scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis.

01 Feb 2001-Psychological Medicine (Cambridge University Press)-Vol. 31, Iss: 2, pp 189-195
TL;DR: The cognitive processes that are thought to lead to the formation and maintenance of the positive symptoms of psychosis are set out and a fuller integration with the findings of biological research will be required.
Abstract: In the last 10 years a consensus has developed that the symptoms of psychosis may be better understood by linking the steps between the phenomenological experiences and social, psychological and neurobiological levels of explanation. Cognitive models of psychosis are an important link in this chain. They provide a psychological description of the phenomena from which hypotheses concerning causal processes can be derived and tested; social, individual, and neurobiological factors can then be integrated via their impact on these cognitive processes. In this paper, we set out the cognitive processes that we think lead to the formation and maintenance of the positive symptoms of psychosis and we attempt to integrate into our model research in social factors. If this model proves useful, a fuller integration with the findings of biological research will be required (Frith, 1992).

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Psychological Medicine, 2001, 31, 189–195. Printed in the United Kingdom
" 2001 Cambridge University Press
THEORETICAL PAPER
A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis
P. A. GARETY,
"
E. KUIPERS, D. FOWLER, D. FREEMAN  P. E. BEBBINGTON
From the Academic Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Guys, Kings and St Thomass Medical
School, Kings College London, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry and Royal Free and
University College Medical School, London; and University of East Anglia, Norwich
INTRODUCTION
In the last 10 years a consensus has developed
that the symptoms of psychosis may be better
understood by linking the steps between the
phenomenological experiences and social,
psychological and neurobiological levels of
explanation. Cognitive models of psychosis are
an important link in this chain. They provide a
psychological description of the phenomena
from which hypotheses concerning causal pro-
cesses can be derived and tested ; social, in-
dividual, and neurobiological factors can then
be integrated via their impact on these cognitive
processes. In this paper, we set out the cognitive
processes that we think lead to the formation
and maintenance of the positive symptoms of
psychosis and we attempt to integrate into our
model research in social factors. If this model
proves useful, a fuller integration with the
findings of biological research will be required
(Frith, 1992).
Our cognitive model builds on the work of
other researchers (e.g. Maher, 1988; Frith, 1992;
Hemsley, 1993; Bentall et al. 1994; Chadwick &
Birchwood, 1994; Morrison et al. 1995) and our
own clinical and theoretical studies. The model
is new in that it incorporates both disruptions in
automatic cognitive processes and maladaptive
conscious appraisals; it covers delusions and
hallucinations in one framework; it posits a
central role for emotion; and it considers how
social factors may contribute to the origins,
"
Address for correspondence: Professor P. A. Garety, Academic
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Guy’s, King’s and St
Thomas’s Medical School, King’s College London, Adamson Centre
for Mental Health, St Thomas’s Hospital, Lambeth Palace Road,
London SE1 7EH.
maintenance or recurrence of symptoms. The
resulting cognitive model of psychosis is con-
sistent with existing theoretical and therapeutic
studies. It generates testable hypotheses and
should lead to theoretical and therapeutic
advances.
A cognitive model of psychosis
There is widespread agreement that psychosis
occurs in people with a vulnerable predisposition
(of biopsychosocial origin); that onset often
follows life events, adverse environments, illicit
drug use, or periods of isolation; that there are
emotional changes, and disruptions in cognitive
processes of attention, perception, or judgement ;
and that, at onset, its most prominent symptoms
are delusional beliefs and hallucinations (Garety
et al. 2000).
We postulate two proximal routes to the
development of the positive symptoms of psy-
chosis: one proceeds through cognitive and
affective changes ; the other proceeds through
affective disturbance alone. We think the first
route is the more common : a triggering event
gives rise in a predisposed person to a disruption
of cognitive processes (Garety & Hemsley, 1994).
The basic automatic cognitive disturbance may
be conceptualized in two ways. First, it may be
viewed as a weakening of the influences of
stored memories of regularities of previous input
on current perception ’, which leads to am-
biguous, unstructured sensory input and the
subsequent intrusion into consciousness of unin-
tended material from memory (Hemsley, 1993).
Secondly, the basic cognitive dysfunction may
be recently developed difficulties with the self-
189

