scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Achievement emotions and academic performance: longitudinal models of reciprocal effects

TL;DR: The model was tested using five annual waves of the Project for the Analysis of Learning and Achievement in Mathematics (PALMA) longitudinal study, which investigated adolescents' development in mathematics, and showed that positive emotions positively predicted subsequent achievement and negative emotions negatively predicted achievement.
Abstract: A reciprocal effects model linking emotion and achievement over time is proposed. The model was tested using five annual waves of the Project for the Analysis of Learning and Achievement in Mathematics (PALMA) longitudinal study, which investigated adolescents’ development in mathematics (Grades 5–9; N = 3,425 German students; mean starting age = 11.7 years; representative sample). Structural equation modeling showed that positive emotions (enjoyment, pride) positively predicted subsequent achievement (math end-of-the-year grades and test scores), and that achievement positively predicted these emotions, controlling for students’ gender, intelligence, and family socioeconomic status. Negative emotions (anger, anxiety, shame, boredom, hopelessness) negatively predicted achievement, and achievement negatively predicted these emotions. The findings were robust across waves, achievement indicators, and school tracks, highlighting the importance of emotions for students’ achievement and of achievement for the development of emotions.

Summary (4 min read)

Introduction

  • Specifically, longitudinal investigations suggested that K-12 students’ test anxiety and academic achievement reciprocally influence each other (Meece, Wigfield, & Eccles, 1990; Pekrun, 1992).
  • In the following sections, the authors use Pekrun’s (2006; Pekrun & Perry, 2014) control-value theory of achievement emotions to derive a theoretical framework for the reciprocal causation of emotion and achievement.

Effects of Emotion on Achievement

  • In the control-value theory, two dimensions describing human affect are used to distinguish types of emotions, namely valence (positive vs. negative or pleasant vs. unpleasant) and activation (activating vs. deactivating).
  • Accordingly, these emotions are expected to positively influence students’ academic achievement under most task conditions.
  • The opposite pattern of effects is proposed for negative deactivating emotions (boredom, hopelessness).
  • These emotions are thought to reduce cognitive resources and task-related attention, to undermine both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and to promote shallow information processing.
  • Moreover, they can facilitate the use of more rigid learning strategies, such as rote memorization.

Reverse Effects of Achievement on the Development of Emotion

  • Achievement reciprocally influences the appraisals that are considered to be proximal antecedents of emotion.
  • Students should enjoy learning when they judge themselves competent to master the learning task, provided they are interested in the material.
  • Anxiety about an upcoming important exam should be high if students judge themselves incompetent to pass it.
  • Competence and control are thought to influence both students’ momentary emotions within a specific situation and their habitual, re-occurring emotions, which are based on re-occurring appraisals and related control-value beliefs (for summaries of empirical evidence, see Daniels & Stupnisky, 2012; Pekrun & Perry, 2014).
  • Similarly, since achievement has positive effects on control, and control has negative effects on negative emotions, it follows that achievement should have negative effects on the development of negative emotions.

Feedback Loops of Emotion and Achievement over Time

  • Because emotions are posited to influence achievement and achievement, in turn, to influence emotion, the two constructs are thought to be linked by reciprocal causation over time.
  • The existing evidence summarized above implies that negative activating emotions typically are aroused by failure and contribute to subsequent failure, suggesting that feedback loops should be positive for these emotions as well in the average student.
  • At the start of secondary school, students are selected into one of three tracks, including lower-track schools , medium-track schools , and higher-track schools , based on their elementary school achievement.
  • Given the stability of context, the authors expected relations between students’ trait-like emotions considered in this study and their achievement to be stable as well, with effects of these emotions on achievement, and effects of achievement on emotions, showing equivalence (i.e., developmental equilibrium) across each of the one-year intervals included.

