL, residents attacked intruders at similar rates.
  • Finally, most intruders performed zero aggressive behaviors towards the resident (see Fig. 5 ; results not shown in raster plots because it occurred at such low rates and only in a few fish; see below), indicating that the resident fish all maintained dominance throughout the challenge (or "won").

Correlational analyses reveal different features of resident aggression scale with relative SL

  • The authors next ran correlational analyses to assess the relationship between relative SL and behavior.
  • When relating the proportion of each behavior performed to the relative SL of the intruder, the authors found a significant positive relationship between lateral display proportion and relative SL (r=0.62, N=17, P=0.008) (Fig. 6c ).

No effects of gonadosomatic index or other physiological factors on resident behavior

  • No significant correlations were found between resident GSI and resident behaviors (r³-0.37, N=17, P³0.13).
  • CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license available under a was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
  • The copyright holder for this preprint (which this version posted July 22, 2020.
  • This overall pattern of differences does not systematically relate to their pattern of behavioral findings (see Fig. 4-8 ), suggesting that their behavioral differences are specifically related to the effects of the intruder's relative SL.

Discussion

  • Specifically, when resident dominant males are exposed to an intruder who is matched or larger in relative SL, they use a behavioral strategy that emphasizes non-physical aggression.
  • Recent studies have shown that physical and non-physical aggression are uncorrelated in A. burtoni, suggesting that these aggressive behaviors function differently during social interactions.
  • ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.22.216473 doi: bioRxiv preprint Indeed, if it was the case that only size differences guided behavioral performance, then intruder males that were larger than the resident should have performed more lateral displays--but this was clearly not the case.
  • Furthermore, testosterone, which is higher in dominant males than non-dominant males (Parikh, Clement, & Fernald, 2006) , increases the winner effect (Oliveira, Silva, & Canário, 2009) .

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

1
Title: A behavioral logic underlying aggression in an African cichlid fish
1
2
Short title: Aggression in a cichlid
3
4
Authors: Beau A. Alward
1,2,3*
, Phillip H. Cathers
3
, Danielle M. Blakkan
3
, Russell D. Fernald
3
5
6
Affiliations:
1
University of Houston, Department of Psychology;
2
University of Houston,
7
Department of Biology and Biochemistry;
3
Stanford University, Department of Biology
8
9
*Corresponding author: balward@uh.edu
10
11
Competing interest: We have no competing interests.
12
13
Author’s contributions: Conceptualization, B.A.A., P.H.C., D.M.B., R.D.F.; Methodology,
14
B.A.A., P.H.C., D.M.B.; Investigation, B.A.A., P.H.C.; Writing Original Draft, B.A.A.; Writing
15
Review & Editing, B.A.A., P.H.C., D.M.B., R.D.F.; Supervision, B.A.A., R.D.F.
16
17
18
Abstract
19
20
Social rank in a hierarchy determines which individuals have access to important resources
21
such as food, shelter, and mates. In the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni, rank is under
22
social control, such that larger males are more likely than smaller males to be dominant in rank.
23
Although it is well known that the relative size of A. burtoni males is critical in controlling social
24
rank, the specific behavioral strategies underlying responses to males of different sizes are not
25
well understood. In this research, our goal was to characterize these responses by performing
26
resident-intruder assays, in which aggressive behaviors were measured in territorial males in
27
response to the introduction of unfamiliar males that differed in relative standard length (SL). We
28
found that the relative SL of intruders played an important role in determining behavioral
29
performance. Resident males exposed to larger (>5% larger in SL) or matched (between 0 and
30
5% larger or smaller in SL) intruder males performed more lateral displays, a type of non-
31
physical aggression, compared to resident males exposed to smaller (>5% smaller in SL)
32
intruder males. However, physical aggression, such as chases and bites, did not differ as a
33
function of relative SL. Our results suggest that A. burtoni males amplify non-physical
34
aggression to settle territorial disputes in response to differences in relative SL that were not
35
previously considered to be behaviorally relevant.
36
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 22, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.22.216473doi: bioRxiv preprint