190 P. A. Garety and others
monitoring of intentions and actions, which
would lead to individuals’ own intentions to act
not being recognized and therefore being ex-
perienced as alien (Frith, 1992). (There are
similarities between these accounts, as Hemsley
(1998) has noted. Both imply a breakdown in
willed intention activity, particularly in the
later stages of the disorder, albeit for different
reasons.) What is important for our model is
that at onset both accounts emphasize that the
basic cognitive disturbance leads to anomalous
conscious experiences (e.g. heightened percep-
tion, actions experienced as unintended, racing
thoughts, thoughts appearing to be broadcast,
thoughts experienced as voices, two unconnected
events appearing to be causally linked). Many
sufferers report of these cognitive and perceptual
changes of the psychotic prodrome that they are
experienced as unfamiliar and not like ordinary
self-generated cognitions they feel external and
potentially threatening. At this point, however,
these experiences have not been transformed
into psychotic symptoms. Emotional changes
also occur, in direct response to the triggering
event and in response to the anomalous
experiences. The generation of arousal is also
implicit in the model of cognitive disturbance
proposed by Hemsley (1993). Such emotional
changes feed back into the moment-by-moment
processing of anomalous experiences and in-
fluence their content. For example, if anxiety
and depression result from a job loss, and
further anxiety directly from the experience of
voices, the person’s voices may develop a
threatening and critical content: You’re useless,
you won’t get another job now. We’re after you,
we’ve got you marked’.
Furthermore, the anomalous experiences, be-
ing puzzling and associated with emotional
changes, seem personally significant and trigger
a search for explanation as to their cause (Maher,
1988). Here biased conscious appraisal processes
are crucial: they contribute to a judgement that
these confusing experiences (which feel external
in any case) are in fact externally caused. Garety
& Freeman (1999) have reviewed the evidence
for biases in cognitive processes and found
empirical support for an information gathering
cognitive style characterized by jumping to
conclusions, externalizing attributional biases,
and deficits in understanding social situations
and the intentions of others. It is likely that these
biased appraisal processes are made worse by
negative emotional states (e.g. anxiety,
depression, anger).
These immediate processes occur against a
conducive social–cognitive background. Inner
city birth and rearing appear to be major risk
factors for psychosis (Mortensen et al. 1999),
and there are also variations in incidence of
psychosis in ethnic groups. Bhugra and his
colleagues (1997) in particular have argued that
these factors are linked to social adversity and
deprivation. We suggest that earlier adverse
experience, such as social marginalization, child-
hood loss (Agid et al. 1999), or severe childhood
trauma, may create an enduring cognitive
vulnerability, characterized by negative sche-
matic models of the self and the world (e.g.
beliefs about the self as vulnerable to threat, or
about others as dangerous) that facilitate ex-
ternal attributions and low self-esteem.
This would be one plausible reason for the
finding of Myhrman et al. (1996) from the 1966
Finland birth cohort that unwanted pregnancies
resulted in children with twice the risk of
developing schizophrenia. Evidence consistent
with a role for negative schemas in the de-
velopment of psychosis has recently been
reported by Van Os (2000). In a large epidemio-
logical study in the Netherlands over 7000
people were screened for symptoms and psy-
chiatric status and followed up for 3 years.
Those who subsequently developed psychosis
were found more likely to have low self-esteem
and depressive schemas. Furthermore,
Birchwood et al. (2000), in developing a model
of auditory hallucinations, have suggested that a
childhood experience of social adversity leads to
the development of negative schemas involving
social humiliation and subordination, which in
turn fuel voices and paranoia.
These pre-existing negative schemas also
provide content to the psychotic attribution
(Bowins & Shugar, 1998 ; Fowler et al. 1998).
Thus, a person with religious beliefs about
innate wickedness concludes that the external
threat is caused by a punishing God. Some
triggering events may have attributes that render
externalizing appraisals particularly compelling;
we have pilot data linking life events categorized
as intrusive to first episodes of psychosis
characterized by persecutory themes (D. Raune,
personal communication). Finally, we suggest

