scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction : An Image-Based Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies

01 Apr 2017-JAMA Psychiatry (JAMA Psychiatry)-Vol. 74, Iss: 4, pp 387-398
TL;DR: To determine the nature and direction of reward-processing disruptions during anticipation and outcome notification of monetary rewards in individuals with addiction using image-based meta-analyses of fMRI studies, the combination of hypoactivation during reward anticipation and hyper activation during reward outcome in the striatum of individuals with substance addiction may be explained using learning-deficit theory.
Abstract: Importance Disrupted reward processing, mainly driven by striatal dysfunction, is a key characteristic of addictive behaviors. However, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have reported conflicting results, with both hypoactivations and hyperactivations during anticipation and outcome notification of monetary rewards in addiction. Objective To determine the nature and direction of reward-processing disruptions during anticipation and outcome notification of monetary rewards in individuals with addiction using image-based meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Data Sources Relevant publications were identified searching PubMed (inclusion until March 2015) using the following terms: reward, fMRI, substance use, cocaine, cannabis, opiates, alcohol, nicotine, smokers, gambling, gamblers, gaming, and gamers. Authors of included articles were contacted to obtain statistical fMRI maps. Study Selection Inclusion criteria: reward task involving monetary reward anticipation and/or outcome; participants showing addictive behaviors; and healthy control group. Exclusion criteria: participants aged younger than 18 years; recreational substance use or gambling; participants at risk for addictive behaviors; and studies using the same patient data as other included studies. Data Extraction and Synthesis Study procedures were conducted in accordance with the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology guidelines. Using Seed-based d Mapping software, meta-analyses were performed using random-effect nonparametric statistics with group whole brain T -maps from individual studies as input. Analyses were performed across all addictions and for substance and gambling addictions separately. Main Outcomes and Measures Group differences (individuals with addiction vs control individuals) in reward-related brain activation during reward anticipation and outcome using fMRI (planned before data collection). Results Twenty-five studies were included in the meta-analysis, representing 643 individuals with addictive behaviors and 609 healthy control individuals. During reward anticipation, individuals with substance and gambling addictions showed decreased striatal activation compared with healthy control individuals. During reward outcome, individuals with substance addiction showed increased activation in the ventral striatum, whereas individuals with gambling addiction showed decreased activation in the dorsal striatum compared with healthy control individuals. Conclusions and Relevance Striatal hypoactivation in individuals with addiction during reward anticipation and in individuals with gambling addiction during reward outcome is in line with the reward-deficiency theory of addiction. However, the combination of hypoactivation during reward anticipation and hyperactivation during reward outcome in the striatum of individuals with substance addiction may be explained using learning-deficit theory.

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction
An Image-Based Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic
Resonance Imaging Studies
Maartje Luijten, PhD; Arnt F. Schellekens, MD, PhD; Simone Kühn, PhD; Marise W. J. Machielse, MD, PhD; Guillaume Sescousse, PhD
IMPORTANCE
Disrupted reward processing, mainly driven by striatal dysfunction, is a key
characteristic of addictive behaviors. However, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
studies have reported conflicting results, with both hypoactivations and hyperactivations
during anticipation and outcome notification of monetary rewards in addiction.
OBJECTIVE To determine the nature and direction of reward-processing disruptions during
anticipation and outcome notification of monetary rewards in individuals with addiction using
image-based meta-analyses of fMRI studies.
DATA SOURCES Relevant publications were identified searching PubMed (inclusion until
March 2015) using the following terms: reward, fMRI, substance use, cocaine, cannabis,
opiates, alcohol, nicotine, smokers, gambling, gamblers, gaming, and gamers. Authors of
included articles were contacted to obtain statistical fMRI maps.
STUDY SELECTION Inclusion criteria: reward task involving monetary reward anticipation
and/or outcome; participants showing addictive behaviors; and healthy control group.
Exclusion criteria: participants aged younger than 18 years; recreational substance use or
gambling; participants at risk for addictive behaviors; and studies using the same patient data
as other included studies.
DATA EXTRACTION AND SYNTHESIS Study procedures were conducted in accordance with the
Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology guidelines. Using Seed-based d
Mapping software, meta-analyses were performed using random-effect nonparametric
statistics with group whole brain T-maps from individual studies as input. Analyses were
performed across all addictions and for substance and gambling addictions separately.
MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Group differences (individuals with addiction vs control
individuals) in reward-related brain activation during reward anticipation and outcome using
fMRI (planned before data collection).