Participants and Design

  • The sample consisted of German adolescents who participated in the PALMA longitudinal study (Pekrun et al., 2007).
  • All instruments were administered in the students’ classrooms by trained external test administrators.
  • The sample comprised students from all three school types within the Bavarian public secondary school system as described earlier, including lowertrack schools (Hauptschule, 37.2% ), intermediate-track schools (Realschule, 27.1%), and higher-track schools (Gymnasium, 35.7%).

Measures

  • Students’ emotions in mathematics were measured using the Achievement Emotions Questionnaire-Mathematics (AEQ-M; Pekrun, Goetz, Frenzel, Barchfeld, & Perry, 2011).
  • The scores were also used to derive indexes for positive and negative affect factors combining positive and negative emotions, respectively (see Data Analysis section).
  • Students’ achievement was assessed by their end-of-the-year grades in mathematics as retrieved from school documents and by standardized test scores.
  • Demographic variables (gender and SES) and intelligence were included as covariates in the analysis.

Strategy of Data Analysis

  • Structural equation modeling (SEM; Mplus, Version 7; Muthén & Muthén, 2012) was used to evaluate the reciprocal effects model.
  • The emotion variables were modeled as latent constructs.
  • Metric invariance additionally requires equal factor loadings, scalar invariance requires equal factor loading and intercepts, and residual invariance requires equal factor loadings, intercepts, and residual variances.
  • To examine the robustness of the analysis, the authors replicated the cross-lagged analyses for emotion and achievement with the subsample of students who participated in the study from the beginning (N = 2,070).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) for the Emotion Constructs

  • To further examine the relations between emotions, item-based CFA models including the seven emotions were estimated.
  • This was done separately for the five measurement occasions.
  • The latent correlations between the emotion variables showed the same pattern as the manifest correlations (Table 1).
  • The configural invariance models showed a good fit to the data, with CFI > .93, RMSEA < .03, and SRMR < .05 for all seven discrete emotion constructs (Supplemental Material, Table S3).
  • As compared with these models, the loss of fit for the metric invariance models was CFI < -.004, RMSEA < .001, and SRMR < .006 for all models, indicating clear support for metric invariance for all of the emotions.

Reciprocal Effects Models of Emotions and Achievement

  • The fit indexes provided support for the cross-lagged structural equation models for all seven emotions as well as positive and negative affect and across both measures of achievement.
  • Factor loadings, path coefficients, and residual variances for the reciprocal effects models including grades are displayed in Table 3.
  • In addition, positive paths emerged from each achievement outcome to the subsequent enjoyment and pride variables (all βs = .11, ps < .001).
  • The findings were consistent across models for the seven discrete emotions, the combined positive and negative affect model, four time intervals, two different measures of achievement (grades, test scores), and the three school tracks while controlling for students’ gender, SES, and intelligence.

Reciprocal Effects Linking Emotion and Achievement

  • The findings are congruent with previous evidence showing that emotions and academic achievement are correlated (Goetz & Hall, 2013; Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2014; Zeidner, 1998).
  • These effects are in line with Pekrun’s (2006) control-value theory which posits that emotions influence learning and achievement outcomes.
  • Previous research has produced mixed findings on the relation between students’ positive affect and their learning, with most studies reporting positive relations (see Linnenbrink, 2007) but some others null findings (e.g., Pekrun, Elliot, & Maier, 2009).
  • This difference may relate to general asymmetries in the impact of negative versus positive states and events on human memory and action (see e.g., Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001).
  • The present research adds to this literature by showing that emotions other than anxiety share similar links with achievement.

Discrete Emotions versus General Affect

  • It is noteworthy that the cross-paths were similar across different discrete emotions.
  • As outlined in their reciprocal effects model, success is expected to generally increase perceived control, thus enhancing positive emotions, and failure is expected to decrease control, leading to negative emotions.
  • Regarding effects of emotion on achievement, emotion theories such as the control-value theory (Pekrun, 2006) imply that the effects of some emotions (e.g., deactivating negative emotions such as boredom) may be more consistent than the effects of other emotions (e.g., activating negative emotions such as anxiety).
  • Instead, the findings clearly indicate that the predictive effects of emotions on students’ long-term achievement were also similar across different emotions.
  • This possibility is underscored by the robust findings for positive and negative affect documented in the present analysis.