Cognitive model of psychosis 191
that social isolation contributes to the accept-
ance of the psychotic appraisal by reducing
access to alternative more normalizing expla-
nations (White et al. 2000).
We argue that people with anomalous quasi-
psychotic experiences do not develop full-blown
psychotic symptoms if they are able to reject the
hypothesis of externality, leading to a protective
self-correcting decision, e.g. I thought I was
hearing the voice of God, but more likely my
mind is playing tricks; Things look different,
somehow, I must be stressed with all that’s
going on’. It is thus quite possible to have
hallucinatory experiences without becoming de-
luded, as has been reported (Peters et al. 1999).
The externalizing appraisal is thus a defining
decision. Psychosis is recognized as occurring
when the individual appraises experiences as
externally caused and personally significant.
Such appraisals are formally identified as
delusions and hallucinations : e.g. I am being
poisoned; God is giving me special powers ;
my voices are coming from persecutors who
want to kill me ; a transmitter is beaming my
thoughts worldwide’.
In a small proportion of cases (e.g. some cases
of delusional disorder), it appears there is a
second route to psychosis: the triggering event
does not appear to cause a basic information
processing disruption, leading to anomalous
experiences. Here, life events trigger only dis-
turbed affect, which in turn directly activates
biased appraisal processes and maladaptive
self\other schemas leading to an externalizing
appraisal (i.e. the delusion) for the life event or
the disturbed affect. In such cases, delusions
occur independently of hallucinations and other
psychotic symptoms.
Central to our model are the factors re-
sponsible for the maintenance\recurrence of the
psychotic appraisal. Why does it not correct
itself when the evidence for it is not forthcoming?
We hypothesize that a number of different
factors maintain the psychotic appraisal, as
follows.
1 Reasoning processes
The biased cognitive processes we think con-
tribute to symptom formation are also likely to
contribute to symptom maintenance, since these
biases have been found in people with persistent
symptoms, but may not be present after recovery
(Garety & Freeman, 1999). These are : a
jumping to conclusions data gathering bias, an
externalizing attributional style, and poor social
understanding or theory of mind. This abnormal
reasoning may in turn be maintained by social
isolation. We would also highlight two other
factors that have received less experimental
attention. First, we have obtained evidence that
a lack of belief flexibility (the willingness to
consider alternatives to delusional beliefs) is
associated with poorer outcome, independent of
the severity of the delusion (Garety et al. 1997).
Secondly, the normal belief confirmation bias is
likely to maintain psychotic beliefs (Maher,
1974). These two factors suggest that delusions
are more likely to be maintained in individuals
who have a dichotomous thinking style and who
cannot tolerate ambiguity.
2 Dysfunctional schemas and adverse social
environments
There is a growing literature on the poor self-
concept and self-esteem of people with psychosis
(e.g. Trower & Chadwick, 1995; Kinderman &
Bentall, 1996). Freeman et al. (1998) found that
self-esteem was poor in many people with
psychosis. Close & Garety (1998) found that
hallucinations and delusions that have negative
content are associated with negative self-
concepts. Psychotic beliefs may be more firmly
held if they are consistent with firmly-held
distorted beliefs about the self (e.g. that one is
bad), others (e.g. that others are hostile) and the
world (e.g. the world is dangerous). Moreover,
once formed, the delusion is likely to be
considered as further confirmation of the nega-
tive beliefs, leading to further strengthening of
the delusion.
Low self-esteem almost certainly develops in
specific social contexts (Brown et al. 1990;
Harris et al. 1990). Aversive social environments,
such as living with high expressed emotion
families, are a robust predictor of poor outcome
(Bebbington & Kuipers, 1994 ; Butzlaff &
Hooley, 1998). Mueser et al. (1998) report that
the lifetime prevalence of traumatic events
among a sample of 275 patients with schizo-
phrenia and bipolar disorder was 98%. We have
preliminary evidence, from a consecutive series
of 77 first episode in-patients with psychosis,
that severe trauma histories are more common
in those with symptoms unresponsive to medi-

