scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

A rapidly acquired foraging-based working memory task, sensitive to hippocampal lesions, reveals age-dependent and age-independent behavioural changes in a mouse model of amyloid pathology.

TL;DR: Novel insight is provided into the role of the hippocampus and the effects of APP overexpression on memory and search behaviour in an open‐field foraging task in PDAPP mice.
About: This article is published in Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.The article was published on 2018-03-01 and is currently open access. It has received 7 citations till now. The article focuses on the topics: Working memory.

Summary (3 min read)

Introduction

  • Spatial working memory tasks, such as the radial arm maze and Barnes maze, often take advantage of rodent’s natural propensity to forage for food.
  • Pigeons were placed in a large open field area and presented with eight food-baited pots, each in different spatial location.
  • The authors hypothesised that mice with hippocampal lesions would show increased working memory errors, i.e., return visits to depleted pots.
  • Finally, Experiment 3 examined whether foraging behaviour was disrupted in PDAPP mice over the course of ageing.

Subjects:

  • Thirteen mice received bilateral HPC excitotoxic lesions and 13 received control (SHAM) surgery (as described below).
  • The same mice were tested at ages 6-8, 10-12 and 14-16 months of age to ascertain any age-dependent changes in performance in PDAPP mice.
  • The cage floors were covered in sawdust, approximately 1cm deep, and contained a cardboard tube, wooden gnawing block and approved nesting material.
  • Holding rooms were maintained at a stable temperature and relative humidity levels at around 21oC ± 2oC and 60 ± 10% respectively.
  • All animals were health-checked weekly and maintained according to UK Home Office and EU regulations and the Animal Scientific Procedures Act (1986).

Surgery:

  • Mice were anaesthetised with Isoflurane [2-chloro-2- -1, 1, 1- trifluoro- ] in O2 during stereotaxic surgery.
  • A bone flap was removed overlying the infusion sites in each hemisphere (see Table 1A).
  • Mice were also provided with sweetened porridge for 24 hrs.
  • The brain was then extracted and post-fixed in 4% PFA at room temperature (RTP) for 6 hours before being transferred to 30% reagent grade sucrose in dH2O.
  • Slides were left to dry for 48 hours prior to staining.

Cresyl violet staining:

  • Staining of coronal sections was carried out by immersing slides in xylene for 4 minutes before immersion into descending concentrations of ethanol (100% 90% 70%) for 2 minutes per ethanol concentration.
  • Slides were then immersed in dH2O for 2 minutes before 0.005% Cresyl violet was applied for 3 minutes.
  • Sections were then imaged using a Leica DMRB microscope and images were captured using an Olympus DP70 camera and assessed using the programme analySIS-D. Lesion size.
  • Ventral hippocampus was defined as starting from 2.54mm posterior to bregma as described by Paxinos and Franklin (2004).

Apparatus:

  • All training and testing was carried out in a quiet testing room.
  • The same arena was used for all experiments in this study.
  • During initial training, mice were removed from their home cage and placed into an identical home cage with sawdust bedding together with one ceramic pot placed in the centre of the cage, for three successive trials separated by a 5-minute inter-trial-interval.
  • During these sessions the arena was set up with six pots arranged in a circular shape, each 20cm apart .
  • The pots were then wiped clean with 70% ethanol wipes and the milk solution replenished before the next mouse was tested.

Scoring

  • A score of foraging behaviour was defined as a mouse jumping onto the rim of a pot and directing its nose in toward the bottom to consume a reward.
  • As total error incorporated all types of errors made within the trial, the repeat error was able to provide a measure of within-trial memory for foraged pots that was independent of perseverative approach behaviours.
  • The order in which the pots were visited was recorded.
  • This measure assessed the extent to which chaining responses (such as circling behaviour) mediated task performance.
  • In Experiment 3, PDAPP and WT mice were tested using the same procedure described above.

Statistical Analysis

  • Data was analysed using Microsoft Excel for calculation of mean number of errors, times and standard error of the mean.
  • IBM SPSS Statistics software was used to analyse all data statistically.
  • Effect sizes were reported for all statistics: Cohen’s d (d) was calculated for independent sample t-tests, partial eta-squared (ηp2) for ANOVA analysis, Cohen’s r value for Mann-Whitney U tests (r) and Kendall’s W for Friedman tests (Cohen 1973, 1988; Fritz et al. 2012; Tomczak & TomcZak 2014).
  • The data were checked for violations of distribution and homogeneity of variance by Shapiro-Wilk test and Levene’s test respectively.
  • Therefore, data that violated these tests were subjected to transformation (i.e. square root, log-10) based on the level of positive/negative skew and reassessed.

Histology:

  • An example of the maximum and minimum tissue damage obtained as a result of excitotoxic lesions are displayed in Figure 2 respectively.
  • An analysis of these scores revealed that HPC lesioned mice had a significantly higher ratio of error scores in neighbouring pots compared to SHAM controls, t(22)=-2.14, p=0.044, d=0.13.

Discussion

  • This study used a procedurally simple open-field uninterrupted foraging task to evaluate the role of the hippocampus (HPC) in both spatial and non-spatial working memory (SWM).
  • PDAPP mice also displayed an age-independent deficit in non-spatial search strategies in the Barnes maze from 3-5 months of age (Huitrón-Reséndiz et al. 2002).
  • Olton, D.S. & Werz, M.A. (1978) Hippocampal function and behavior: Spatial discrimination and response inhibition.

