scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Cognitive Enhancement: Methods, Ethics, Regulatory Challenges

19 Jun 2009-Science and Engineering Ethics (Springer Netherlands)-Vol. 15, Iss: 3, pp 311-341
TL;DR: Present and anticipated methods for cognitive enhancement create challenges for public policy and regulation and raise a range of ethical issues.
Abstract: Cognitive enhancement takes many and diverse forms. Various methods of cognitive enhancement have implications for the near future. At the same time, these technologies raise a range of ethical issues. For example, they interact with notions of authenticity, the good life, and the role of medicine in our lives. Present and anticipated methods for cognitive enhancement also create challenges for public policy and regulation.

Summary (4 min read)

Introduction

  • Cognitive enhancement may be defined as the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind through improvement or augmentation of internal or external information processing systems.
  • A cognitively enhanced person, rather, is somebody who has benefited from an intervention that improves the performance of some cognitive subsystem without correcting some specific, identifiable pathology or dysfunction of that subsystem.
  • The spectrum of cognitive enhancements includes not only medical interventions, but also psychological interventions (such as learned ''tricks'' or mental strategies), as well as improvements of external technological and institutional structures that support cognition.
  • They are often well established and culturally accepted.
  • Nevertheless, these unconventional forms of enhancements deserve serious consideration for several reasons: .

Methods of Cognitive Enhancement

  • Education, Enriched Environments and General Health Education has many benefits beyond higher job status and salary.
  • The relative ease and utility of improving crystallized intelligence and specific abilities have made them popular targets of internal and external software development.
  • Pharmacological cognitive enhancements have physiological effects on the brain.
  • This also suggests that interventions, whether environmental or pharmaceutical, that make exploring and learning more appealing to children might improve cognition.
  • These kinds of intervention might be classified as preventative or therapeutic rather than enhancing, but the distinction is blurry.

Mental Training

  • Mental training and visualization techniques are widely practiced in elite sport (Feltz and Landers 1983) and rehabilitation (Jackson et al. 2004) , with apparently good effects on performance.
  • Such methods have been used since antiquity with much success (Yates 1966; Patten 1990 ).
  • Today, memory techniques tend to be used in service of everyday needs such as remembering door codes, passwords, shopping lists, and by students who need to memorize names, dates, and terms when preparing for exams (Minninger 1997; Lorrayne 1996) .
  • There exists a vast array of mental techniques alleged to boost various skills, such as creativity training, speed reading methods (Calef et al. 1999) , and mind-maps (Buzan 1982; Farrand et al. 2002) .
  • Even if a technique improves performance on some task under laboratory conditions, it does not follow that the technique is practically useful.

Drugs

  • Stimulant drugs such as nicotine and caffeine have long been used to improve cognition.
  • They include stimulants (Lee and Ma 1995; Soetens et al.
  • Diet, and dietary supplements, can affect cognition.
  • It is not yet known whether these drugs also promote useful learning in real-life situations, but beneficial enhancement of memory through pharmacological means is likely to be possible.
  • Modafinil was originally developed as a treatment for narcolepsy, and can be used to reduce performance decrements due to sleep loss with apparently small side effects and little risk of dependency (Teitelman 2001; Myrick et al. 2004 ).

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

  • Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can increase or decrease the excitability of the cortex, thereby changing its level of plasticity (Hummel and Cohen 2005) .
  • While TMS appears to be quite versatile and non-invasive, there are risks of triggering epileptic seizures, and the effects of long-term use are not known.
  • Moreover, individual brain differences may necessitate much adjustment before it can be used to improve specific cognitive capacities.
  • It is still doubtful whether TMS will ever be a practically useful enhancement method.

Genetic Modifications

  • Genetic memory enhancement has been demonstrated in rats and mice.
  • During the maturation of normal animals, synthesis of the NR2B subunit of the NMDA receptor is gradually replaced with synthesis of the NR2A subunit.
  • Given these early results, it seems likely that there exist many potential genetic interventions that would directly or indirectly improve aspects of memory.
  • This would indicate that genetic enhancement of intelligence through direct insertion of a few beneficial alleles is unlikely to have a big enhancing effect.