192 P. A. Garety and others
cation than in those whose symptoms were
responsive (Fowler, 1999) while Doering and
colleagues (1998) have found that traumatic
experiences and adverse circumstances in child-
hood were related to relapse and re-
hospitalization in schizophrenia. Furthermore,
as discussed earlier, differences in the incidence
of psychosis in the inner city and in certain
ethnic groups has been linked to social adversity.
It seems likely that social marginalization,
difficult or traumatic experiences or un-
supportive family environments contribute to
the development of negative schemas. We
speculate from this literature that early trauma
and chronic stress can create dysfunctional
negative schemas, which in turn contribute to
treatment resistance and a vulnerability to
relapse.
3 Emotion (e.g. anxiety, depression, anger,
mania) and cognitive processes associated
with emotion
Clearly, dysfunctional negative schemas will be
closely associated with levels of emotional
distress. The presence of emotional distress will
also contribute to the maintenance of the
psychotic appraisal through other processes.
Birchwood and colleagues have demonstrated
the importance of depression in psychosis (e.g.
see Birchwood & Iqbal, 1998), They report that
residual symptoms of both hallucinations and
delusions are more common in depressed people
with psychosis and propose that feelings of
hopelessness and uncontrollability contribute to
symptom maintenance. Consistent with this,
hopelessness is a predictor of poor outcome in
early schizophrenia (Aguilar et al. 1997). We
ourselves have focused on anxiety. We argue
that three processes traditionally associated with
anxiety disorders may be of particular import-
ance. Information processing biases will provide
evidence or substantiation for psychotic beliefs
and hence maintain them (e.g. deployment of
attention; Freeman et al. 2000). Safety-
behaviours will prevent the receipt of discon-
firmatory evidence and hence prevent change in
psychotic beliefs (Freeman & Garety, 2000).
Meta-cognitive beliefs, such as beliefs concerning
the uncontrollability of one’s thoughts, will
increase the distress caused by psychotic
experiences (Freeman & Garety, 1999).
Clinically, it has also long been observed that
anxiety triggers hallucinations and increases in
delusional thoughts, i.e. affects cognitive pro-
cessing (Slade, 1972). Finally, the experience of
emotion will drive a search for meaning and
understanding that is consistent with affect-
associated beliefs: for example, anxiety will
increase the probability that a threatening
explanation is sought and accepted.
4 The secondary appraisal
The secondary appraisal of the experience of
psychosis itself (‘illness perception or insight).
We suggest appraisals of illness influence en-
gagement with treatment and adaptive behav-
iour. Standard assessments of insight are only
modestly correlated with outcome (David, 1998).
However, insight is correlated with the belief
maintenance subscale of the Maudsley As-
sessment of Delusions, and improved outcome
for delusions in response to CBT is associated
with and predicted by changes in the MADS
subscale (Garety et al. 1997). Also important are
appraisals of the experience of chronic mental
illness as stigmatizing and humiliating,
appraisals which may influence the development
of depression (Birchwood & Iqbal, 1998). These
appraisals probably have some basis in reality,
given evidence that significant others readily
apply negative labels to people developing first
episodes of schizophrenia (Bean et al. 1996).
The cognitive model and psychological
treatments
We think theory and therapy in this area are
mutually enhancing. Therapy provides a context
for theoretical developments and the empirical
investigation of our model, which in turn should
clarify the targets of psychological treatment,
refine its techniques, and enhance its efficacy.
Two sorts of psychological intervention seem
to be effective in diminishing positive symptoms
and their re-emergence. These are cognitive
behaviour therapy (CBT) and interventions with
the families of people with psychosis (FI)
(Haddock et al. 1998 ; Birchwood & Spencer,
1999; Kuipers et al. 1999). It is unlikely that
these work in the same way.
We hypothesize that CBT acts directly on
psychological processes (this is after all its aim).
In particular, it is most effective in treating
psychosis when the key appraisal, of inner

Cognitive model of psychosis 193
mental disturbance as externally caused, is re-
appraised as inner. In traditional terminology,
good insight is developed. This new schema is
incompatible with its psychotic predecessor.
Both symptoms and the risk of relapse are
thereby reduced. Therapy achieves this by
changing the appraisals and, where possible
negative self-schemata, and by compensating for
the biased reasoning processes (Fowler et al.
1995). We would also propose that clinical
improvements and reduced risks of relapse can
still occur when the externalizing psychotic
appraisal remains unaltered (i.e. insight remains
poor’), provided some of the hypothesized
maintenance factors are changed (e.g. safety
behaviours), disrupting the vicious cycle.
Family intervention (FI), in contrast, pri-
marily targets the behaviour of family members.
It improves social function (Barrowclough &
Tarrier, 1990) and reduces exacerbations of
positive symptoms (Pharoah et al. 1999; Kuipers
et al. 1999). We offer competing hypotheses
about how it affects the cognitive processing of
sufferers.
One possibility is that it operates by reducing
environmental stress and improving the
associated affect (anxiety or depression). Anom-
alous experiences would be improved mainly by
way of these affective changes. We know that
conflictual relationships predict poor outcome
in social and psychiatric patholology, and that
affirmative ones can improve outcome even in
those with persistent emotional damage (Lewis,
1998). People with schizophrenia are well able to
perceive criticism in their relatives (Tompson et
al. 1995; Scazufca et al. 2000). Such relationships
must also affect mood : while critical or intrusive
behaviour might increase anxiety and de-
pression, supportive relationships would reduce
them.
It is of interest that supportive counselling
and befriending had some effect in reducing
symptoms in two recent trials of CBT in
schizophrenia (Tarrier et al. 1998; Sensky et al.
2000). However, the benefits were limited to the
duration of the intervention. There is now
evidence from a meta-analysis of 19 randomized
controlled trials (reported in Kuipers et al. 1999)
that FI also needs to be continued for positive
effects to be maintained. This analysis found
that for single family therapy, the number needed
to treat (NNT) to prevent relapse in the first
year of treatment was 6n3; in the second year of
treatment this fell to 3n9. Once treatment ended
the NNT went back up to 7n1 to prevent relapse,
and 20n8 to prevent readmission. Thus, although
individual studies have found that treatment
effects can continue for as long as 8 years after
FI finishes (e.g. Tarrier et al. 1994) this was not
confirmed by the meta-analysis. This suggests
that both FI and supportive therapies have
effects which continue only for as long as therapy
is offered. In contrast, evidence is emerging that
changes from CBT may continue (and may even
be enhanced) when therapy finishes (Kuipers et
al. 1998; Tarrier et al. 1999; Sensky et al. 2000).
In families (and possibly also the supportive
therapies), it seems likely that therapeutic im-
provement occurs via the reduction of negative
affect through the provision of a less stressful
environment, and the improvement of social
functioning by appropriate prompting and re-
inforcement of small changes in behaviour.
There might also be improvements in thinking
processes (e.g. relative to negative schemas or
cognitive flexibility), but these would be sec-
ondary to the other changes. Thus, under this
hypothesis, the cognitive changes brought about
by FI would be less specific than with CBT and
the maintenance of effects would be less secure.
The contrasting hypothesis is that FI helps
family members to discuss directly with sufferers
their psychotic experiences and to provide
alternative explanations. FI would then operate
through cognitive change like CBT. No evidence
currently exists to test between these hypotheses
in terms of change in individual affect, schemas
or appraisals. The second hypothesis requires
that carers effectively perform as therapists, but
it seems unlikely that most would have the
specific skills to do this without training.
Conclusion
We have put forward a new cognitive model of
the positive symptoms of psychosis. While our
model is speculative, we think it has plausibility
in the context of what we currently know about
the various processes involved in delusional
thinking and anomalous experience. Moreover,
it integrates much of this information in a way
that will contribute to the further expansion of
knowledge, and may have important practical
benefits for people suffering from psychosis.