Effects of Gender, Intelligence, and SES

  • The findings on gender differences are consistent with previous evidence showing that girls report less enjoyment and more anxiety and shame in mathematics even if they perform as well as boys.
  • As such, the findings suggest that girls exhibit a more maladaptive profile of math emotions, except for boredom.
  • As expected, intelligence had substantial predictive effects on the achievement variables.
  • Given that students’ mathematics achievement was included in the analysis, this finding suggests that higher general cognitive ability can help to reduce negative mathematics emotions, above and beyond any effects of students’ academic success in mathematics.
  • Finally, SES also had positive, albeit weaker, effects on math achievement, suggesting that the family exerts an influence on students’ achievement, over and above any effects of cognitive ability.

Limitations, Suggestions for Future Research, and Implications for Practice

  • The present study represents a significant advancement over previous research, because it documents reciprocal effects of emotion and achievement over time while controlling for general cognitive ability and critical demographic background variables.
  • More research on the link between emotion and achievement as mediated by these cognitive and motivational mechanisms is needed to better understand students’ emotions and their relations with important school outcomes.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Durham Research Online
Deposited in DRO:
29 March 2018
Version of attached le:
Accepted Version
Peer-review status of attached le:
Peer-reviewed
Citation for published item:
Pekrun, R. and Lichtenfeld, S. and Marsh, H.W. and Murayama, K. and Goetz, T. (2017) 'Achievement
emotions and academic performance : longitudinal models of reciprocal eects.', Child development., 88 (5).
pp. 1653-1670.
Further information on publisher's website:
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12704
Publisher's copyright statement:
This is the accepted version of the following article: Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H.W., Murayama, K. Goetz,
T. (2017). Achievement Emotions and Academic Performance: Longitudinal Models of Reciprocal Eects. Child
Development 88(5): 1653-1670, which has been published in nal form at https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12704. This
article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance With Wiley Terms and Conditions for self-archiving.
Additional information:
Use policy
The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for
personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that:
a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source
a link is made to the metadata record in DRO
the full-text is not changed in any way
The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.
Please consult the full DRO policy for further details.
Durham University Library, Stockton Road, Durham DH1 3LY, United Kingdom
Tel : +44 (0)191 334 3042 | Fax : +44 (0)191 334 2971
https://dro.dur.ac.uk

Running head: EMOTION AND ACHIEVEMENT
1
Paper accepted for publication in: Child Development
Achievement Emotions and Academic Performance:
Longitudinal Models of Reciprocal Effects
Reinhard Pekrun
Stephanie Lichtenfeld
University of Munich
Herbert W. Marsh
Australian Catholic University and University of Oxford
Kou Murayama
University of Reading
Thomas Goetz
University of Konstanz and Thurgau University of Teacher Education

Running head: EMOTION AND ACHIEVEMENT
2
Author Note
Reinhard Pekrun, Department of Psychology, University of Munich, Munich, Germany;
Stephanie Lichtenfeld, Department of Psychology, University of Munich, Munich, Germany;
Herbert W. Marsh, Institute for Positive Psychology and Education, Australian Catholic
University, Sydney, Australia, and Department of Education, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK;
Kou Murayama, Department of Psychology, University of Reading, Reading, UK; Thomas
Goetz, Department of Empirical Educational Research, University of Konstanz, Konstanz,
Germany, and Thurgau University of Teacher Education, Thurgau, Switzerland.
This research was supported by a LMU Research Chair grant awarded to R. Pekrun by
the University of Munich and four grants from the German Research Foundation (DFG) to R.
Pekrun (PE 320/11-1, PE 320/11-2, PE 320/11-3, PE 320/11-4). Parts of this paper were
presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association,
Philadelphia, PA, April 2014, and at the International Congress of Applied Psychology, France,
Paris, July 2014.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Reinhard Pekrun,
Department of Psychology, University of Munich, Leopoldstrasse 13, 80802 Munich, Germany.
E-mail: pekrun@lmu.de

