scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Coma and consciousness: paradigms (re)framed by neuroimaging.

01 Jun 2012-NeuroImage (Neuroimage)-Vol. 61, Iss: 2, pp 478-491
TL;DR: Advances in measurement support a model of consciousness as the emergent property of the collective behavior of widespread frontoparietal network connectivity modulated by specific forebrain circuit mechanisms.
About: This article is published in NeuroImage.The article was published on 2012-06-01 and is currently open access. It has received 330 citations till now. The article focuses on the topics: Consciousness.

Summary (3 min read)

Keywords

  • Coma, vegetative state, minimally conscious state, fMRI, PET, consciousness, traumatic brain injury *7.
  • Manuscript Click here to view linked References.

Key Points

  • Neuroimaging shows cognition in some patients without motor responsiveness.
  • This improves clinical and pain management of chronic disorders of consciousness.
  • It permits selection for thalamic deep brain stimulation and plasticity enhancement Awareness depends on frontoparietal & mesocircuit functional connectivity Multi-center studies are offering evidence-based diagnostic and prognostic tests.

A brief history of coma

  • The invention of the artificial respirator in the 1950s made it possible for many patients to ―survive‖ their brain damage and led to the redefinition of death based on brain function criteria (i.e., brain death or irreversible coma with absent brainstem reflexes, and to the identification of locked-in syndrome or pseudo-coma (for review see e.g., Laureys, 2005b).
  • Unfortunately persistent and permanent VS share the abbreviation ―PVS‖ often leading to unwarranted confusion.
  • Emergence from MCS was defined by functional communication or functional use of objects.
  • While such a conflation has always implied a failure to adhere to the strict definitions of VS, as MCS explicitly subcategorizes patients within the overly broad category of severe disability and not a subset of patients that would have fulfilled the definitional criteria for VS.
  • As the authors will see, recent studies have demonstrated it is important to disentangle both clinical entities as functional neuroimaging have shown differences in residual cerebral processing and hence, conscious perception (Boly et al., 2008a; Coleman et al., 2009; Rodriguez Moreno et al., 2010; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2010), as well as important differences in outcome (e.g., Luaute et al.).

The challenge of measuring consciousness

  • For neurologists, consciousness can be reduced to arousal (i.e., wakefulness or vigilance) and awareness (i.e., comprising all subjective perceptions, feelings and thoughts) (Posner et al., 2007).
  • Awareness in turn can be divided into ―external awareness‖ (i.e., sensory or perceptual awareness of the environment) and ―internal awareness‖ (i.e., stimulusindependent thoughts, mental imagery, inner speech, daydreaming or mind wandering) (Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2011).
  • Clinically, arousal is typically measured by examining eyeopening and reproducible command-following to establish proof of awareness.
  • The bedside examination of consciousness in severely brain damaged patients, however, is extremely challenging because movements can be very small, inconsistent and easily exhausted, potentially leading to diagnostic errors.
  • This issue is further complicated when patients have underlying deficits in the domain of verbal or non-verbal communication functions, such as aphasia, agnosia or apraxia (Bruno et al., 2010a; Majerus et al., 2009; 2005).

Measuring the brain at “rest”

  • Voxel-based statistical analytical tools next permitted to recognize not only global or a priori defined region-of-interest changes in brain function but more detailed data-driven regional differences and more importantly assessed changes in effective connectivity when VS was compared with healthy conscious waking.
  • Structural MRI studies such as diffusion tensor imaging also permit to quantify lesions to the brain‘s white matter tracts in severe brain injury, often invisible to conventional radiological approaches (Newcombe et al., 2010) and may help differentiating VS from MCS (2010a).
  • The study of VS patients who subsequently recovered offered an additional causal link between consciousness and the functional integrity of this long-range frontoparietal network – pointing to the role of non-specific (central intralaminar) thalamic projections in the support of these large-scale distributed cortico-cortical connections (Laureys et al., 2000b).