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The research addressing the relationship of childhood trauma to psychosis and schizophrenia is reviewed, and the theoretical and clinical implications are discussed.
Abstract: Objective: To review the research addressing the relationship of childhood trauma to psychosis and schizophrenia, and to discuss the theoretical and clinical implications. Method: Relevant studies and previous review papers were identified via computer literature searches. Results: Symptoms considered indicative of psychosis and schizophrenia, particularly hallucinations, are at least as strongly related to childhood abuse and neglect as many other mental health problems. Recent large-scale general population studies indicate the relationship is a causal one, with a dose-effect. Conclusion: Several psychological and biological mechanisms by which childhood trauma increases risk for psychosis merit attention. Integration of these different levels of analysis may stimulate a more genuinely integrated bio-psycho-social model of psychosis than currently prevails. Clinical implications include the need for staff training in asking about abuse and the need to offer appropriate psychosocial treatments to patients who have been abused or neglected as children. Prevention issues are also identified.

1,422 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Findings of previous studies implicating migration as a risk factor for the development of schizophrenia and a quantitative index of the associated effect size are synthesized to suggest a role for psychosocial adversity in the etiology of schizophrenia.
Abstract: OBJECTIVE: The authors synthesize findings of previous studies implicating migration as a risk factor for the development of schizophrenia and provide a quantitative index of the associated effect size. METHOD: MEDLINE was searched for population-based incidence studies concerning migrants in English-language publications appearing between the years 1977 and 2003. Article bibliographies and an Australian database were cross-referenced. Studies were included if incidence reports provided numerators and denominators and if age correction was performed or could be performed by the authors. Relative risks for migrant groups were extracted or calculated for each study. Significant heterogeneity across studies indicated the need for a mixed-effects meta-analytic model. RESULTS: The mean weighted relative risk for developing schizophrenia among first-generation migrants (40 effect sizes) was 2.7 (95% confidence interval [CI]=2.3–3.2). A separate analysis performed for second-generation migrants (seven effect siz...

1,136 citations


Cites background from "A cognitive model of the positive s..."

  • ...Experiences of discrimination may foster a paranoid attributional style that facilitates the development of psychotic symptoms (69)....

    [...]