Running head: EMOTION AND ACHIEVEMENT
3
Abstract
A reciprocal effects model linking emotion and achievement over time is proposed. The model
was tested using five annual waves of the PALMA longitudinal study, which investigated
adolescentsdevelopment in mathematics (grades 5-9; N=3,425 German students; mean starting
age=11.7 years; representative sample). Structural equation modeling showed that positive
emotions (enjoyment, pride) positively predicted subsequent achievement (math end-of-the-year
grades and test scores), and that achievement positively predicted these emotions, controlling for
students’ gender, intelligence, and family socio-economic status. Negative emotions (anger,
anxiety, shame, boredom, hopelessness) negatively predicted achievement, and achievement
negatively predicted these emotions. The findings were robust across waves, achievement
indicators, and school tracks, highlighting the importance of emotions for students’ achievement
and of achievement for the development of emotions.
Keywords: achievement emotion, anxiety, academic achievement, mathematics
achievement, control-value theory

Running head: EMOTION AND ACHIEVEMENT
4
Research has shown that children’s and adolescents’ emotions are linked to their academic
achievement. Typically, positive emotions such as enjoyment of learning show positive links
with achievement, and negative emotions such as test anxiety show negative links (for
overviews, see Goetz & Hall, 2013; Pekrun & Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2014; Zeidner, 1998).
However, most of the available studies were correlational and do now allow any inferences about
the causal ordering of emotion and achievement over time. As such, it remains unclear how the
observed links should be interpreted. It is open to question if students’ emotions impact their
learning, if success and failure at learning influence the development of their emotions, if other
variables cause the association, or if several of these possibilities are at work. Given the need to
acquire knowledge about the antecedents of both students’ achievement and their emotions, this
is an issue of considerable theoretical and practical importance. To address this issue, the present
investigation went beyond merely observing correlations at a single point in time and attempted
to disentangle the temporal ordering of these constructs across multiple waves of data collection
and a developmental time span of several school years.
The investigation is based on a reciprocal effects model of emotion and achievement which
posits that the two variables reciprocally influence each other over time. This stands in contrast
to traditional unidirectional perspectives, which suggest that the link between emotion and
achievement is simply due to effects of emotions on students’ learning and performance. For
example, correlations between test anxiety and students’ achievement were interpreted as
indicating that anxiety impacts achievement, and test anxiety theories put forward various
suggestions about mediating mechanisms (e.g., cognitive interference, motivation; Zeidner,
1998, 2014). In a similar vein, in studies on affect and performance more generally, researchers
have been interested in the impact of moods and emotions on cognitive performance and created

Citations
More filters
01 Jan 2016
TL;DR: The applied missing data analysis is universally compatible with any devices to read and is available in the digital library an online access to it is set as public so you can download it instantly.
Abstract: Thank you for downloading applied missing data analysis. Maybe you have knowledge that, people have look hundreds times for their favorite readings like this applied missing data analysis, but end up in infectious downloads. Rather than enjoying a good book with a cup of tea in the afternoon, instead they juggled with some malicious bugs inside their laptop. applied missing data analysis is available in our digital library an online access to it is set as public so you can download it instantly. Our digital library hosts in multiple locations, allowing you to get the most less latency time to download any of our books like this one. Merely said, the applied missing data analysis is universally compatible with any devices to read.