INSERT TABLE 1

  • From “Activation” studies to passive stimulations Menon et al. (1998b) first claimed to have demonstrated residual ‗cognition‘ in a VS patient using functional neuroimaging techniques.
  • These findings should strongly influence physicians to systematically use analgesic agents in MCS patients, even if (by definition) they cannot communicate their sensations (Schnakers et al., 2010).
  • Bardin et al. (2011b) demonstrated two examples where fMRI based command following did not lead to communication using this signal.
  • This methodology was also adapted by asking patients to count the number of deviant trials in an auditory oddball series (Bekinschtein et al., 2009a).

INSERT TABLE 3

  • Table 3 offers an overview of the fMRI, EEG, evoked potential and EMG studies aiming to show signs of consciousness and communication not accessible by bedside behavioral examination.
  • The clinical management of these disorders of consciousness remains very challenging, but technological advances in neuroimaging are now offering new ways to improve their diagnosis.
  • Taken together, with the studies of passive stimuli in MCS patients, these findings suggested that relatively intact cerebral integrative processes remained, but like the unreliable behavioral responsiveness seen in these patients, neuronal responses were similarly unstable.
  • These brain regions are known to have a very high resting metabolic rate that dominates the pattern of resting brain activity and may represent a ‗default‘ state as proposed by Raichle (2007).
  • A proof-of-concept study demonstrated that despite long-standing MCS level function, a patient 6 years after injury recovered multiple cognitively-mediated behaviors after placement of bilateral central thalamic deep brain stimulation electrodes (Schiff et al., 2007).

Modeling consciousness

  • An organizing mesocircuit model provides an economical explanation of the vulnerability of the anterior forebrain in the setting of widespread deafferentation and neuronal cell loss associated with a variety of severe brain injuries that produce unstable levels of behavioral responsiveness associated with MCS and patients just past the border of MCS in confusional state .
  • The thalamocortical projections from the central thalamus strongly innervate both the frontal cortex and the striatum (see refs for further details Schiff, 2010).
  • Schnakers et al. (2008a) showed specific changes in frontoparietal metabolism induced by amantadine .

A common model for changes in precuneus/posterior medial parietal complex and

  • Anterior forebrain mesocircuit during recovery of consciousness.
  • This pattern of evoked cerebral activity is consistent with projections from the central thalamus to medial parietal cortical regions.
  • A longitudinal assessment of this patient identified regions that showed significant change in measured fractional anisotropy over a 18 month time period beginning over 20 years following injury.
  • It is clear from this overview that their understanding of consciousness and disorders of consciousness after coma is currently witnessing a significant paradigm shift (Laureys and Boly, 2008; Owen et al., 2009).
  • Moreover, for the group of patients with capacity to communicate these scientific discoveries bear on their fundamental rights for accurate diagnostic assessments and the basics of clinical care (Fins, 2009b; Fins et al., 2008b).

Figures

  • The monolithic way of looking at severe brain damage (in grey) is being replaced by a more graded nosology (in white) based on quantitative behavioral assessments and functional neuroimaging methods.
  • Low level activation 3 VS High level activation 5 MCS Low/high level activation (Fernandez-Espejo et al., 2008) 7 2 VS TBI 1-11m auditory (forward/backward speech) 2011 fMRI 5/43 included VS 4TBI 1TBI-anoxic 10d Motor task Activation of contralateral dorsal premotor cortex in 2VS Bardin et al.
  • Yes (motor mental imagery) -or-no (spatial mental imagery) autobiographical questions.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The state of the science with regard to clinical management of patients with prolonged disorders of consciousness is described, and consciousness-altering pathophysiological mechanisms, specific clinical syndromes, and novel diagnostic and prognostic applications of advanced neuroimaging and electrophysiological procedures are reviewed.
Abstract: The concept of consciousness continues to defy definition and elude the grasp of philosophical and scientific efforts to formulate a testable construct that maps to human experience. Severe acquired brain injury results in the dissolution of consciousness, providing a natural model from which key insights about consciousness may be drawn. In the clinical setting, neurologists and neurorehabilitation specialists are called on to discern the level of consciousness in patients who are unable to communicate through word or gesture, and to project outcomes and recommend approaches to treatment. Standards of care are not available to guide clinical decision-making for this population, often leading to inconsistent, inaccurate and inappropriate care. In this Review, we describe the state of the science with regard to clinical management of patients with prolonged disorders of consciousness. We review consciousness-altering pathophysiological mechanisms, specific clinical syndromes, and novel diagnostic and prognostic applications of advanced neuroimaging and electrophysiological procedures. We conclude with a provocative discussion of bioethical and medicolegal issues that are unique to this population and have a profound impact on care, as well as raising questions of broad societal interest.