2
Keywords: Aggression, territorial, resident-intruder, social behavior, cichlid
37
38
Highlights
39
40
Relative size determines social rank in the African cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni
41
Resident male A. burtoni respond differently to small size differences in intruder males
42
Residents perform more non-physical aggression against larger intruders
43
Residents do not alter physical aggression as a function of differently sized intruders
44
Distinct behavioral strategies are used against different intruders
45
46
Introduction
47
48
Intraspecific aggression is widespread among social animals (van Staaden, Searcy, &
49
Hanlon, 2011). Aggressive behavior, either through physical attacks or non-physical signaling, is
50
used to resolve conflicts related to access to resources such as food, shelter, territory, and
51
mates. Extraordinary diversity exists in how different species express aggression and the rules
52
that govern aggressive interactions. However, one rule seems to apply across species: physical
53
or injurious behaviors are considered to be escalatory, occurring primarily in response to
54
conflicts that are difficult to resolve (Holekamp & Strauss, 2016; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988;
55
van Staaden et al., 2011). The degree of conflict has been formally defined in terms of
56
differences in resource holding potential (RHP). RHP can take the form of different levels of
57
fighting ability as measured by body or weapon size. When large asymmetries in RHP exist
58
among animals, aggressive interactions do not escalate from non-physical to physical; however,
59
when asymmetries in RHP among animals are smaller, aggressive interactions are more likely
60
to escalate, involving more physical and injurious forms of aggression.
61
62
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 22, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.22.216473doi: bioRxiv preprint

3
Evolution has shaped social dynamics across species to resolve aggressive interactions
63
with as little physical fighting as possible, as this ensures individual and species survival
64
(Holekamp & Strauss, 2016; Maynard Smith & Harper, 1988; van Staaden et al., 2011). This is
65
abundantly clear in social animals that exist in a hierarchy, where rank determines which
66
individuals possess a territory and the behaviors they perform. This is the case for the African
67
cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni, where males stratify along a dominance hierarchy and exist as
68
either non-dominant or dominant (Fernald, 2012). Dominant males possess a territory which
69
they defend through aggressive interactions and in which they mate with females, while non-
70
dominant males do not perform these behaviors. Dominant males also possess larger testes
71
and brighter body coloration compared to non-dominant males. Social hierarchies in A. burtoni
72
remain in flux, however, as non-dominant males constantly survey the environment, searching
73
for a social opportunity to ascend in social rank to dominance. Social opportunity for a non-
74
dominant A. burtoni male typically occurs when a larger male is absent from the environment,
75
which a given smaller non-dominant male perceives as an opportunity to ascend to dominant
76
rank. Within minutes of the opportunity, the non-dominant male increases aggressive and
77
reproductive behavior in an attempt to establish a territory. Dominant males who encounter a
78
larger dominant male in their environment will begin to descend in social rank by reducing
79
aggressive and reproductive behavior (Maruska, Becker, Neboori, & Fernald, 2013).
80
81
Size-induced social control of social status in A. burtoni has been shown in several
82
studies. The reliable occurrence of this phenomenon makes size an excellent tool for
83
controlling social environments in the laboratory, with the goal of generating fish with a given
84
social status and studying the associated physiological underpinnings (for examples, see
85
Alward, Hilliard, York, & Fernald, 2019; Maruska, Becker, Neboori, & Fernald, 2013; Maruska &
86
Fernald, 2010). Although it has been shown repeatedly that size influences social status in male
87
A. burtoni, a precise understanding of the relationship between size and behavior has not been
88
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 22, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.22.216473doi: bioRxiv preprint

4
established. For instance, while size is something that modifies social decisions in male A.
89
burtoni, it is unclear what size difference males actually perceive as different and how they
90
modify their behavior accordingly. Previous work has defined male A. burtoni as “matched” in
91
size within a large range of standard length (e.g., 0-10% larger or smaller in standard length
92
(SL=measured from the most anterior portion of the mouth to the most anterior portion of the
93
caudal fin); see Alcazar, Hilliard, Becker, Bernaba, & Fernald, 2014; Desjardins & Fernald,
94
2010)). However, recent work suggests that very small size differences between male A. burtoni
95
can affect social interactions. For example, Alcazar et al ( 2014) found that males that were 2.1-
96
4.9% larger in SL than their competitor consistently won during a contest, suggesting that size
97
differences previously regarded as “matched” may actually be behaviorally relevant. However,
98
this study was focused on which fish won each contest and not on the specific behavioral
99
strategies underlying responses to differently sized males.
100
101
Characterizing the specific behavioral patterns in A. burtoni that occur in response to
102
differently sized males may yield insight into the capacity of A. burtoni to discern different levels
103
of social opportunities, which would allow for a deeper understanding of the cognitive abilities
104
required to successfully navigate a social hierarchy. In the present study we characterized
105
behavioral responses in male A. burtoni as a function of differently sized male competitors
106
during resident-intruder assays, in which a dominant male with a territory (i.e., the resident) was
107
exposed to an unfamiliar, non-dominant male intruder that differed in relative standard length
108
(SL). (illustrated in Fig. 1). The results of this study could shed light on the rules of engagement
109
during social interactions in male A. burtoni.
110
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 22, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.22.216473doi: bioRxiv preprint