Prenatal and Perinatal Enhancement

  • Administering choline supplementation to pregnant rats improved the performance of their pups, apparently as a result of changes in neural development (Meck et al.
  • Supplementation of a mother's diet during late pregnancy and 3 months postpartum with long chained fatty acids has also been shown to improve cognitive performance in human children (Helland et al. 2003) .
  • Other tools such as expert systems, symbolic math programs, decision support software, and search agents amplify specific skills and capacities.
  • What is new is the growing interest in creating intimate links between the external systems and the human user through better interaction.

Brain-Computer Interfaces

  • Wearable computers and PDAs (personal digital assistants) are already intimate devices worn on the body, but there have been proposals for even tighter interfaces.
  • Development is rapid, both on the hardware side, where multielectrode recordings from more than 300 electrodes permanently implanted in the brain are currently state-of-the art, and on the software side, with computers learning to interpret the signals and commands (Nicolelis et al.
  • The digital parts of the implant could in principle be connected to any kind of external software and hardware.

Collective Intelligence

  • Much of human cognition is distributed across many minds.
  • Such distributed cognition can be enhanced through the development and use of more efficient tools and methods of intellectual collaboration.
  • When such interconnected information resources exist, automated systems such as search engines (Kleinberg 1999 ) can often radically improve the ability to extract useful information from them.
  • Groups of volunteers with shared interests, such as amateur journalist ''bloggers'' and open source programmers, have demonstrated that they can successfully complete large and highly complex projects, such as online political campaigns, the Wikipedia encyclopedia, and the Linux operating system.
  • Such markets appear to be self-correcting and resilient, and have been shown to outperform alternative methods of generating probabilistic forecasts, such as opinion polls and expert panels (Hanson et al. 2006) .

Safety

  • Safety concerns tend to focus on medical risks of internal biological enhancements.
  • Yet risks accompany any intervention, not just biomedical procedures.
  • Education can enhance cognitive skills and capacities, but it can also create fanatics, dogmatists, sophistic arguers, skilled rationalizers, cynical manipulators, and indoctrinated, prejudiced, confused, or selfishly calculating minds.
  • The risks of chronic use of a cognition enhancing drug include the possibility of both medical side effects and effects more directly tied to the drug's intended function.
  • There are also many forms of enhancement that do not fit into the medical framework, such as psychological techniques and diet, but which nevertheless produce medical effects.

Enhancements for Minors and Incompetent Individuals

  • Young children are not in a position to give informed consent for medical interventions.
  • One might also ask, supposing it became technologically feasible, whether some animals (such as the great apes) ought to be given cognitive enhancements (''uplifted'') to enable them to function at a level closer to that of normal humans.
  • There is, however, currently no clear evidence for the hypothesis that parents making use of enhancement options in procreation would become incapable of accepting and loving their children.
  • This objection appears to apply equally to the use of preimplantation genetic diagnosis to screen embryos for genetic abnormalities, in that abnormal embryos are seen as unworthy of being allowed to develop.
  • It is important to determine the reason for this, and to examine whether there are ethically relevant differences between what may appear to be simply various means to the same end.

Authenticity

  • The issue of authenticity has many sides.
  • Yet, if there were shortcuts to excellence, then access to such shortcuts would instead become the determining factor of success and failure.
  • Society does not denounce athletes for wearing protective (and performance enhancing) shoes, since they enable the athletes to concentrate on interesting talents rather than on developing thick soles.
  • Insofar as cognitive enhancements amplify the capacities required for autonomous agency and independent judgment, they can help a person lead a more authentic life by enabling one to base choices on more deeply considered beliefs about unique circumstances, personal style, ideals, and the options available.
  • Hyper-Agency, Playing God, and the Status Quo The concern about ''hyper-agency'' is in a sense opposite to the concern about authenticity.

Cheating, Positional Goods, and Externalities

  • On some campuses it is now not uncommon for students to take Ritalin when preparing for exams (not to mention caffeine, glucose snacks, and energy drinks).
  • Whether an action constitutes cheating depends on the agreed game rules for different activities.
  • Most cognitive functions, however, are not purely positional goods (Bostrom 2003) .
  • Nevertheless, competitive aspects of enhancements should be taken into account when assessing the impact they might have on society.
  • The case might be compared to that of literacy, which is also forced upon citizens in modern societies.