RESULTS Twenty-five studies were included in the meta-analysis, representing 643
individuals with addictive behaviors and 609 healthy control individuals. During reward
anticipation, individuals with substance and gambling addictions showed decreased striatal
activation compared with healthy control individuals. During reward outcome, individuals
with substance addiction showed increased activation in the ventral striatum, whereas
individuals with gambling addiction showed decreased activation in the dorsal striatum
compared with healthy control individuals.
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Striatal hypoactivation in individuals with addiction during
reward anticipation and in individuals with gambling addiction during reward outcome is in
line with the reward-deficiency theory of addiction. However, the combination of
hypoactivation during reward anticipation and hyperactivation during reward outcome in the
striatum of individuals with substance addiction may be explained using learning-deficit
theory.
JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(4):387-398. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084
Published online February 1, 2017.
Supplemental content
Author Affiliations: Behavioural
Science Institute, Radboud
University, Nijmegen, the
Netherlands (Luijten); Radboud
University Medical Centre,
Department of Psychiatry, Nijmegen
Institute for Scientist Practitioners in
Addiction, Nijmegen, the
Netherlands (Schellekens);
Department of Psychiatry and
Psychotherapy, University Medical
Center Hamburg-Eppendorf,
Hamburg, Germany (Kühn);
Department of Psychiatry, Academic
Medical Center, Amsterdam, the
Netherlands (Machielse); Donders
Institute for Brain, Cognition, and
Behaviour, Radboud University,
Nijmegen, the Netherlands
(Sescousse).
Corresponding Author: Maartje
Luijten, PhD, Behavioural Science
Institute, Radboud University, PO Box
9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, the
Netherlands (m.luijten@bsi.ru.nl).
Research
JAMA Psychiatry | Original Investigation | META-ANALYSIS
(Reprinted) 387
Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 08/26/2022

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
A
ddictive behaviors, including substance use disorders
(SUD) and gambling disorder (GD), are among the most
common and devastating psychopathologies.
1
Sub-
stance use disorders have a prevalence of about 4% to 5% in
the general population
2
and account for about 20% of all dis-
ability-adjusted life years attributed to mental conditions
worldwide.
1
Additionally, 21% of the world population uses
tobacco products, which are associated with many serious
health risks including dependence.
3
The prevalence of GD in
developed countries ranges between 1% and 3% and has been
rising in college students in the past decade.
4,5
Despite the
growing number of evidence-based treatments for SUD, their
efficacy remains moderate, with relapse rates for SUD of
about 50% to 60% within 1 year.
6,7
For GD, only few
evidence-based treatments are available, and their efficacy
has been mostly disappointing.
8
Insight into the neural
mechanisms of addictive behaviors is crucial to target the
pathophysiology of addiction and develop more effective
treatments.
One of the key findings on the pathophysiology of addic-
tive behaviors is a dysfunction of so-called cortico-striatal re-
ward pathways, including the ventral striatum (VS) and the me-
dial prefrontal cortex (mPFC).
9-14
In the past decade, reward
processing in individuals with addictive behaviors has been
extensively studied by measuring brain reactivity to nondrug
rewards (mostly monetary) using functional magnetic reso-
nance imaging (fMRI). However, the nature and direction of
reward-processing dysfunction remain unclear because some
studies show hypoactivations and others hyperactivations (or
nondeviant activations) in the VS of individuals with addic-
tive behaviors compared with healthy control individuals (for
reviews, see Balodis et al,
9
Hommer et al,
10
Diekhof et al,
12
and
Leyton et al
14
).
These conflicting results have been interpreted in the
context of 3 dominant but largely incompatible theories,
known as the reward deficiency syndrome (RDS) theory,
impulsivity theory, and incentive sensitization theory. The
RDS theory
15,16
posits that individuals with addictive behav-
iors have a general deficit in recruiting brain reward path-
ways, resulting in chronic hypoactivation of these circuits
and supposedly reduced pleasurable experience from
rewards. Addictive behaviors, such as substance use or gam-
bling, are consequently initiated to compensate for this
reward deficiency and stimulate brain reward centers,
including the VS. In contrast, the impulsivity theory
17-19
sug-
gests that addictive behaviors are the result of a generally
hyperactive brain reward system. Individuals with a hyperre-
active brain reward system may show a strong response to
cues predicting potential rewards, thereby explaining novelty
seeking, impulsivity, and the continuous drive and motiva-
tion to obtain substances of abuse or other potentially
rewarding stimuli. Finally, the incentive sensitization
theory
13
proposes that individuals with addictive behaviors
show a bias toward addiction-related cues owing to acquired
incentive salience of these cues. As a result, addictive behav-
iors and associated cues hijack the brain-reward system,
resulting in relatively increased VS responses to drug cues
and relatively reduced VS responses to nondrug cues in
SUD.