564 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Cerebral PET could be used to complement bedside examinations and predict long-term recovery of patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome and active fMRI might also be useful for differential diagnosis, but seems to be less accurate.

395 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A significant anticorrelation between external and internal awareness is found with a mean switching frequency of 0.05 Hz (range: 0.01–0.1 Hz), which is similar to BOLD fMRI slow oscillations.
Abstract: Evidence from functional neuroimaging studies on resting state suggests that there are two distinct anticorrelated cortical systems that mediate conscious awareness: an "extrinsic" system that encompasses lateral fronto-parietal areas and has been linked with processes of external input (external awareness), and an "intrinsic" system which encompasses mainly medial brain areas and has been associated with internal processes (internal awareness). The aim of our study was to explore the neural correlates of resting state by providing behavioral and neuroimaging data from healthy volunteers. With no a priori assumptions, we first determined behaviorally the relationship between external and internal awareness in 31 subjects. We found a significant anticorrelation between external and internal awareness with a mean switching frequency of 0.05 Hz (range: 0.01-0.1 Hz). Interestingly, this frequency is similar to BOLD fMRI slow oscillations. We then evaluated 22 healthy volunteers in an fMRI paradigm looking for brain areas where BOLD activity correlated with "internal" and "external" scores. Activation of precuneus/posterior cingulate, anterior cingulate/mesiofrontal cortices, and parahippocampal areas ("intrinsic system") was linearly linked to intensity of internal awareness, whereas activation of lateral fronto-parietal cortices ("extrinsic system") was linearly associated with intensity of external awareness.

384 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
11 Jun 2014-Brain
TL;DR: It is shown that low-frequency power, electroencephalography complexity, and information exchange constitute the most reliable signatures of the conscious state.
Abstract: In recent years, numerous electrophysiological signatures of consciousness have been proposed. Here, we perform a systematic analysis of these electroencephalography markers by quantifying their efficiency in differentiating patients in a vegetative state from those in a minimally conscious or conscious state. Capitalizing on a review of previous experiments and current theories, we identify a series of measures that can be organized into four dimensions: (i) event-related potentials versus ongoing electroencephalography activity; (ii) local dynamics versus inter-electrode information exchange; (iii) spectral patterns versus information complexity; and (iv) average versus fluctuations over the recording session. We analysed a large set of 181 high-density electroencephalography recordings acquired in a 30 minutes protocol. We show that low-frequency power, electroencephalography complexity, and information exchange constitute the most reliable signatures of the conscious state. When combined, these measures synergize to allow an automatic classification of patients’ state of consciousness.

375 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, a case definition of minimally conscious states (MCS) was presented. But, there were insufficient data to establish evidence-based guidelines for diagnosis, prognosis, and management of MCS, therefore, a consensus-based case definition with behaviorally referenced diagnostic criteria was formulated to facilitate future empirical investigation.
Abstract: Objective: To establish consensus recommendations among health care specialties for defining and establishing diagnostic criteria for the minimally conscious state (MCS). Background: There is a subgroup of patients with severe alteration in consciousness who do not meet diagnostic criteria for coma or the vegetative state (VS). These patients demonstrate inconsistent but discernible evidence of consciousness. It is important to distinguish patients in MCS from those in coma and VS because preliminary findings suggest that there are meaningful differences in outcome. Methods: An evidence-based literature review of disorders of consciousness was completed to define MCS, develop diagnostic criteria for entry into MCS, and identify markers for emergence to higher levels of cognitive function. Results: There were insufficient data to establish evidence-based guidelines for diagnosis, prognosis, and management of MCS. Therefore, a consensus-based case definition with behaviorally referenced diagnostic criteria was formulated to facilitate future empirical investigation. Conclusions: MCS is characterized by inconsistent but clearly discernible behavioral evidence of consciousness and can be distinguished from coma and VS by documenting the presence of specific behavioral features not found in either of these conditions. Patients may evolve to MCS from coma or VS after acute brain injury. MCS may also result from degenerative or congenital nervous system disorders. This condition is often transient but may also exist as a permanent outcome. Defining MCS should promote further research on its epidemiology, neuropathology, natural history, and management.