Inequality

  • Concern has been voiced that cognitive enhancements might exacerbate social inequality by adding to the advantages of elites.
  • Different kinds of enhancements pose different social challenges.
  • Individual cognitive capacity (imperfectly estimated by IQ scores) is positively correlated with income.
  • The medicine-as-treatment-for-disease framework creates problems not only for pharmaceutical companies but also for users (''patients'') whose access to enhancers is often dependent on being able to find an open-minded physician who will prescribe the drug.
  • It is clear that much research and development are needed to make cognitive enhancement practical and efficient.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

ORIGINAL PAPER
Cognitive Enhancement: Methods, Ethics, Regulatory
Challenges
Nick Bostrom Æ Anders Sandberg
Received: 12 August 2006 / Accepted: 25 March 2009 / Published online: 19 June 2009
Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009
Abstract Cognitive enhancement takes many and diverse forms. Various methods
of cognitive enhancement have implications for the near future. At the same time,
these technologies raise a range of ethical issues. For example, they interact with
notions of authenticity, the good life, and the role of medicine in our lives. Present
and anticipated methods for cognitive enhancement also create challenges for public
policy and regulation.
Keywords Cognitive enhancement Ethics Human enhancement IQ
Intelligence Policy
Introduction
Cognitive enhancement may be defined as the amplification or extension of core
capacities of the mind through improvement or augmentation of internal or external
information processing systems. As cognitive neuroscience has advanced, the list of
prospective internal, biological enhancements has steadily expanded (Farah et al.
2004). Yet to date, it is progress in computing and information technology that has
produced the most dramatic advances in the ability to process information.
1
N. Bostrom (&) A. Sandberg
Future of Humanity Institute, Faculty of Philosophy & James Martin 21st Century School,
Oxford University, Littlegate House, 16/17 St Ebbes Street, Oxford OX1 1PT, UK
e-mail: nick.bostrom@philosophy.ox.ac.uk
A. Sandberg
e-mail: anders.sandberg@philosophy.ox.ac.uk
1
Advances in social organization have also enabled individual minds—through interactions with other
people’s minds—to become vastly more effective. Improvements in social organization that are not
directly mediated by technology lie outside the scope of this review.
123
Sci Eng Ethics (2009) 15:311–341
DOI 10.1007/s11948-009-9142-5
www.nickbostrom.com

External hardware and software supports now routinely give human beings effective
cognitive abilities that in many respects far outstrip those of biological brains.
Cognition can be defined as the processes an organism uses to organize
information. This includes acquiring information (perception), selecting (attention),
representing (understanding) and retaining (memory) information, and using it to
guide behavior (reasoning and coordination of motor outputs). Interventions to
improve cognitive function may be directed at any one of these core faculties.
An intervention that is aimed at correcting a specific pathology or defect of a
cognitive subsystem may be characterized as therapeutic.Anenhancement is an
intervention that improves a subsystem in some way other than repairing something
that is broken or remedying a specific dysfunction. In practice, the distinction
between therapy and enhancement is often difficult to discern, and it could be
argued that it lacks practical significance. For example, cognitive enhancement of
somebody whose natural memory is poor could leave that person with a memory
that is still worse than that of another person who has retained a fairly good memory
despite suffering from an identifiable pathology, such as early-stage Alzheimer’s
disease. A cognitively enhanced person, therefore, is not necessarily somebody with
particularly high (let alone super-human) cognitive capacities. A cognitively
enhanced person, rather, is somebody who has benefited from an intervention that
improves the performance of some cognitive subsystem without correcting some
specific, identifiable pathology or dysfunction of that subsystem.
The spectrum of cognitive enhancements includes not only medical interven-
tions, but also psychological interventions (such as learned ‘tricks’ or mental
strategies), as well as improvements of external technological and institutional
structures that support cognition. A distinguishing feature of cognitive enhance-
ments, however, is that they improve core cognitive capacities rather than merely
particular narrowly defined skills or domain-specific knowledge.
Most efforts to enhance cognition are of a rather mundane nature, and some have
been practiced for thousands of years. The prime example is education and training,
where the goal is often not only to impart specific skills or information, but also to
improve general mental faculties such as concentration, memory, and critical
thinking. Other forms of mental training, such as yoga, martial arts, meditation, and
creativity courses are also in common use. Caffeine is widely used to improve
alertness. Herbal extracts reputed to improve memory are popular, with sales of Ginko
biloba alone in the order of several hundred million dollars annually in the U.S. (van
Beek 2002). In an ordinary supermarket there are a staggering number of energy
drinks on display, vying for consumers who are hoping to turbo-charge their brains.
Education and training, as well as the use of external information processing
devices, may be labeled as ‘conventional’ means of enhancing cognition. They are
often well established and culturally accepted. By contrast, methods of enhancing
cognition through ‘unconventional’ means, such as ones involving deliberately
created nootropic drugs, gene therapy, or neural implants, are nearly all to be
regarded as experimental at the present time. Nevertheless, these unconventional
forms of enhancements deserve serious consideration for several reasons:
312 N. Bostrom, A. Sandberg
123