Book
10 Jun 2004
TL;DR: In this article, the authors proposed an insightful and original approach to understand these disorders, one that focuses on what they have in common, instead of examining in isolation, for example, obsessive compulsive disorders, insomnia, schizophrenia.
Abstract: Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) has established itself as one of the most effective therapies for treating a wide range of psychological disorders. However, research and treatment in this field typically adopts a DSM driven 'disorder-focused' approach - researchers and clinicians target a specific disorder, try to understand its aetiology and maintenance, and try to develop more effective strategies to treat the disorder. This book proposes an insightful and original approach to understanding these disorders, one that focuses on what they have in common. Instead of examining in isolation, for example, obsessive compulsive disorders, insomnia, schizophrenia, it asks - what do patients with these disorders have in common? It takes each cognitive and behavioural process - attention, memory, reasoning, thought, behaviour, and examines whether it is a transdiagnostic process - i.e., serves to maintain a broad range of psychological disorders. Having shown how these disorders share several important processes, it then describes the practical implications of such an approach to diagnosis and treatment. Importantly it explores why the different psychological disorders can present so differently, despite being maintained by the same cognitive and behavioural processes. It also provides an account of the high rates of comorbidity observed among the different disorders. This book provides a novel review and integration of the empirical literature and gives clinicians and researchers a valuable new theoretical base for assessing and treating psychological disorders. Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) has established itself as one of the most effective therapies for treating a wide range of psychological disorders. However, research and treatment in this field typically adopts a DSM driven 'disorder-focused' approach - researchers and clinicians target a specific disorder, try to understand its aetiology and maintenance, and try to develop more effective strategies to treat the disorder. This book proposes an insightful and original approach to understanding these disorders, one that focuses on what they have in common. Instead of examining in isolation, for example, obsessive compulsive disorders, insomnia, schizophrenia, it asks - what do patients with these disorders have in common? It takes each cognitive and behavioural process - attention, memory, reasoning, thought, behaviour, and examines whether it is a transdiagnostic process - i.e., serves to maintain a broad range of psychological disorders. Having shown how these disorders share several important processes, it then describes the practical implications of such an approach to diagnosis and treatment. Importantly it explores why the different psychological disorders can present so differently, despite being maintained by the same cognitive and behavioural processes. It also provides an account of the high rates of comorbidity observed among the different disorders. This book provides a novel review and integration of the empirical literature and gives clinicians and researchers a valuable new theoretical base for assessing and treating psychological disorders.

1,046 citations


Cites background from "A cognitive model of the positive s..."

  • ...There is a growing theoretical view that hallucinations are the result of private mental events being misattributed to an external source, possibly as a result of cognitive deficits (Frith 1992; Hemsley 1993) and/or cognitive biases (Bentall 1990; Morrison et al. 1995; Garety et al. 2001)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Evidence for the continuity of psychotic symptoms with normal experiences is reviewed, focusing on the symptoms of hallucinations and delusions, and the theoretical and treatment implications of such a continuum are discussed.

922 citations


Cites background from "A cognitive model of the positive s..."

  • ...…is a growing body of research on the role of emotions, perceptions, and inferential processes in relation to the onset and persistence of psychotic symptoms (Garety & Hemsley, 1994; Garety et al., 2001), and the coping mechanisms that people use to deal with them (Bak et al., 2001a; Carr, 1988)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A multifactorial model of the formation and maintenance of persecutory delusions is presented, which includes the (non-defended) direct roles given to emotion in delusion formation, the detailed consideration of both the content and form of delusions, and the hypotheses concerning the associated emotional distress.
Abstract: A multifactorial model of the formation and maintenance of persecutory delusions is presented. Persecutory delusions are conceptualized as threat beliefs. The beliefs are hypothesized to arise from a search for meaning for internal or external experiences that are unusual, anomalous, or emotionally significant for the individual. The persecutory explanations formed reflect an interaction between psychotic processes, pre-existing beliefs and personality (particularly emotion), and the environment. It is proposed that the delusions are maintained by processes that lead to the receipt of confirmatory evidence and processes that prevent the processing of disconfirmatory evidence. Novel features of the model include the (non-defended) direct roles given to emotion in delusion formation, the detailed consideration of both the content and form of delusions, and the hypotheses concerning the associated emotional distress. The clinical and research implications of the model are outlined.

776 citations


Cites background from "A cognitive model of the positive s..."

  • ...The differences from the more general framework of Garety et al. (2001) are those of emphasis....

    [...]