1,924 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors extended the classic constructive dialogue/debate between self-concept and self-efficacy researchers regarding the distinctions between these two constructs, and demonstrated negative frame-of-reference effects in social (bigfish-little-pond effect) and dimensional (internal/external frame of reference effect) comparisons for three selfconcept-like constructs in each of the first four years of secondary school.
Abstract: This study extends the classic constructive dialogue/debate between self-concept and self-efficacy researchers (Marsh, Roche, Pajares, & Miller, 1997) regarding the distinctions between these 2 constructs. The study is a substantive-methodological synergy, bringing together new substantive, theoretical, and statistical models and developing new tests of the classic jingle-jangle fallacy. We demonstrate that in a representative sample of 3,350 students from math classes in 43 German schools, generalized math self-efficacy and math outcome expectancies were indistinguishable from math self-concept, but were distinct from test-related and functional measures of self-efficacy. This is consistent with the jingle-jangle fallacies that are proposed. On the basis of pretest variables, we demonstrate negative frame-of-reference effects in social (big-fish-little-pond effect) and dimensional (internal/external frame-of-reference effect) comparisons for three self-concept-like constructs in each of the first 4 years of secondary school. In contrast, none of the frame-of-reference effects were significantly negative for either of the two self-efficacy-like constructs in any of the 4 years of testing. After controlling for pretest variables, each of the 3 self-concept-like constructs (math self-concept, outcome expectancy, and generalized math self-efficacy) in each of the 4 years of secondary school was more strongly related to posttest outcomes (school grades, test scores, future aspirations) than were the corresponding 2 self-efficacy-like factors. Extending discussion by Marsh et al. (1997), we clarify distinctions between self-efficacy and self-concept; the role of evaluation, worthiness, and outcome expectancy in self-efficacy measures; and complications in generalized and global measures of self-efficacy. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved)

202 citations


Additional excerpts

  • ...…and Achievement in Mathematics (PALMA; Frenzel, Goetz, Lüdtke, Pekrun, & Sutton, 2009; Frenzel, Pekrun, Dicke, & Goetz, 2012; Marsh, Pekrun, Murayama, Guo et al., 2017; Murayama, Pekrun, Lichtenfeld, & vom Hofe, 2013; Murayama, Pekrun, Suzuki, Marsh, & Lichtenfeld, 2016; Pekrun et al., 2007, 2017)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Analysis of 747 effect sizes accumulated from research conducted between 1992 and 2018 found a small-to-moderate, negative, and statistically significant correlation (r = -.28) between math anxiety and math achievement.
Abstract: Meta-analyses from the 1990s previously have established a significant, small-to-moderate, and negative correlation between math achievement and math anxiety. Since these publications, research has continued to investigate this relation with more diverse samples and measures. Thus, the goal of the present meta-analysis was to provide an update of the math anxiety-math achievement relation and its moderators. Analyzing 747 effect sizes accumulated from research conducted between 1992 and 2018, we found a small-to-moderate, negative, and statistically significant correlation (r = -.28) between math anxiety and math achievement. The relation was significant for all moderator subgroups, with the exception of the relation between math anxiety and assessments measuring the approximate number system. Grade level, math ability level, adolescent/adult math anxiety scales, math topic of anxiety scale, and math assessments were significant moderators of this relation. There is also a tendency for published studies to report significantly stronger correlations than unpublished studies, but overall, large, negative effect sizes are underreported. Our results are consistent with previous findings of a significant relation between math anxiety and math achievement. This association starts in childhood, remains significant through adulthood, is smaller for students in Grades 3 through 5 and postsecondary school, is larger for math anxiety than for statistics anxiety and for certain math anxiety scales, and is smaller for math exam grades and samples selected for low math ability. This work supports future research efforts to determine effective math achievement and math anxiety interventions, which may be most helpful to implement during childhood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