They are relatively new, and consequently there does not exist a large body of
‘received wisdom’ about their potential uses, safety, efficacy, or social
consequences;
They could potentially have enormous leverage (consider the cost-benefit ratio
of a cheap pill that safely enhances cognition compared to years of extra
education);
They are sometimes controversial;
They currently face specific regulatory problems, which may impede advances;
and
They may eventually come to have important consequences for society and
even, in the longer run, for the future of humankind.
In examining the challenges for public policy with regard to cognitive enhancement,
it is important to consider the full range of different possibilities that are becoming
available, and their different individual characteristics. From such a comprehensive
viewpoint, the inadequacies of some aspects of the current regulatory and policy
framework become apparent, as it treats different modes of enhancement differently
even though, arguably, there is no good justification for doing so.
One general caveat must be noted about the survey that follows. Many of the
cognitive enhancement methods being studied today remain highly experimental or
have small effect sizes. This makes the present scientific literature a weak guide to
their eventual usefulness (Ioannidis 2005). Findings need to be repeated in multiple
studies and larger clinical trials before they can be fully trusted. It is likely that many
enhancement techniques will in the long run prove less efficacious than their current
promoters claim. At the same time, the sheer range of enhancement methods suggests
that it would be very unlikely that all current methods are ineffective or that future
advances will fail to produce an increasingly potent toolbox for enhancing cognition.
Methods of Cognitive Enhancement
Education, Enriched Environments and General Health
Education has many benefits beyond higher job status and salary. Longer education
reduces the risks of substance abuse, crime and many illnesses while improving
quality of life, social connectedness, and political participation (Johnston 2004).
There is also positive feedback between performance on cognitive tests such as IQ
tests and scholastic achievement (Winship and Korenman 1997).
Much of what is learned in school is ‘mental software’ for managing various
cognitive domains: mathematics, categories of concepts, language, and problem
solving in particular subjects. This kind of mental software reduces one’s mental
load through clever encoding, organization, or processing. Instead of memorizing
multiplication tables, the pattern of arithmetic relationships is compressed into
simpler rules of multiplication, which in turn (among very ambitious students) can
be organized into efficient mental calculation methods like the Trachtenberg system
(Trachtenberg 2000). Such specific methods have a narrower range of applicability
Cognitive Enhancement 313
123

