scispace - formally typeset
Open AccessJournal ArticleDOI

A multitrait-multimethod validation of the Implicit Association Test: implicit and explicit attitudes are related but distinct constructs.

Brian A. Nosek, +1 more
- 01 Jan 2007 - 
- Vol. 54, Iss: 1, pp 14-29
Reads0
Chats0
TLDR
The authors applied Campbell and Fiske's (1959) classic multitrait-multimethod design precepts to test the construct validity of implicit attitudes as measured by the Implicit Association Test (IAT).
Abstract
Recent theoretical and methodological innovations suggest a distinction between implicit and explicit evaluations. We applied Campbell and Fiske's (1959) classic multitrait-multimethod design precepts to test the construct validity of implicit attitudes as measured by the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Participants (N = 287) were measured on both self-report and IAT for up to seven attitude domains. Through a sequence of latent-variable structural models, systematic method variance was distinguished from attitude variance, and a correlated two-factors-per-attitude model (implicit and explicit factors) was superior to a single-factor-per-attitude specification. That is, despite sometimes strong relations between implicit and explicit attitude factors, collapsing their indicators into a single attitude factor resulted in relatively inferior model fit. We conclude that these implicit and explicit measures assess related but distinct attitude constructs. This provides a basis for, but does not distinguish between, dual-process and dual-representation theories that account for the distinctions between constructs.

read more

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

B.A. Nosek & F.L. Smyth: A Multitrait-MultimethodValidation of the Implicit Association TestExperimentalP sychology 2007; Vol. 54 (1):14–29© 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
A Multitrait-Multimethod Validation
of the Implicit Association Test
Implicit and Explicit Attitudes Are Related
butDistinctConstructs
Brian A. Nosek and Frederick L. Smyth
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA
Abstract. Recent theoretical and methodological innovations suggest a distinction between implicit and explicit evaluations. We applied
Campbell and Fiske’s (1959) classic multitrait-multimethod design precepts to test the construct validity of implicit attitudes as measured
by the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Participants (N = 287) were measured on both self-report and IAT for up to seven attitude domains.
Through a sequence of latent-variable structural models, systematic method variance was distinguished from attitude variance, and a
correlated two-factors-per-attitude model (implicit and explicit factors) was superior to a single-factor-per-attitude specification. That is,
despite sometimes strong relations between implicit and explicit attitude factors, collapsing their indicators into a single attitude factor
resulted in relatively inferior model fit. We conclude that these implicit and explicit measures assess related but distinct attitude constructs.
This provides a basis for, but does not distinguish between, dual-process and dual-representation theories that account for the distinctions
between constructs.
Keywords: implicit social cognition, attitudes, individual differences, construct validity, structural equation modeling