5
111
Figure 1. The behavioral patterns in male Astatotliapia burtoni underlying responses to
112
differently sized males have not been characterized. Male A. burtoni change their social
113
status depending on the social environment. Large males socially suppress smaller males and
114
large males are more likely to be dominant. The specific behaviors males perform in response
115
to differences in relative size, however, have not been determined. We asked during a resident-
116
intruder assay what behavior patterns resident males use when presented with male intruders
117
that were smaller, larger, or matched in size.
118
119
Methods
120
121
Ethical Note
122
123
The protocols and procedures used here were approved by the Stanford University
124
Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care (protocol number: APLAC_9882) and followed
125
the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the use of animals in research. We were able to monitor the
126
behaviours of all fish throughout each day of the study (see below). Throughout the whole
127
assay, each tank was monitored in real time through a Wi-Fi-enabled camcorder remotely
128
connected to a tablet (iPad). Fish in all other tanks were monitored three times daily by visual
129
inspection, to ensure they experienced no physical harm. No fish were physically harmed at any
130
point during the assay.
131
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 22, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.22.216473doi: bioRxiv preprint

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In the past few years, advances in machine learning have fueled an explosive growth of descriptive and generative models of animal behavior as mentioned in this paper , offering higher levels of detail and granularity than has previously been possible, allowing for fine-grained segmentation of animals' actions and precise quantitative mappings between an animal's sensory environment and its behavior.

12 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article , the authors used a cichlid fish, Astatotilapia burtoni, to examine resident dominant male responses to a territorial intrusion within the social group and found that resident dominant males shifted from border defense to overt attack behavior, accompanied by decreased basolateral amygdala activity.

5 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper , the authors used the Burton's Mouthbrooder (Astatotilapia burtoni) to investigate juvenile social status and behavior, as well as the underlying neuroendocrine mechanisms.

4 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A status‐specific function for NO signaling in the visual and olfactory systems that may be important for sensory perception related to mating or territorial interactions to maintain the social hierarchy is suggested.
Abstract: Nitric oxide (NO) produced by the enzyme neuronal nitric oxide synthase serves as an important neurotransmitter in the central nervous system that is involved in reproductive regulation, learning, sensory processing, and other forms of neural plasticity. Here, we map the distribution of nnos‐expressing cells in the brain and retina of the cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni using in situ hybridization. In the brain, nnos‐expressing cells are found from the olfactory bulbs to the hindbrain, including within specific nuclei involved in decision‐making, sensory processing, neuroendocrine regulation, and the expression of social behaviors. In the retina, nnos‐expressing cells are found in the inner nuclear layer, presumably in amacrine cells. We also used quantitative PCR to test for differences in nnos expression within the eye and olfactory bulbs of males and females of different reproductive states and social statuses. In the eye, males express more nnos than females, and socially dominant males express more nnos than subordinate males, but expression did not differ among female reproductive states. In the olfactory bulbs, dominant males had greater nnos expression than subordinate males. These results suggest a status‐specific function for NO signaling in the visual and olfactory systems that may be important for sensory perception related to mating or territorial interactions to maintain the social hierarchy. The widespread distribution of nnos‐expressing cells throughout the cichlid brain is similar to that in other teleosts, with some conserved localization patterns across vertebrates, suggesting diverse functions for this important neurotransmitter system.

1 citations

Posted ContentDOI
10 May 2023-bioRxiv
TL;DR: In this paper , the authors manipulated early-life social experience in the highly social African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni to investigate the effects on behaviour and neuroendocrine stress axis function.
Abstract: Early-life social experiences shape adult phenotype, yet the underlying behavioural mechanisms remain poorly understood. We manipulated early-life social experience in the highly social African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni to investigate the effects on behaviour and neuroendocrine stress axis function. Juveniles experienced different numbers of early-life social partners in stable pairs (1 partner), stable groups (6 fish; 5 partners), and socialized pairs (a novel fish was exchanged every 5 days; 5 partners). Treatments differed in group size (groups vs. pairs) and stability (stable vs. socialized). We then measured behaviour in multiple contexts and collected water-borne cortisol. We found effects of treatment on behaviour across all assays: open field exploration, social cue investigation, dominant behaviour, and subordinate behaviour. Cortisol did not differ across treatments. Principal components (PC) analysis revealed robust co- variation of behaviour across contexts, including with cortisol, to form behavioural syndromes sensitive to early-life social experience. PC1 (25.1%) differed by numbers of social partners: juveniles with more social partners were more active during the social cue investigation, spent less time in the territory, and were more interactive as dominants. Differences in PC5 (8.5%) were based on stability: socialized pairs were more dominant, spent less time in and around the territory, were more socially investigative, and had lower cortisol than stable groups or pairs. Behaviour observations in the home tanks provided further insights into the behavioural mechanisms underlying these effects. These results contribute to our understanding of how early- life social experiences are accrued and exert strong, lasting effects on adult phenotype.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Decisions in pairwise contests over resources in a species where two individual contestants are likely to remain in the same social group, and regularly and repeatedly interact are explored to shed insight on rules of engagement within hierarchical social groups.