but can dramatically improve performance within a particular domain. They
represent a form of crystallized intelligence, distinct from the fluid intelligence of
general cognitive abilities and problem solving capacity (Cattell 1987). The relative
ease and utility of improving crystallized intelligence and specific abilities have
made them popular targets of internal and external software development.
Enhancement of fluid intelligence is more difficult.
Pharmacological cognitive enhancements (nootropics) have physiological effects
on the brain. So, too, do education and other conventional interventions. In fact,
conventional interventions often produce more permanent neurological changes
than do drugs. Learning to read alters the way language is processed in the brain
(Petersson 2000). Enriched rearing environments have been found to increase
dendritic arborisation and to produce synaptic changes, neurogenesis, and improved
cognition in animals (Walsh et al. 1969; Greenoug and Volkmar 1973; Diamond
et al. 1975; Nilsson et al. 1999). While analogous controlled experiments cannot
easily be done for human children, it is very likely that similar effects would be
observed. Stimulation-seeking children, who might be seeking out and creating
enriched environments for themselves, score higher on IQ tests and do better at
school than less stimulation-seeking children (Raine et al. 2002). This also suggests
that interventions, whether environmental or pharmaceutical, that make exploring
and learning more appealing to children might improve cognition.
Enriched environments also make brains more resilient to stress and neurotoxins
(Schneider et al. 2001). Reducing neurotoxins and preventing bad prenatal
environments are simple and widely accepted methods of improving cognitive
functioning. These kinds of intervention might be classified as preventative or
therapeutic rather than enhancing, but the distinction is blurry. For instance, an
optimized intrauterine environment will not only help avoid specific pathology and
deficits but is also likely to promote the growth of the developing nervous system in
ways that enhance its core capacities.
In brains that have already been damaged, e.g. by lead exposure, nootropics may
alleviate some of the cognitive deficits (Zhou and Suszkiw 2004). It is not always
clear whether they do so by curing the damage or by amplifying (enhancing)
capacities that compensate for the loss, or whether the distinction is even always
meaningful. Comparing chronic exposure to cognition-enhancing drugs with an
enriched rearing environment, one study in rats found that both conditions improved
memory performance and produced similar changes in the neural matter (Murphy
et al. 2006). The improvements in the drug-treated group persisted even after
cessation of treatment. The combination of drugs and enriched environment did not
improve the rats’ abilities beyond the improvement provided by one of the
interventions alone. This suggests that both interventions produced a more robust
and plastic neural structure capable of learning more efficiently.
Improving general health has cognition-enhancing effects. Many health problems
act as distracters or directly impair cognition (Schillerstrom et al. 2005). Improving
sleep, immune function, and general conditioning promotes cognitive functioning.
Bouts of exercise have been shown to improve temporarily various cognitive
capacities, the size of the effect depending on the type and intensity of the exercise
(Tomporowski 2003). Long-term exercise also improves cognition, possibly
314 N. Bostrom, A. Sandberg
123

through a combination of increased blood supply to the brain and the release of
nerve growth factors (Vaynman and Gomez-Pinilla 2005).
Mental Training
Mental training and visualization techniques are widely practiced in elite sport
(Feltz and Landers 1983) and rehabilitation (Jackson et al. 2004), with apparently
good effects on performance. Users vividly imagine themselves performing a task
(running a race, going to a store), repeatedly imagining every movement and how it
would feel. A likely explanation for the efficacy of such exercises is that they
activate the neural networks involved in executing a skill at the same time as the
performance criteria for the task is held in close attention, optimizing neural
plasticity and appropriate neural reorganization.
General mental activity—‘‘working the brain muscle’’—can improve perfor-
mance (Nyberg et al. 2003) and long-term health (Barnes et al. 2004), while
relaxation techniques can help regulate the activation of the brain (Nava et al. 2004).
It has been suggested that the Flynn-effect (Flynn 1987), a secular increase in raw
intelligence test scores by 2.5 IQ points per decade in most western countries, is
attributable to increased demands of certain forms of abstract and visuospatial
cognition in modern society and schooling, although improved nutrition and health
status may also play a part (Neisser 1997; Blair et al. 2005). On the whole, however,
the Flynn effect seems to reflect a change in which specific forms of intelligence are
developed, rather than an increase in general fluid intelligence.
The classic form of cognitive enhancement software consists of learned strategies
to memorize information. Such methods have been used since antiquity with much
success (Yates 1966; Patten 1990). One such classic strategy is ‘the method of loci’’.
The user visualizes a building, either real or imaginary, and in her imagination she
walks from room to room, depositing imaginary objects that evoke natural
associations to the subject matter that she is memorizing. During retrieval, the user
retraces her imaginary steps, and the sequence of memorized information is recalled
when she ‘sees’ the objects she placed along the route. This technique harnesses the
brain’s spatial navigation system to remember objects or propositional contents.
Other memory techniques make use of rhyming or the fact that one more easily recalls
dramatic, colorful, or emotional scenes, which can serve as proxies for items that are
more difficult to retain, such as numbers or letters. The early memory arts were often
used as a substitute for written text or to memorize speeches. Today, memory
techniques tend to be used in service of everyday needs such as remembering door
codes, passwords, shopping lists, and by students who need to memorize names, dates,
and terms when preparing for exams (Minninger 1997; Lorrayne 1996).
One study which compared exceptional memorizers (participants in the World
Memory Championships) with normal subjects found no systematic differences in
brain anatomy (Maguire et al. 2003). However, it found differences in activity
patterns during memorization, likely reflecting the use of a deliberate encoding
strategy. Areas of the brain involved in spatial representation and navigation were
disproportionately activated in the skilled memorizers, regardless of whether the
items to be memorized were numbers, faces, or the shapes of snowflakes. Asked
Cognitive Enhancement 315
123