Realizing that the human mind is more than the sum of its
conscious processes, a number of theorists have proposed
a conceptual distinction between evaluations that are the
products of introspection, called explicit attitudes, and
those that occur automatically and may exist outside of
conscious awareness, called implicit attitudes (Greenwald
& Banaji, 1995; Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000).
Greenwald and Banaji (1995, p. 8), for example, defined
implicit attitudes as “introspectively unidentified (or inac-
curately identified) traces of past experience that mediate
favorable or unfavorable feelings toward an attitude ob-
ject.” This theory has developed in conjunction with the
invention of measurement tools that assess automatic eval-
uative associations without introspection (e.g., Fazio, San-
bonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986; Greenwald, McGhee,
& Schwartz, 1998; Nosek & Banaji, 2001; Wittenbrink,
Judd, & Park, 1997).
Some experiences with these new measurement tools
have spawned doubts about whether they measure attitudes
at all (Karpinski & Hilton, 2001), and whether a conceptual
distinction between implicit and explicit attitudes is worth-
while (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Fazio and Olson contend “it
is more appropriate to view the measures as implicit or
explicit, not the attitude (or whatever other construct)”
(2003, p. 303). The purpose of the research we report was
to test whether the structure of attitude variance derived
from an implicit measure (the Implicit Association Test
[IAT]; Greenwald et al., 1998) and from an explicit one
(semantic differentials) is best represented by one latent
factor or by two correlated latent factors, when stripped of
confounding method variance. If our hypothesis that the
latter specification will fit the data better is sustained, it will
support a view that substantively different attitude con-
structs, distinguishable from the techniques used to mea-
sure them, underlie data collected by explicit and implicit
methods.
The finding would not, however, lend weight to one side
or the other in the debate about origins of the distinction
between implicit and explicit attitudes i.e., are they de-
rived from a single representation at different stages of pro-
cessing (Fazio & Olson, 2003) or do they reflect distinct
evaluative sources (Strack & Deutsch, 2004; Wilson et al.,
2000). Also, this multitrait-multimethod (MTMM) ap-
proach does not identify the cognitive processes that dis-
tinguish the constructs.
The context of this analysis follows from Cronbach and
Meehl’s (1955) classic discussion of construct validation
where a construct is an indeterminant function of represen-
tation and process. So, our reference to distinct implicit and
explicit attitude constructs should be interpreted as refer-
ring to distinguishable attitudinal components, without im-
plying a specific commitment to distinguishable formative
processes, single versus dual mental representations, or sin-
gle versus dual operative processes.
Any of these theoretical perspectives can explain dual
constructs by postulating combinations of representations
DOI 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14
Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):14–29 © 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers

and processes to account for the observed differences be-
tween constructs. Dual-construct validation justifies the
need to have theoretical models account for the distinction
without providing evidence for or against any particular
explanation. Further illustration of the difference between
construct validation versus commitments to dual-process
or dual-representation theories appears in the discussion
(see also Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, in press).
Construct Validation
The conceptual and empirical justification for a psycholog-
ical construct requires development of a nomological net
of facts, relationships, and validity evidence that clarifies
the identity and utility of the construct (Cronbach & Meehl,
1955; McArdle & Prescott, 1992). The nomological net
supporting the validity of implicit attitudes has been gain-
ing strength (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Nosek, Green-
wald, & Banaji, in press; Wilson et al., 2000). For example,
Poehlman, Uhlmann, Greenwald, and Banaji (2004) con-
ducted a meta-analysis of studies examining the predictive
validity of the IAT, a measure thought to be influenced by
automatic associations, and found that the IAT had robust
predictive validity across domains, and outperformed self-
report measures in some domains (stereotyping and preju-
dice), while self-report outperformed the IAT in other do-
mains (e.g., political preferences). Also, recent social-neu-
roscience research finds evidence for a neurological
distinction between automatic and controlled evaluative
processes (Cunningham, Johnson, Gatenby, Gore, & Bana-
ji, 2003; Cunningham, Johnson et al., 2004). Our research
provides another avenue of evidence for this growing no-
mological net by examining the relationship between im-
plicit and explicit attitude measures to determine whether
they can be fairly interpreted as measuring a single con-
struct, or whether they assess related, but distinct con-
structs.
Preliminary Evidence
Greenwald and Farnham (2000) observed that a model de-
scribing implicit and explicit self-esteem as distinct latent
factors provided a better fit than a single self-esteem con-
ceptualization. Likewise, Cunningham, Preacher, and Ba-
naji (2001) found implicit and explicit measures of racial
attitudes to reveal related, but distinct factors, as did Cun-
ningham, Netlek, and Banaji (2004) for implicit and explic-
it ethnocentrism. Following this approach of comparing
single versus dual factors in structural equation modeling,
we reanalyzed a large dataset reported by Nosek (2005).
We found support for the generalizability of a model of
distinct-but-related latent implicit and explicit attitude con-
structs across 56 of 57
1
widely varying attitude domains
showing that this observation is quite general (see Table 1
and the supplement to this paper available at http://brian-
nosek.com/ for a full report). Even so, in this and the other
previous structural modeling studies, specification of im-
plicit and explicit attitude constructs is confounded with
measurement method. As a consequence, a two-factor so-
lution is an indeterminant function of both attitude and
method variance.
2
To transcend this inferential limitation, here, guided by
principles articulated by Campbell and Fiske (1959), we use
a MTMM design and comparative structural modeling anal-
yses. This approach allowed us to distinguish attitude and
method variance from IAT and thermometer ratings, the op-
erationalizations of implicit and explicit attitudes, respective-
ly. Our findings demonstrate (1) convergent and discriminant
validity of the IAT, (2) that a model of distinct, but related
implicit and explicit attitudes best fits the data, and (3) that
this characterization is not attributable to attitude-irrelevant
method variance of the IAT or of self-report.
MTMM and Confirmatory Factor
Analysis
In their classic article on construct validation, Campbell
and Fiske (1959) articulated a strategy for using MTMM
matrices to evaluate convergent and discriminant validity.
This strategy requires measurement of two or more osten-
sibly distinct trait constructs by two or more measurement
methods. Convergent validity is demonstrated when indi-
cators of a given trait (or, in our study, attitude) correlate
highly across measurement method, while discriminant va-
lidity obtains when correlations between ostensibly differ-
ent traits are low. Campbell and Fiske argued that “the
clear-cut demonstration of the presence of method variance
requires both several traits and several methods” (p. 85).
They described ways to statistically evaluate the respective
contributions of trait and method factors, but looked for-
ward to continued progress in developing more rigorous
validation methods.
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) has emerged as a
tool well-suited to the partitioning of MTMM data envi-
sioned by Campbell and Fiske (Jöreskog, 1974; Widaman,
1985). According to Marsh and Grayson (1995, p. 181),
B.A. Nosek & F.L. Smyth: A Multitrait-Multimethod Validation of the Implicit Association Test 15
© 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):14–29
1 The two-factor model for the males-females attitude domain failed to converge, leaving the hypothesis untested for this domain.
2 Cunningham, Nezlek et al. (2004) footnoted this limitation, but argued that, since a control IAT, birds vs. trees, did not load with five
ingroup-outgroup IATs on an implicit ethnocentrism factor, IAT method variance was not a strong driver of the two-factor solution. They
further suggested that if “systematic measurement error alone” was responsible for the implicit ethnocentrism factor, then the substantial
correlation between implicit and explicit ethnocentrism (r = .47) would be unlikely. We do not disagree, and we test this supposition
systematically.