Lesion Area Mean % Area

  • Errors are defined and examples of when these errors are Error Measurement Definition Example of behaviour Total Error A mouse returning to a pot where the reward was previously consumed.
  • The mouse then forages in pot B before foraging in pot A Distal pot error A mouse making an error in a pot one or more distant from a pot it has just foraged or made an error in.
  • There were no significant group differences within trials.

FIGURES:

  • Illustration of the pot locations during training and test periods, also known as Figure 1.
  • (A) Two pots placed opposite each other in the arena-training phase.
  • (B) Six pots are placed in a radial formation for the test phase of the foraging task.
  • (C) Novel pot designs used in experiment 2.
  • Position of the pots was changed each day, but the radial formation remained.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Figures (5)
Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors used an allothetic place avoidance alternation task (APAAT) with hippocampal lesioned and sham-operated rats and found that hippocampus is an anatomical substrate for the working memory.

3 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper found that only a meager n = 50 publications (i.e., 11.9% of the publications sampled) met the criteria for proper anxiogenic and anxiolytic Lux reported.
Abstract: Behavioral neuroscience tests such as the Light/Dark Test, the Open Field Test, the Elevated Plus Maze Test, and the Three Chamber Social Interaction Test have become both essential and widely used behavioral tests for transgenic and pre-clinical models for drug screening and testing. However, as fast as the field has evolved and the contemporaneous involvement of technology, little assessment of the literature has been done to ensure that these behavioral neuroscience tests that are crucial to pre-clinical testing have well-controlled ethological motivation by the use of lighting (i.e., Lux). In the present review paper, N = 420 manuscripts were examined from 2015 to 2019 as a sample set (i.e., n = ~20–22 publications per year) and it was found that only a meager n = 50 publications (i.e., 11.9% of the publications sampled) met the criteria for proper anxiogenic and anxiolytic Lux reported. These findings illustrate a serious concern that behavioral neuroscience papers are not being vetted properly at the journal review level and are being released into the literature and public domain making it difficult to assess the quality of the science being reported. This creates a real need for standardizing the use of Lux in all publications on behavioral neuroscience techniques within the field to ensure that contributions are meaningful, avoid unnecessary duplication, and ultimately would serve to create a more efficient process within the pre-clinical screening/testing for drugs that serve as anxiolytic compounds that would prove more useful than what prior decades of work have produced. It is suggested that improving the standardization of the use and reporting of Lux in behavioral neuroscience tests and the standardization of peer-review processes overseeing the proper documentation of these methodological approaches in manuscripts could serve to advance pre-clinical testing for effective anxiolytic drugs. This report serves to highlight this concern and proposes strategies to proactively remedy them as the field moves forward for decades to come.

2 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

49,129 citations


"A rapidly acquired foraging-based w..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...Effect sizes were reported for all statistics: Cohen’s d (d) was calculated for independent sample t-tests, partial eta-squared (ηp2) for ANOVA analysis, Cohen’s r value for Mann-Whitney U tests (r) and Kendall’s W for Friedman tests (Cohen 1973, 1988; Fritz et al. 2012; Tomczak & TomcZak 2014)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A straightforward guide to understanding, selecting, calculating, and interpreting effect sizes for many types of data and to methods for calculating effect size confidence intervals and power analysis is provided.
Abstract: The Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (American Psychological Association, 2001, American Psychological Association, 2010) calls for the reporting of effect sizes and their confidence intervals. Estimates of effect size are useful for determining the practical or theoretical importance of an effect, the relative contributions of factors, and the power of an analysis. We surveyed articles published in 2009 and 2010 in the Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, noting the statistical analyses reported and the associated reporting of effect size estimates. Effect sizes were reported for fewer than half of the analyses; no article reported a confidence interval for an effect size. The most often reported analysis was analysis of variance, and almost half of these reports were not accompanied by effect sizes. Partial η2 was the most commonly reported effect size estimate for analysis of variance. For t tests, 2/3 of the articles did not report an associated effect size estimate; Cohen's d was the most often reported. We provide a straightforward guide to understanding, selecting, calculating, and interpreting effect sizes for many types of data and to methods for calculating effect size confidence intervals and power analysis.

3,117 citations


"A rapidly acquired foraging-based w..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...Effect sizes were reported for all statistics: Cohen’s d (d) was calculated for independent sample t-tests, partial eta-squared (ηp2) for ANOVA analysis, Cohen’s r value for Mann-Whitney U tests (r) and Kendall’s W for Friedman tests (Cohen 1973, 1988; Fritz et al. 2012; Tomczak & TomcZak 2014)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This review summarizes data from electrophysiological recordings, lesion studies, immediate-early gene imaging, transgenic mouse models, as well as human functional neuroimaging that provide convergent evidence for the involvement of particular hippocampal subfields in pattern separation.

1,134 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
Jacob Cohen1

1,104 citations


"A rapidly acquired foraging-based w..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...Effect sizes were reported for all statistics: Cohen’s d (d) was calculated for independent sample t-tests, partial eta-squared (ηp2) for ANOVA analysis, Cohen’s r value for Mann-Whitney U tests (r) and Kendall’s W for Friedman tests (Cohen 1973, 1988; Fritz et al. 2012; Tomczak & TomcZak 2014)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: High level production of the pathogenic Aβ42 form of Aβ peptide was associated with an early impairment in TgCRND8 mice in acquisition and learning reversal in the reference memory version of the Morris water maze, present by 3 months of age.

986 citations