Citations
More filters
01 Jan 2011
TL;DR: To understand the central claims of evolutionary psychology the authors require an understanding of some key concepts in evolutionary biology, cognitive psychology, philosophy of science and philosophy of mind.
Abstract: Evolutionary psychology is one of many biologically informed approaches to the study of human behavior. Along with cognitive psychologists, evolutionary psychologists propose that much, if not all, of our behavior can be explained by appeal to internal psychological mechanisms. What distinguishes evolutionary psychologists from many cognitive psychologists is the proposal that the relevant internal mechanisms are adaptations—products of natural selection—that helped our ancestors get around the world, survive and reproduce. To understand the central claims of evolutionary psychology we require an understanding of some key concepts in evolutionary biology, cognitive psychology, philosophy of science and philosophy of mind. Philosophers are interested in evolutionary psychology for a number of reasons. For philosophers of science —mostly philosophers of biology—evolutionary psychology provides a critical target. There is a broad consensus among philosophers of science that evolutionary psychology is a deeply flawed enterprise. For philosophers of mind and cognitive science evolutionary psychology has been a source of empirical hypotheses about cognitive architecture and specific components of that architecture. Philosophers of mind are also critical of evolutionary psychology but their criticisms are not as all-encompassing as those presented by philosophers of biology. Evolutionary psychology is also invoked by philosophers interested in moral psychology both as a source of empirical hypotheses and as a critical target.

4,670 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The use of tDCS in schizophrenia is in the early stages of investigation for relief of symptoms in people who are not satisfied with their response to antipsychotic medication.

434 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is suggested that declarative memory can be improved by stimulants, with some evidence consistent with enhanced consolidation of memories, and the cognitive effects of stimulants on normal healthy people cannot yet be characterized definitively.
Abstract: Use of prescription stimulants by normal healthy individuals to enhance cognition is said to be on the rise. Who is using these medications for cognitive enhancement, and how prevalent is this practice? Do prescription stimulants in fact enhance cognition for normal healthy people? We review the epidemiological and cognitive neuroscience literatures in search of answers to these questions. Epidemiological issues addressed include the prevalence of nonmedical stimulant use, user demographics, methods by which users obtain prescription stimulants, and motivations for use. Cognitive neuroscience issues addressed include the effects of prescription stimulants on learning and executive function, as well as the task and individual variables associated with these effects. Little is known about the prevalence of prescription stimulant use for cognitive enhancement outside of student populations. Among college students, estimates of use vary widely but, taken together, suggest that the practice is commonplace. The cognitive effects of stimulants on normal healthy people cannot yet be characterized definitively, despite the volume of research that has been carried out on these issues. Published evidence suggests that declarative memory can be improved by stimulants, with some evidence consistent with enhanced consolidation of memories. Effects on the executive functions of working memory and cognitive control are less reliable but have been found for at least some individuals on some tasks. In closing, we enumerate the many outstanding questions that remain to be addressed by future research and also identify obstacles facing this research.

416 citations


Cites background from "Cognitive Enhancement: Methods, Eth..."