Table 1. Results of one- and two-factor (oblique) attitude models fit to measures of 57 topics, a structural equation model
reanalysis of Nosek (2005)
Attitude topic comparisons One-factor model Two-factor model
AB N χ² ε
a
90%CIε
a
χ² ε
a
90%CIε
a
Factors r (t)
Whites Asians 279 44 .28 .21–.35 1.4 .04 .00–.17 .01 (0.1)
Cold Hot 211 262 .79 .71–.87 0.5 .00 .00–.16 .13 (2.1)
Skirts Pants 255 73 .37 .30–.45 0.1 .00 .00–.10 .15 (3.1)
Future Past 235 76 .40 .32–.48 3 .10 .00–.23 .26 (1.9)
Thin people Fat people 275 50 .30 .23–.37 0.4 .00 .00–.14 .27 (2.0)
Approaching Avoiding 180 28 .27 .19–.36 0.3 .00 .00–.16 .29 (1.3)
Simple Difficult 210 56 .36 .28–.44 0.0 .00 .00–..01 .29 (3.4)
Public Private 196 40 .31 .23–.40 0.0 .00 .00–..05 .30 (3.7)
Freedom Security 220 67 .38 .31–.47 1 .00 .00–.16 .31 (2.5)
Short people Tall people 226 53 .34 .26–.42 3 .09 .00–.22 .31 (2.9)
Family Career 238 42 .29 .22–.37 0.1 .00 .00–.12 .33 (2.7)
Married Single 261 169 .57 .50–.64 0.5 .00 .00–.14 .33 (3.4)
Rich people Poor people 222 89 .44 .37–.52 1 .00 .00–.17 .34 (3.2)
Education Defense 214 50 .33 .26–.42 2 .07 .00–.21 .36 (3.7)
Letters Numbers 228 75 .40 .33–.48 0.1 .00 .00–.12 .37 (3.4)
Nerds Jocks 239 80 .40 .33–.48 0.0 .00 .00–..00 .38 (4.0)
Young people Old people 250 42 .28 .21–.36 0.0 .00 .00–.00 .40 (3.1)
Imprisonment Capital punishment 260 66 .35 .28–.43 0.2 .00 .00–.13 .41 (3.7)
Yankees Diamondbacks 200 42 .32 .24–.40 1 .04 .00–.20 .41 (4.5)
Flexible Stable 201 35 .29 .21–.37 0.0 .00 .00–.10 .42 (4.4)
Meg Ryan Julia Roberts 255 41 .28 .21–.35 1 .03 .00–.17 .43 (4.7)
Emotion Reason 175 84 .49 .40–.58 0.0 .00 .00–.09 .44 (3.9)
Conforming Rebellious 208 132 .56 .48–.64 0.4 .00 .00–.16 .44 (4.2)
Summer Winter 260 133 .50 .43–.58 0.4 .00 .00–.14 .44 (5.5)
Leaders Helpers 265 60 .33 .26–.41 2 .07 .00–.19 .45 (5.4)
Tom Cruise Denzel Washington 242 52 .32 .25–.40 0.3 .00 .00–.14 .46 (4.8)
Management Labor 194 47 .34 .26–.43 0.1 .00 .00–.14 .47 (3.9)
Exercising Relaxing 258 185 .60 .53–.67 0.3 .00 .00–.14 .48 (5.8)
Jay Leno David Letterman 196 49 .35 .27–.43 0.0 .00 .00–.04 .48 (4.5)
American places Foreign places 205 44 .32 .24–.40 0.0 .00 .00–.11 .49 (5.3)
Microsoft Apple 205 77 .43 .35–.51 3 .09 .00–.23 .49 (4.1)
California New York 253 66 .36 .29–.43 2 .05 .00–.18 .49 (4.3)
Tea Coffee 250 90 .42 .35–.50 0.0 .00 .00–.00 .50 (5.6)
Abstaining Drinking 249 100 .44 .37–.52 0.2 .00 .00–.13 .50 (5.9)
Christian Jewish 253 77 .39 .31–.46 0.0 .00 .00–.00 .50
(5.2)
Classical Hip Hop 243 106 .47 .39–.54 1 .00 .00–.16 .51 (6.0)
Northerners Southerners 207 62 .39 .30–.47 1 .00 .00–.17 .51 (5.8)
Jews Muslims 243 52 .32 .25–.40 2 .07 .00–.20 .52 (5.5)
Books Television 233 77 .40 .33–.48 0.1 .00 .00–.11 .54 (5.2)
Cats Dogs 258 77 .38 .31–.46 1 .00 .00–.15 .54 (5.6)
16 B.A. Nosek & F.L. Smyth: A Multitrait-Multimethod Validation of the Implicit Association Test
Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):14–29 © 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers

“the operationalizations of convergent validity, discrimi-
nant validity, and method effects in the CFA approach ap-
parently better reflect Campbell and Fiske’s (1959) original
intentions than do their own guidelines.” By comparing the
fits of nested structural models, the relative merits of alter-
native hypotheses concerning the structure of trait (atti-
tude) and method variance can be systematically tested
(Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1979; Loehlin, 2004; McDonald,
1985). We used this approach to distinguish method vari-
ance from trait variance for seven attitude object pairs.
And, within this framework, we tested whether a single
attitude construct or distinct implicit and explicit attitude
constructs provides a better fit for the data.
This MTMM design, because it enables direct modeling
of method factors, increases confidence that a finding of
distinct implicit and explicit attitude factors indicates a sub-
stantive distinction and not one that is driven by confound-
ing influences in the measurement requirements. Our im-
plicit measurement instrument, the IAT, measures associa-
tions between concepts (e.g., thin–fat) and attributes (e.g.,
good–bad) by comparing the average response times for
sorting exemplars of those concepts and attributes in two
distinct response conditions one in which sorting thin and
good exemplars requires a single response and sorting fat
and bad exemplars requires an alternate response, and a
second in which sorting fat and good exemplars requires a
single response and sorting thin and bad exemplars requires
an alternate response.
This method is distinct from attitude self-report in which
a participant self-assesses attitudes by reporting the mag-
nitude of good or bad feelings on a response scale. Because
of their radically different measurement properties, it is
possible that the unique components of the better two-fac-
tor models observed in earlier research are the result of
sources of method variance such as cognitive fluency or
task-switching ability, two known influences on IAT effects
(Mierke & Klauer, 2003; see Nosek et al., 2006, for a re-
view).
Study Overview
Data are from four laboratory studies in which attitudes
toward seven different attitude-object pairs were measured:
flowers–insects, Democrats–Republicans, humanities–sci-
ence, straight–gay, Whites–Blacks, creationism–evolution,
and thin people–fat people. These domains were selected
because on their face they cover a broad range of attitudes.
This was important so that our goal of accounting for
common method variance would not be confounded with
common substantive variance. For example, Cunningham,
Nezlek, and Banaji’s (2004) examination of implicit and
explicit ethnocentrism specifically hypothesized that atti-
tudes for a variety of ingroup-outgroup domains (e.g.,
poor–rich, Blacks–Whites, Jews–Christians) would share a
Attitude topic comparisons One-factor model Two-factor model
AB N χ² ε
a
90%CIε
a
χ² ε
a
90%CIε
a
Factors r (t)
European Americans African Americans 254 39 .27 .20–.35 0.0 .00 .00–.09 .55 (4.7)
American Canadian 290 44 .27 .20–.34 0.5 .00 .00–.14 .55 (4.7)
Teen pop Jazz 239 81 .41 .33–.48 1 .00 .00–.15 .56 (6.1)
Vegetables Meat 234 76 .40 .32–.48 0.3 .00 .00–.14 .56 (5.4)
Social programs Tax reductions 188 77 .45 .36–.53 5 .15 .04–.28 .57 (5.6)
USA Japan 246 49 .31 .24–.39 0.0 .00 .00–.00 .57 (5.2)
Gun rights Gun control 216 85 .44 .36–.52 1 .04 .00–.19 .59 (6.8)
Straight people Gay people 175 35 .31 .22–.40 0.1 .00 .00–.14 .60 (5.3)
Religion Atheism 211 160 .61 .53–.69 2 .05 .00–.20 .61 (7.1)
Coke Pepsi 250 71 .37 .30–.45 1 .00 .00–.16 .66 (7.1)
Liberals Conservatives 215 124 .53 .46–.61 1 .00 .00–.17 .67 (6.9)
Creationism Evolution 231 99 .46 .39–.54 3 .08 .00–.21 .68 (7.2)
Feminism Traditional values 226 75 .40 .33–.48 0.4 .00 .00–.15 .71 (7.4)
Gore Bush 211 61 .37 .30–.46 0.0 .00 .00–.09 .74 (7.2)
Democrats Republicans 195 42 .32 .24–.41 2 .08 .00–.23 .75 (6.7)
Prochoice Prolife 242 52 .32 .25–.40 0.4 .00 .00–.10 .79 (8.6)
Females Males 289 112 .44 .37–.51 * * * *
Note. Attitude object A was implicitly preferred on average. All one-factor models have df = 2, and two-factor models have df =1.ε
a
=
root-mean-square error of approximation. CI = confidence interval. t = r/se. Factor correlations in boldface have t 2.0. * model did not
converge.
Table 1 continued.
B.A. Nosek & F.L. Smyth: A Multitrait-Multimethod Validation of the Implicit Association Test 17
© 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):14–29