1,843 citations

01 Jan 2007
TL;DR: The concept of a default mode of brain function arose out of a focused need to explain the appearance of activity decreases in functional neuroimaging data when the control state was passive visual fixation or eyes closed resting as discussed by the authors.
Abstract: The concept of a default mode of brain function arose out of a focused need to explain the appearance of activity decreases in functional neuroimaging data when the control state was passive visual fixation or eyes closed resting. The problem was particularly compelling because these activity decreases were remarkably consistent across a wide variety of task conditions. Using PET, we determined that these activity decreases did not arise from activations in the resting state. Hence, their presence implied the existence of a default mode. While the unique constellation of brain areas provoking this analysis has come to be known as the default system, all areas of the brain have a high level of organized default functional activity. Most critically, this work has called attention to the importance of intrinsic functional activity in assessing brain behavior relationships.

1,819 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This work determined that activity decreases in functional neuroimaging data did not arise from activations in the resting state, and thereby implied the existence of a default mode of brain function.

1,791 citations


"Coma and consciousness: paradigms (..." refers background in this paper

  • ...These brain regions are known to have a very high resting metabolic rate that dominates the pattern of resting brain activity and may represent a “default” state as proposed by Raichle and Snyder (2007)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Giacino et al. as discussed by the authors evaluated the diagnostic utility of the JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) with 80 patients admitted to an inpatient Coma Intervention Program with a diagnosis of either vegetative state (VS) or minimally conscious state (MCS).

1,433 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
08 Sep 2006-Science
TL;DR: Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to demonstrate preserved conscious awareness in a patient fulfilling the criteria for a diagnosis of vegetative state and the patient activated predicted cortical areas in a manner indistinguishable from that of healthy volunteers.
Abstract: We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to demonstrate preserved conscious awareness in a patient fulfilling the criteria for a diagnosis of vegetative state. When asked to imagine playing tennis or moving around her home, the patient activated predicted cortical areas in a manner indistinguishable from that of healthy volunteers.

1,427 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (5)
Q1. What did the authors claim to have demonstrated in a VSpatient?

From “Activation” studies to passive stimulationsMenon et al. (1998b) first claimed to have demonstrated residual ‗cognition‘ in a VSpatient using functional neuroimaging techniques. 

In the human thalamus, the central lateral nucleus and surrounding paralaminar regions receive the heaviest innervations of both brainstem and basal forebrain cholinergic systems and project widely to supergranular cortical regions (Heckers et al., 1992; Van der Werf et al., 2002). 

These observations require other mechanisms that in addition to axonal regrowth likely include changes in synaptic efficacy, pools of available receptors and other fundamental alterations in the cellular profile of individual neurons over time as brain state changes. 

Perhaps the most counterintuitive set of observations of behavioral improvements associated with pharmacological interventions in MCS patients accounted for by the model is the quite paradoxical phenomenon of marked behavioral facilitation occasionally observed with administration of the sedative agent zolpidem (a non-benzodiazepine hypnotic that potentiatesGABA-A alpha 1 receptors, (Brefel-Courbon et al., 2007; Clauss and Nel, 2006; Schiff and Posner, 2007). 

Experimental studies demonstrate that powerful consequences on background firing rates occur even with disfacilitation producing only modest reductions in cerebral blood flow (Gold and Lauritzen, 2002).