  • ...The ethics of cognitive enhancement have been extensively debated in the academic literature (e.g., Bostrom & Sandberg, 2009; Farah et al., 2004; Greely et al., 2008; Mehlman, 2004; Sahakian & Morein-Zamir, 2007)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In recent years, "technology-enhanced learning" has become a widely accepted term in the UK and Europe for describing the interface between digital technology and higher education teaching, to a large extent taking the place of other recently popular terminologies such as e-learning, learning technology and computer-based learning.
Abstract: In recent years, ‘technology-enhanced learning’, or ‘TEL’, has become a widely accepted term in the UK and Europe for describing the interface between digital technology and higher education teaching, to a large extent taking the place of other recently popular terminologies such as ‘e-learning’, ‘learning technology’ and ‘computer-based learning’ Yet there has been little critique in the literature of the assumptions embedded within the terminology of TEL: rather it has been adopted as an apparently useful, inoffensive and descriptive shorthand for what is in fact a complex and often problematic constellation of social, technological and educational change This paper subjects the term to a deeper analysis, drawing on insights from critical posthumanism, science and technology studies and Biesta's critique of the ‘learnification’ of education In particular, it foregrounds the instrumentalisation of technology enacted by TEL, explores some of the problematic links between TEL and the philosophy of trans

201 citations


Cites background from "Cognitive Enhancement: Methods, Eth..."

  • ...For example, the definition of ‘cognitive enhancement’ given by Bostrom and Sandberg (2009) provides a useful starting point: Cognitive enhancement may be defined as the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind through improvement or augmentation of internal or external information…...

    [...]

  • ...Where Bostrom and Sandberg (2009) hold up the promise of genetic modification for improved memory, internal hardware tool implantation for an efficient brain–computer interface, and enhanced productivity and better information retention via cognitive enhancement drugs, the UK Technology Enhanced…...

    [...]

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Consider writing, perhaps the first information technology: The ability to capture a symbolic representation of spoken language for long-term storage freed information from the limits of individual memory.
Abstract: Specialized elements of hardware and software, connected by wires, radio waves and infrared, will soon be so ubiquitous that no-one will notice their presence.

9,073 citations


"Cognitive Enhancement: Methods, Eth..." refers background in this paper

  • ...An example is the vision of ‘‘ubiquitous computing’’, in which objects would be equipped with unique identities and given the ability to communicate with and actively support the user (Weiser 1991)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
Jon Kleinberg1
TL;DR: This work proposes and test an algorithmic formulation of the notion of authority, based on the relationship between a set of relevant authoritative pages and the set of “hub pages” that join them together in the link structure, and has connections to the eigenvectors of certain matrices associated with the link graph.
Abstract: The network structure of a hyperlinked environment can be a rich source of information about the content of the environment, provided we have effective means for understanding it. We develop a set of algorithmic tools for extracting information from the link structures of such environments, and report on experiments that demonstrate their effectiveness in a variety of context on the World Wide Web. The central issue we address within our framework is the distillation of broad search topics, through the discovery of “authorative” information sources on such topics. We propose and test an algorithmic formulation of the notion of authority, based on the relationship between a set of relevant authoritative pages and the set of “hub pages” that join them together in the link structure. Our formulation has connections to the eigenvectors of certain matrices associated with the link graph; these connections in turn motivate additional heuristrics for link-based analysis.

8,328 citations

Journal Article
TL;DR: In this article, the authors propose that specialized elements of hardware and software, connected by wires, radio waves and infrared, will soon be so ubiquitous that no-one will notice their presence.
Abstract: Specialized elements of hardware and software, connected by wires, radio waves and infrared, will soon be so ubiquitous that no-one will notice their presence

5,041 citations

15 Aug 2006
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors discuss the implications of these problems for the conduct and interpretation of research and suggest that claimed research findings may often be simply accurate measures of the prevailing bias.
Abstract: There is increasing concern that most current published research findings are false. The probability that a research claim is true may depend on study power and bias, the number of other studies on the same question, and, importantly, the ratio of true to no relationships among the relationships probed in each scientific field. In this framework, a research finding is less likely to be true when the studies conducted in a field are smaller; when effect sizes are smaller; when there is a greater number and lesser pre-selection of tested relationships; where there is greater flexibility in designs, definitions, outcomes, and analytical modes; when there is greater financial and other interest and prejudice; and when more teams are involved in a scientific field in chase of statistical significance. Simulations show that for most study designs and settings, it is more likely for a research claim to be false than true. Moreover, for many current scientific fields, claimed research findings may often be simply accurate measures of the prevailing bias. In this essay, I discuss the implications of these problems for the conduct and interpretation of research.