common ethnocentrism factor. They reported support for
this idea and also found that implicit and explicit ethnocen-
trism factors were related, but distinct.
Our goal, in one sense, was the opposite of Cunningham
et al.’s: they sought to demonstrate convergent validity be-
tween conceptually related attitude domains revealing an
ethnocentrism factor. We sought to demonstrate discrimi-
nant validity between attitude domains hypothesizing that
the attitude domains would form distinct factors; and con-
vergent validity across measurement types (IAT and self-
report) hypothesizing that the implicit and explicit atti-
tude constructs would be related, but retain distinctiveness
not accounted for by method factors. This simultaneous ex-
amination of discriminant and convergent validity is the
core value of the MTMM approach.
Campbell and Fiske (1959) stressed that “One cannot
define [a construct] without implying distinctions, and the
verification of these distinctions is an important part of the
validational processes” (p. 84). Self-report and IAT mea-
sures were obtained for each attitude object pair (e.g.,
Whites–Blacks) and participants were measured on multi-
ple pairs. We fitted a sequence of nested covariance struc-
ture models, beginning with one in which method variance
was partitioned into latent factors, and we predicted that a
model specifying distinct implicit and explicit attitude fac-
tors would provide the best data fit, whether or not the par-
titioning of method variance proved important.
Method
Participants
A total of 287 Yale University undergraduates from four
data collections in 2000 and 2001 (n = 81, 86, 60, 60) com-
prise the study sample.
Materials
Implicit Association Test (IAT)
One of the four samples received IATs for all seven object
pairs, while the others received subsets of at least four
pairs, including the flowers–insects and Democrats–Re-
publicans pairs (patterns of measured variables are listed
in the Appendix). All IAT category headings and exem-
plars are listed in the Appendix. IAT D scores were com-
puted based on the scoring algorithm suggested by Green-
wald, Nosek, and Banaji (2003), that is, by taking the dif-
ference in mean response latency between the two critical
block conditions and scaling it by the participant’s aver-
age latency standard deviation for both blocks. Most of
the IATs administered across the four samples consisted
of 56 trials in each of the critical double-discrimination
blocks.
3
IAT scores were removed from analysis if more
than 10% of trials were unreasonably fast (< 300 ms) or
if the error rate for any block of trials was greater than
39%. This cleaning resulted in elimination of less than 1%
of IAT scores (14 of 1475).
In all data collections, participants first completed a
flowers–insects IAT with the order of blocks conforming
to that suggested by Greenwald et al. (2003), i.e., (1) a
single-discrimination block of trials for bad–good exem-
plars, followed by (2) a single-discrimination block for
flower–insect exemplars, then (3) a double-discrimination
block of (counterbalanced) either flowers+good/in-
sects+bad or flowers+bad/insects+good, followed by (4)
another single-discrimination block to practice the
switched flower–insect key assignments for the (5) final,
reversed, double-discrimination block. For the remaining
IATs, response blocks for all tasks were randomized and
single-discrimination practice blocks were eliminated.
For example, after the flowers–insects IAT, a participant
could receive the race attitude compatible block of trials
(i.e., compatible with the dominant prejudice) in which
Black faces and bad words are to be categorized with one
response key and White faces and good words are catego-
rized with another key, without any opportunity to prac-
tice the simple, single-discrimination task of sorting Black
from White faces; then the participant could receive the
incompatible block for the gay–straight IAT (gay+good
with one key, straight+bad with the other); then the Dem-
ocrat+bad/Republican+good block; then the incompatible
block for race attitude, etc. This atypical approach pro-
vides a tough test for identifying attitude factors because
the component performance tasks are intermixed with per-
formance tasks for the other attitude domains. In this way,
we allow substantial opportunity for method factors to in-
fluence IAT performance and challenge the hypothesis
that distinguishable attitude factors can be identified de-
spite intermixed performance blocks.
To facilitate latent variable analyses, four IAT D scores
were calculated for each attitude domain based on the dif-
ference between the means of each fourth of the trials, in
turn, across the critical blocks. That is, for IATs with 56
critical trials in each double-discrimination block, the
mean latency for trials 1–14 in one double-discrimination
condition was compared with that of trials 1–14 in the
other, and so on for sets of trials 15–28, 29–42, and
43–56.
4
18 B.A. Nosek & F.L. Smyth: A Multitrait-Multimethod Validation of the Implicit Association Test
Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):14–29 © 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers
3 Exceptions to the 56-trial critical block format were the flowers–insects IAT for two of the samples (40 trials in critical blocks) and the
creation–evolution and thin–fat IATs for 13 participants in one of the samples, also 40 critical trials.
4 We also conducted the analysis with just two IAT indicators, first half vs. second half of trials from each block, and found comparable results
in our comparative structural modeling. Using four indicators was preferable, however, in terms of attaining stable estimates in the more
complex models.