References
More filters
Book
01 Sep 1992
TL;DR: The Nature of Sch schizophrenia, Behavioural Abnormalities in Schizophrenia, and Communication in Sch schizophrenia: Linking the Mind and the Brain are studied.
Abstract: Schizophrenic patients have bizarre experiences which reflect a disorder in the contents of consciousness. For example, patients hear voices talking about them or they are convinced that alien forces are controlling their actions. Their abnormal behaviour includes incoherence and lack of will. In this book an explanation of these baffling signs and symptoms is provided using the framework of cognitive neuropsychology.The cognitive abnormalities that underlie these signs and symptoms suggest impairment in a system which constructs and monitors representations of certain abstract (especially mental) events in consciousness. For example, schizophrenic patients can no longer construct representations of their intentions to act. Thus, if actions occur, these will be experienced as coming out of the blue and hence can seem alien. The patient who lacks awareness of his own intentions will stop acting spontaneously and hence will show a lack of will.The psychological processes that are abnormal in schizophrenia can be related to underlying brain systems using evidence from human and animal neuropsychology. Interactions between prefrontal cortex and other parts of the brain, especially temporal cortex appear critical for constructing the contents of consciousness. It is these interactions that are likely to be impaired in schizophrenia.

1,931 citations


"A cognitive model of the positive s..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Secondly, the basic cognitive dysfunction may be recently developed difficulties with the self- 189 monitoring of intentions and actions, which would lead to individuals’ own intentions to act not being recognized and therefore being experienced as alien (Frith, 1992)....

    [...]

  • ...Our cognitive model builds on the work of other researchers (e.g. Maher, 1988; Frith, 1992; Hemsley, 1993; Bentall et al. 1994; Chadwick & Birchwood, 1994; Morrison et al. 1995) and our own clinical and theoretical studies....

    [...]

  • ...monitoring of intentions and actions, which would lead to individuals’ own intentions to act not being recognized and therefore being experienced as alien (Frith, 1992)....

    [...]

  • ...If this model proves useful, a fuller integration with the findings of biological research will be required (Frith, 1992)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A meta-analysis of all available EE and outcome studies in schizophrenia confirmed that EE is a significant and robust predictor of relapse in schizophrenia and demonstrated that the EE-relapse relationship was strongest for patients with more chronic schizophrenic illness.
Abstract: Background Expressed emotion (EE) is a measure of the family environment that has been demonstrated to be a reliable psychosocial predictor of relapse in schizophrenia. However, in recent years some prominent nonreplications of the EE-relapse relationship have been published. To more fully address the question of the predictive validity of EE, we conducted a meta-analysis of all available EE and outcome studies in schizophrenia. We also examined the predictive validity of the EE construct for mood disorders and eating disorders. Methods An extensive literature search revealed 27 studies of the EE-outcome relationship in schizophrenia. Using meta-analytic procedures, we combined the findings of these investigations to provide an estimate of the effect size associated with the EE-relapse relationship. We also used meta-analysis to provide estimates of the effect sizes associated with EE for mood and eating disorders. Results The results confirmed that EE is a significant and robust predictor of relapse in schizophrenia. Additional analyses demonstrated that the EE-relapse relationship was strongest for patients with more chronic schizophrenic illness. Interestingly, although the EE construct is most closely associated with research in schizophrenia, the mean effect sizes for EE for both mood disorders and eating disorders were significantly higher than the mean effect size for schizophrenia. Conclusion These findings highlight the importance of EE in the understanding and prevention of relapse in a broad range of psychopathological conditions.

1,229 citations


"A cognitive model of the positive s..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Aversive social environments, such as living with high expressed emotion families, are a robust predictor of poor outcome (Bebbington & Kuipers, 1994; Butzlaff & Hooley, 1998)....

    [...]

  • ...We are grateful to Professor David Hemsley for his helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A multi-factorial model of delusion formation and maintenance incorporating a data-gathering bias and attributional style, together with other factors (e.g. perceptual processing, meta-representation) is consistent with the current evidence.
Abstract: Purpose. To review critically the evidence for three contemporary theories of delusions. Methods. The theoretical approaches to delusions proposed by Frith and colleagues (‘theory of mind’ deficits), Garety and colleagues (multi-factorial, but involving probabilistic reasoning biases) and Bentall and colleagues (attributional style and self-discrepancies) are summarised. The findings of empirical papers directly relevant to these proposals are critically reviewed. These papers were identified by computerised literature searches (for the years 1987-1997) and a hand search. Results. The evidence does not unequivocally support any of the approaches as proposed. However, strong evidence is found to support modifications of Garety and colleagues and Bentall and colleagues theories. Studies have replicated a ‘jumping to conclusions’ data-gathering bias and an externalising attributional bias in people with delusions. There is preliminary evidence for a ‘theory of mind’ deficit, as proposed by Frith, although possibly related to a more general reasoning bias. Evidence for an underlying discrepancy between ideal and actual self-representations is weaker. Conclusions. A multi-factorial model of delusion formation and maintenance incorporating a data-gathering bias and attributional style, together with other factors (e.g. perceptual processing, meta-representation) is consistent with the current evidence. It is recommended that these findings be incorporated into cognitive therapy approaches. However, there are limitations to existing research. Future studies should incorporate longitudinal designs and first episode studies, and should not neglect the co-morbidity of delusions, including affective processes, or the multi-dimensional nature of delusions.