86 citations


Additional excerpts

  • ...Measuring the effects of size differences: group-level and continuous analyses Previous work in A. burtoni and other cichlids suggests that a size difference between 0 and 5% is considered “matched” in size (Alcazar et al., 2014; Reddon et al., 2011; Taborsky, 1984, 1985)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI

85 citations


"A behavioral logic underlying aggre..." refers background in this paper

  • ...For instance, in the convict cichlid Cichlasoma nigrofasciatum, Iateral displays are performed less when fish could interact visually before allowed to interact physically, compared to when they could not see each other before physical interaction (Keeley & Grant, 1993)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown for the first time in a non-mammalian vertebrate that dropping in social rank rapidly activates specific socially relevant brain nuclei in a pattern that differs from when males rise to a higher status position.
Abstract: In social species that form hierarchies where only dominant males reproduce, lower-ranking individuals may challenge higher-ranking ones, often resulting in changes in relative social status. How does a losing animal respond to loss of status? Here, using the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni, we manipulated the social environment, causing males to descend in rank, and then examined changes in behavior, circulating steroids and immediate early gene (IEG) expression (cfos, egr-1) in micro-dissected brain regions as a proxy for neuronal activation. In particular, we examined changes in the conserved 'social behavior network' (SBN), a collection of brain nuclei known to regulate social behaviors across vertebrates. Astatotilapia burtoni has rapidly reversible dominant-subordinate male phenotypes, so that within minutes, descending males lost their bright body coloration, switched to submissive behaviors and expressed higher plasma cortisol levels compared with non-descending and control males. Descending males had higher IEG expression throughout the SBN, but each brain region showed a distinct IEG-specific response in either cfos or egr-1 levels, but not both. Overall, SBN IEG patterns in descending males were distinctly different from the pattern observed in males ascending (subordinate to dominant) in social status. These results reveal that the SBN rapidly coordinates the perception of social cues about status that are of opposite valence, and translates them into appropriate phenotypic changes. This shows for the first time in a non-mammalian vertebrate that dropping in social rank rapidly activates specific socially relevant brain nuclei in a pattern that differs from when males rise to a higher status position.

80 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The mechanisms through which behavior changes the brain in the service of reproduction using a teleost fish model system are described.
Abstract: In the course of evolution, social behavior has been a strikingly potent selective force in shaping brains to control action. Physiological, cellular, and molecular processes reflect this evolutionary force, particularly in the regulation of reproductive behavior and its neural circuitry. Typically, experimental analysis is directed at how the brain controls behavior, but the brain is also changed by behavior over evolution, during development, and through its ongoing function. Understanding how the brain is influenced by behavior offers unusual experimental challenges. General principles governing the social regulation of the brain are most evident in the control of reproductive behavior. This is most likely because reproduction is arguably the most important event in an animal's life and has been a powerful and essential selective force over evolution. Here I describe the mechanisms through which behavior changes the brain in the service of reproduction using a teleost fish model system.

70 citations


"A behavioral logic underlying aggre..." refers background in this paper

  • ...In A. burtoni, size is a critical factor in determining social rank (Fernald & Maruska, 2012; Fernald, 2012)....

    [...]

  • ...This is the case for the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni, where males stratify along a dominance hierarchy and exist as either non-dominant or dominant (Fernald, 2012)....

    [...]

  • ...This is the case for the African 67 cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni, where males stratify along a dominance hierarchy and exist as 68 either non-dominant or dominant (Fernald, 2012)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Although prior residence is the primary determinant of the outcome of territorial encounters in this species, both relative body size and prior possession of territory also influence the nature of the interaction.
Abstract: Territorial interactions between pairs of size mis-matched, sexually mature male angelfish Pterophyllum scalare were investigated in three different conditions: with the larger fish resident (the large resident condition), with the smaller fish resident (the small resident condition) and in a neutral territory (the neutral condition). In the two resident conditions, approximately half of the intruders had previously held territories and half had not. In all categories of fight, one fish showed submissive postures and lost the fight; eventual losers performed both attack and threat at a lower rate than eventual winners. Attack rate declined as the encounter progressed, while rate of performance of threat postures increased. In fights on neutral territories, the larger fish won all fights. In all fights with a resident-intruder asymmetry, the resident fish won the encounter, regardless of relative size. In eventual winners but not in eventual losers, levels of attack were lowest in the neutral encounters. In the small resident condition, levels of attack (corrected for activity of the resident) were lower in intruders that had previously held a breeding territory. Relative size influenced behaviour shown during fights, in that overall intensity was correlated negatively with size differential in all conditions. Thus although prior residence is the primary determinant of the outcome of territorial encounters in this species, both relative body size and prior possession of territory also influence the nature of the interaction.

67 citations