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

When bias binds: Effect of implicit outgroup bias on ingroup affiliation.

TL;DR: It is found that White participants with higher implicit anti-Black bias reported less affiliative responses toward White targets with Black friends compared withWhite targets with White friends, and this effect persisted above and beyond the effects of implicit pro-White bias and explicit racial bias.
Journal ArticleDOI

Cognitive processes in implicit attitude tasks: An experimental validation of the Trip Model

TL;DR: In this article, the authors investigated the underlying processes of the Go/No-go Association Task (GNAT), a go/no-go variant of the well-known Implicit Association Test (IAT).
Journal ArticleDOI

Development of gender identity implicit association tests to assess attitudes toward transmen and transwomen.

TL;DR: The authors developed and validated two gender identity implicit association tests (GI-IATs) designed to assess attitudes toward transsexual men (TransmenIAT) and transsexual women (TranswomenIAT).
Journal ArticleDOI

The role of Christianity and Islam in explaining prejudice against asylum seekers: Evidence from Malaysia

TL;DR: In this paper, a body of literature has revealed that this is partly driven by religious affiliation, and that the population of Malaysia is multirelated and multirelationship is high.
Journal ArticleDOI

On the relationship between implicit and explicit self-esteem: The moderating role of dismissing attachment

TL;DR: This article found that dismissing attachment would increase implicit-explicit self-esteem discordance, and that dismissing style, but not other attachment dimensions, increased implicit self-Esteem discordance.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit

TL;DR: In this paper, two types of error involved in fitting a model are considered, error of approximation and error of fit, where the first involves the fit of the model, and the second involves the model's shape.
Journal ArticleDOI

Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix.

TL;DR: This transmutability of the validation matrix argues for the comparisons within the heteromethod block as the most generally relevant validation data, and illustrates the potential interchangeability of trait and method components.
Journal ArticleDOI

Construct validity in psychological tests.

TL;DR: The present interpretation of construct validity is not "official" and deals with some areas where the Committee would probably not be unanimous, but the present writers are solely responsible for this attempt to explain the concept and elaborate its implications.
Journal ArticleDOI

Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test.

TL;DR: An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute when instructions oblige highly associated categories to share a response key, and performance is faster than when less associated categories share a key.
Journal ArticleDOI

Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling.

TL;DR: In this article, a framework for hypothesis testing and power analysis in the assessment of fit of covariance structure models is presented, where the value of confidence intervals for fit indices is emphasized.
Related Papers (5)
Frequently Asked Questions (1)
Q1. What are the contributions in "A multitrait-multimethod validation of the implicit association test implicit and explicit attitudes are related but distinct constructs" ?

This provides a basis for, but does not distinguish between, dual-process and dual-representation theories that account for the distinctions between constructs.