157 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Results for all 3 theoretical models were consistent across the 5 secondary school years, which supports the prediction of developmental equilibrium and highlights the robustness of support over the potentially volatile early to middle adolescent period.
Abstract: Our newly proposed integrated academic self-concept model integrates 3 major theories of academic self-concept formation and developmental perspectives into a unified conceptual and methodological framework. Relations among math self-concept (MSC), school grades, test scores, and school-level contextual effects over 6 years, from the end of primary school through the first 5 years of secondary school (a representative sample of 3,370 German students, 42 secondary schools, 50% male, M age at grade 5 = 11.75) support the (1) internal/external frame of reference model: Math school grades had positive effects on MSC, but the effects of German grades were negative; (2) reciprocal effects (longitudinal panel) model: MSC was predictive of and predicted by math test scores and school grades; (3) big-fish-little-pond effect: The effects on MSC were negative for school-average achievement based on 4 indicators (primary school grades in math and German, school-track prior to the start of secondary school, math test scores in the first year of secondary school). Results for all 3 theoretical models were consistent across the 5 secondary school years: This supports the prediction of developmental equilibrium. This integration highlights the robustness of support over the potentially volatile early to middle adolescent period; the interconnectedness and complementarity of 3 ASC models; their counterbalancing strengths and weaknesses; and new theoretical, developmental, and substantive implications at their intersections. (PsycINFO Database Record

140 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the importance of teachers in relation to the emotions students experience in class was highlighted, arguing that the specific relationship that evolves between teachers and students drives students' emotional experiences.

108 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the adequacy of the conventional cutoff criteria and several new alternatives for various fit indexes used to evaluate model fit in practice were examined, and the results suggest that, for the ML method, a cutoff value close to.95 for TLI, BL89, CFI, RNI, and G...
Abstract: This article examines the adequacy of the “rules of thumb” conventional cutoff criteria and several new alternatives for various fit indexes used to evaluate model fit in practice. Using a 2‐index presentation strategy, which includes using the maximum likelihood (ML)‐based standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) and supplementing it with either Tucker‐Lewis Index (TLI), Bollen's (1989) Fit Index (BL89), Relative Noncentrality Index (RNI), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Gamma Hat, McDonald's Centrality Index (Mc), or root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA), various combinations of cutoff values from selected ranges of cutoff criteria for the ML‐based SRMR and a given supplemental fit index were used to calculate rejection rates for various types of true‐population and misspecified models; that is, models with misspecified factor covariance(s) and models with misspecified factor loading(s). The results suggest that, for the ML method, a cutoff value close to .95 for TLI, BL89, CFI, RNI, and G...

76,383 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, two types of error involved in fitting a model are considered, error of approximation and error of fit, where the first involves the fit of the model, and the second involves the model's shape.
Abstract: This article is concerned with measures of fit of a model. Two types of error involved in fitting a model are considered. The first is error of approximation which involves the fit of the model, wi...

25,611 citations


"Achievement emotions and academic p..." refers result in this paper

  • ...Traditionally, values of CFI and TLI higher than .90 and close to .95, values of RMSEA lower than .06, and values of SRMR lower than .08 were interpreted as indicating good fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Hu & Bentler, 1999)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the sensitivity of maximum likelihood (ML), generalized least squares (GLS), and asymptotic distribution-free (ADF)-based fit indices to model misspecification, under conditions that varied sample size and distribution.
Abstract: This study evaluated the sensitivity of maximum likelihood (ML)-, generalized least squares (GLS)-, and asymptotic distribution-free (ADF)-based fit indices to model misspecification, under conditions that varied sample size and distribution. The effect of violating assumptions of asymptotic robustness theory also was examined. Standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) was the most sensitive index to models with misspecified factor covariance(s), and Tucker-Lewis Index (1973; TLI), Bollen's fit index (1989; BL89), relative noncentrality index (RNI), comparative fit index (CFI), and the MLand GLS-based gamma hat, McDonald's centrality index (1989; Me), and root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) were the most sensitive indices to models with misspecified factor loadings. With ML and GLS methods, we recommend the use of SRMR, supplemented by TLI, BL89, RNI, CFI, gamma hat, Me, or RMSEA (TLI, Me, and RMSEA are less preferable at small sample sizes). With the ADF method, we recommend the use of SRMR, supplemented by TLI, BL89, RNI, or CFI. Finally, most of the ML-based fit indices outperformed those obtained from GLS and ADF and are preferable for evaluating model fit.