793 citations


"A cognitive model of the positive s..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Garety & Freeman (1999) have reviewed the evidence for biases in cognitive processes and found empirical support for an information gathering cognitive style characterized by jumping to conclusions, externalizing attributional biases, and deficits in understanding social situations and the intentions of others....

    [...]

  • ...Garety & Freeman (1999) have reviewed the evidence for biases in cognitive processes and found empirical support for an information gathering cognitive style characterized by jumping to conclusions, externalizing attributional biases, and deficits in understanding social situations and the…...

    [...]

  • ...The biased cognitive processes we think contribute to symptom formation are also likely to contribute to symptom maintenance, since these biases have been found in people with persistent symptoms, but may not be present after recovery ( Garety & Freeman, 1999 )....

    [...]

  • ...1 Reasoning processes The biased cognitive processes we think contribute to symptom formation are also likely to contribute to symptom maintenance, since these biases have been found in people with persistent symptoms, but may not be present after recovery (Garety & Freeman, 1999)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown that highly disparate relationships with voices-fear, reassurance, engagement and resistance-reflect vital differences in beliefs about the voices, and how these core beliefs about voices may become a new target for treatment.
Abstract: We offer provisional support for a new cognitive approach to understanding and treating drug-resistant auditory hallucinations in people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Study 1 emphasises the relevance of the cognitive model by detailing the behavioural, cognitive and affective responses to persistent voices in 26 patients, demonstrating that highly disparate relationships with voices-fear, reassurance, engagement and resistance-reflect vital differences in beliefs about the voices. All patients viewed their voices as omnipotent and omniscient. However, beliefs about the voice's identity and meaning led to voices being construed as either 'benevolent' or 'malevolent'. Patients provided cogent reasons (evidence) for these beliefs which were not always linked to voice content; indeed in 31% of cases beliefs were incongruous with content, as would be anticipated by a cognitive model. Without fail, voices believed to be malevolent provoked fear and were resisted and those perceived as benevolent were courted. However, in the case of imperative voices, the primary influence on whether commands were obeyed was the severity of the command. Study 2 illustrates how these core beliefs about voices may become a new target for treatment. We describe the application of an adapted version of cognitive therapy (CT) to the treatment of four patients' drug-resistant voices. Where patients were on medication, this was held constant while beliefs about the voices' omnipotence, identity, and purpose were systematically disputed and tested. Large and stable reductions in conviction in these beliefs were reported, and these were associated with reduced distress, increased adaptive behaviour, and unexpectedly, a fall in voice activity. These changes were corroborated by the responsible psychiatrists. Collectively, the cases attest to the promise of CT as a treatment for auditory hallucinations.

777 citations


Additional excerpts

  • ...Our cognitive model builds on the work of other researchers (e.g. Maher, 1988; Frith, 1992; Hemsley, 1993; Bentall et al. 1994; Chadwick & Birchwood, 1994; Morrison et al. 1995) and our own clinical and theoretical studies....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A population-based cohort of 1.75 million persons whose mothers were Danish women born between 1935 and 1978 identified 2669 cases of schizophrenia among cohort members and additional cases among their parents, indicating that the place and season of birth account for many more cases on a population basis.
Abstract: Background Although a family history of schizophrenia is the best-established risk factor for schizophrenia, environmental factors such as the place and season of birth may also be important. Methods Using data from the Civil Registration System in Denmark, we established a population-based cohort of 1.75 million persons whose mothers were Danish women born between 1935 and 1978. We linked this cohort to the Danish Psychiatric Central Register and identified 2669 cases of schizophrenia among cohort members and additional cases among their parents. Results The respective relative risks of schizophrenia for persons with a mother, father, or sibling who had schizophrenia were 9.31 (95 percent confidence interval, 7.24 to 11.96), 7.20 (95 percent confidence interval, 5.10 to 10.16), and 6.99 (95 percent confidence interval, 5.38 to 9.09), as compared with persons with no affected parents or siblings. The risk of schizophrenia was associated with the degree of urbanization of the place of birth (relative risk ...

759 citations


"A cognitive model of the positive s..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Inner city birth and rearing appear to be major risk factors for psychosis (Mortensen et al. 1999), and there are also variations in incidence of psychosis in ethnic groups....

    [...]