5,003 citations

Book
01 Jan 2004
TL;DR: Surowiecki explores a seemingly counter-intuitive idea that has profound implications as mentioned in this paper : Decisions taken by a large group, even if the individuals within the group aren't smart, are always better than decisions made by small numbers of 'experts'.
Abstract: In this landmark work, NEW YORKER columnist James Surowiecki explores a seemingly counter-intuitive idea that has profound implications. Decisions taken by a large group, even if the individuals within the group aren't smart, are always better than decisions made by small numbers of 'experts'. This seemingly simply notion has endless and major ramifications for how businesses operate, how knowledge is advanced, how economies are (or should be) organised and how nation-states fare. With great erudition, Surowiecki ranges across the disciplines of psychology, economics, statistics and history to show just how this principle operates in the real world. Along the way Surowiecki asks a number of intriguing questions about a subject few of us actually understand - economics. What are prices? How does money work? Why do we have corporations? Does advertising work? His answers, rendered in a delightfully clear prose, demystify daunting prospects. As Surowiecki writes: 'The hero of this book is, in a curious sense, an idea, a hero whose story ends up shedding dramatic new light on the landscapes of business, politics and society'.

3,256 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (16)
Q1. What are the contributions in "Cognitive enhancement: methods, ethics, regulatory challenges" ?

Cognitive enhancement is the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind through improvement or augmentation of internal or external information processing systems this paper. 

Proponents of a positive right to enhancements could argue for their position on grounds of fairness or equality, or on grounds of a public interest in 6 For an argument that it should, see James Hughes ’ Citizen Cyborg Why Democratic Societies Must Respond to the Redesigned Human of the Future ( Hughes 2004 ). 

Stimulants enhance memory by increasing neuronal activation or by releasing neuromodulators, facilitating the synaptic changes that underlie learning. 

The ultimate criterion of efficacy would be various forms of life success rather than performance in narrow psychological lab tests. 

Because of the low cost and large potential impact of enriched infant formula if applied at a population level, it should be a priority to conduct more research to establish the optimal composition of infant formula. 

Given the availability of external memory support, from writing to wearable computers, it is likely that the crucial form of memory demand on humans in the future will increasingly be the ability to link information into usable concepts, associations, and skills rather than the ability to memorize large amounts of raw data. 

memory techniques tend to be used in service of everyday needs such as remembering door codes, passwords, shopping lists, and by students who need to memorize names, dates, and terms when preparing for exams (Minninger 1997; Lorrayne 1996). 

competitive aspects of enhancements should be taken into account when assessing the impact they might have on society. 

An intervention that is aimed at correcting a specific pathology or defect of a cognitive subsystem may be characterized as therapeutic. 

External hardware is of course already used to amplify cognitive abilities, be it pen and paper, calculators, or personal computers. 

A downside with enhancement licenses is that people with low cognitive capacity, who may have the most to gain from enhancements, could find it hard to get access if the license requirements were too demanding. 

These implants are intended to ameliorate functional deficits and are unlikely to be attractive for healthy people in the foreseeable future. 

As social acceptance of other enhancements increases, and if these are available at a reasonable price, it is possible that social support for people who refuse to take advantage of enhancements will diminish. 

Educational enhancements are widely applied to subjects who are too young to give informed consent to the procedure, and who are unable critically to evaluate what they are being taught. 

Society does not denounce athletes for wearing protective (and performance enhancing) shoes, since they enable the athletes to concentrate on interesting talents rather than on developing thick soles. 

One option would be to establish some baseline level of acceptable risk in allowable interventions, perhaps by comparison to other risks that society allows individuals to take, such as the risks from smoking, mountain climbing, or horseback riding.