9,249 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this chapter a theory of motivation and emotion developed from an attributional perspective is presented, suggesting that causal attributions have been prevalent throughout history and in disparate cultures and some attributions dominate causal thinking.
Abstract: In this chapter a theory of motivation and emotion developed from an attributional perspective is presented Before undertaking this central task, it might be beneficial to review the progression of the book In Chapter 1 it was suggested that causal attributions have been prevalent throughout history and in disparate cultures Studies reviewed in Chapter 2 revealed a large number of causal ascriptions within motivational domains, and different ascriptions in disparate domains Yet some attributions, particularly ability and effort in the achievement area, dominate causal thinking To compare and contrast causes such as ability and effort, their common denominators or shared properties were identified Three causal dimensions, examined in Chapter 3, are locus, stability, and controllability, with intentionality and globality as other possible causal properties As documented in Chapter 4, the perceived stability of a cause influences the subjective probability of success following a previous success or failure; causes perceived as enduring increase the certainty that the prior outcome will be repeated in the future And all the causal dimensions, as well as the outcome of an activity and specific causes, influence the emotions experienced after attainment or nonattainment of a goal The affects linked to causal dimensions include pride (with locus), hopelessness and resignation (with stability), and anger, gratitude, guilt, pity, and shame (with controllability)

6,982 citations


"Achievement emotions and academic p..." refers background in this paper

  • ...The control-value theory (Pekrun, 2006; Pekrun & Perry, 2014) integrates propositions from expectancy-value, attributional, and control approaches to achievement emotions (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Pekrun, 1992; Turner & Schallert, 2001; Weiner, 1985)....

    [...]

  • ...…Effects Model of Emotion and Achievement The control-value theory (Pekrun, 2006; Pekrun & Perry, 2014) integrates propositions from expectancy-value, attributional, and control approaches to achievement emotions (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Pekrun, 1992; Turner & Schallert, 2001; Weiner, 1985)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance at three commonly tested levels: factor loadings, intercepts, and residual variances was examined, and the most intriguing finding was that changes in fit statistics are affected by the interaction between the pattern of invariance and the proportion of invariant items.
Abstract: Two Monte Carlo studies were conducted to examine the sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance at 3 commonly tested levels: factor loadings, intercepts, and residual variances. Standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) appears to be more sensitive to lack of invariance in factor loadings than in intercepts or residual variances. Comparative fit index (CFI) and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) appear to be equally sensitive to all 3 types of lack of invariance. The most intriguing finding is that changes in fit statistics are affected by the interaction between the pattern of invariance and the proportion of invariant items: when the pattern of lack of invariance is uniform, the relation is nonmonotonic, whereas when the pattern of lack of invariance is mixed, the relation is monotonic. Unequal sample sizes affect changes across all 3 levels of invariance: Changes are bigger when sample sizes are equal rather than when they are unequal. Cutoff points for t...

6,202 citations


"Achievement emotions and academic p..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...To establish equivalence of constructs for analyzing correlations and path coefficients, metric invariance is the minimum needed (Chen, 2007; Steenkamp & Baumgartner, 1998)....

    [...]

  • ...To compare model fit, we followed recommendations by Chen (2007)....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (17)
Q1. What are the contributions mentioned in the paper "Running head: emotion and achievement 1 paper accepted for publication in: child development achievement emotions and academic performance: longitudinal models of reciprocal effects" ?

The model was tested using five annual waves of the PALMA longitudinal study, which investigated adolescents ’ development in mathematics ( grades 5-9 ; N=3,425 German students ; mean starting age=11. 7 years ; representative sample ). 

Nevertheless, several limitations should be considered when interpreting the study findings and can be used to suggest directions for future research. As such, although the present analysis used multi-wave longitudinal structural equation modeling and controlled for related variables and autoregressive effects, the possibility still exists that their findings are attributable to other variables that were not included in the study. To balance the benefits and drawbacks of different methodologies and make headway in this avenue of research, future studies should further pursue the approach taken herein while complementing this approach with experimental studies. Additionally, future research should explore if these findings generalize to emotions in achievement domains other than mathematics, 

For positive and negative affect, the loss of fit was CFI < .008, RMSEA < .004,and SRMR < .005 for the metric, intercept, and residual invariance models, demonstratingsupport for invariance for these second-order constructs as well. 

In the proposed model of reciprocal effects, it is posited that effects of emotion on achievement are due to the influence of emotions on cognitive resources, motivation, and strategy use. 

When constraining autoregressive effects, cross-laggedeffects, and factor residual variances to be equal across time intervals, the loss of fit was CFI <.003, RMSEA < .001, and SRMR < .003 for all of the models. 

More specifically, it appears that doing well in school can strengthen students’ positive emotions andreduce their negative emotions over time, whereas doing poorly in school undermines positive emotions and exacerbates negative emotions. 

from the perspective of grades as sources of students’ emotional development, they could be seen as having almost perfectreliability---grades, rather than objective achievement, provide the feedback that shapes students’ perceptions of success and failure and any development based on these perceptions. 

The loss of fit for the scalarinvariance models was CFI < -.007,RMSEA < .004, and SRMR < .007 for all of theemotions, documenting that scalar invariance was supported as well. 

Using these dimensions renders four groups of emotions: positive activating (e.g., enjoyment, hope, pride), positive deactivating (e.g., relaxation, relief), negative activating (e.g., anger, anxiety, shame), and negative deactivating (e.g., boredom, hopelessness). 

The background variables were included as covariates; for each of these variables, directional paths to all of the emotion variables and to all of the achievement variables were included. 

Participants responded on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) scale, and the scores were summed to form the emotion indexes (Alpha range .86 to .92 across all scales and measurement occasions; see Table 1). 

notwithstanding individual differences regarding effects, the authors expect that the average overall influence of positive deactivating emotions on achievement is positive, and that the average overall influence of negative activating emotions is negative. 

FIML has been found to result in trustworthy, unbiased estimates for missing values even in the case of large numbers of missing values (Enders, 2010) and to be an adequate method to manage missing data in longitudinal studies (Jeličič, Phelps, & Lerner, 2009). 

Foreffects of achievement on emotion, this is to be expected, as success and failure are thought to impact the development of different positive and negative emotions in similar ways. 

Because emotions are posited to influence achievement and achievement, in turn, toinfluence emotion, the two constructs are thought to be linked by reciprocal causation over time. 

The present study represents a significant advancement over previous research, because itdocuments reciprocal effects of emotion and achievement over time while controlling for general cognitive ability and critical demographic background variables. 

Perceived competence and control depend on students’ individual achievement history,with success strengthening control and failure undermining it. 

Trending Questions (3)
How emotion affect students learning and performance?

The paper discusses a reciprocal effects model that suggests emotions and achievement influence each other over time. It highlights the importance of emotions for students' achievement and the impact of achievement on the development of emotions.

What are some facts and statistics about positive and negative effects in academic achievement?

The paper provides evidence that positive emotions such as enjoyment and pride positively predict academic achievement, while negative emotions such as anger, anxiety, shame, hopelessness, and boredom negatively predict achievement.

Does anger affect one's ability to do mathematics?

The paper states that negative emotions, including anger, negatively predict achievement in mathematics. Therefore, it can be inferred that anger can affect one's ability to do mathematics.