20
Note that in the case of GD, where money is the
reward of interest, the incentive sensitization theory would
predict an increased response to gambling/monetary cues in
the brain reward pathways.
Several issues may explain inconsistencies in fMRI find-
ings on reward-processing deficits in addiction. First, differ-
ent studies report activity during reward anticipation vs re-
ward outcome phases, which reflect distinct processes with
different relevance for addictive behaviors.
21
For example, re-
ward anticipation often results from the perception of appe-
titive cues whose incentive value is innate or has been learned
by association with positive outcomes, thus reflecting moti-
vational processes.
22
In contrast, reward outcome is more rel-
evant for learning processes and signaling the salience of new
stimuli.
21,23
While reward anticipation and reward outcome
processes can be separately investigated using fMRI, the domi-
nant addiction theories remain relatively vague in specifying
which of these processes are specifically affected in the course
of addiction. A second reason for previously reported incon-
sistencies relates to the fact that reward-processing deficits in
addicted individuals are likely phase dependent. For ex-
ample, normalization of structural abnormalities within the
reward neuro-circuitry has been described after prolonged
abstinence.
24,25
Furthermore, often co-occurring psychiatric
disorders, such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, also
affect reward processing and have been associated with reward-
anticipation deficits.
26,27
To unravel contradictory findings on reward-processing
dysfunction in addiction, we performed a quantitative image-
based meta-analysis of the fMRI literature, using whole-
brain group T maps from individual studies as input. This ap-
proach is more powerful than traditional coordinate-based
meta-analyses in that it uses the full image information, in-
cluding effect sizes and between-subject variance, in addi-
tion to activation localization.
28,29
Moreover, image-based
meta-analyses partly address issues of limited sample sizes in
individual studies and variability in data analysis. We per-
formed separate analyses focusing on reward anticipation vs
outcome processing in both individuals with SUD and indi-
viduals with GD. Substance use disorders and GD were both
included to test the hypothesis of shared neurobiological
Key Points
Question What are the nature and direction of pathophysiologic
reward-processing disruptions in the brain during anticipation and
outcome notification of monetary rewards in individuals with
addiction to substances and gambling?
Findings In this image-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging
studies, striatal activation during reward anticipation was
decreased among individuals with addiction compared with those
in control groups. During reward outcome, substance-addicted
individuals showed increased activation in the ventral striatum,
whereas gambling-addicted individuals showed decreased
activation in the dorsal striatum, compared with controls.
Meaning These findings provide evidence for both reward
deficiency and learning-deficit theories in addiction.
Research Original Investigation Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction
388 JAMA Psychiatry April 2017 Volume 74, Number 4 (Reprinted) jamapsychiatry.com
Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 08/26/2022

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
mechanisms underlying substance and behavioral addic-
tions. Finally, we explored the influence of psychiatric comor-
bidities and different stages of the addiction cycle on reward
processing.
9,10,30
We hypothesized that a blunted reward re-
sponse (mainly in VS and mPFC) in both SUD and GD sup-
ports the RDS theory, while an increased reward response sup-
ports the impulsivity theory. In contrast, a blunted reward
response in SUD individuals, together with an increased re-
sponse in GD individuals, supports incentive sensitization
theory, given the monetary nature of the task paradigms.
Methods
Inclusion of Studies
A literature search was performed using PubMed. Search terms
“reward” and “fMRI” had to co-occur with 1 of the following
keywords: substance use, cocaine, cannabis, opiates, alco-
hol, nicotine, smokers, gambling, gamblers, gaming, and gam-
ers. Reference lists of included studies were screened to iden-
tify additional relevant articles. Inclusion criteria were inclusion
in PubMed before March 1, 2015, participants showing addic-
tive behavior, reward task involving monetary reward antici-
pation and/or outcome, and inclusion of a healthy control
group. Exclusion criteria were inclusion of adolescents (aged
younger than 18 years); target population with recreational sub-
stance use, gambling or gaming, or at-risk groups (eg, ad-
dicted family members); and studies using the same patient
data as other included studies. Meta-analysis of Observa-
tional Studies in Epidemiology guidelines for meta-analyses
of observational studies
31
were followed in this study. Next,
corresponding authors were contacted via e-mail requesting
to share their group-level fMRI data, ie, T maps representing
monetary reward anticipation and/or outcome contrasts, both
within groups (SUD, GD, and healthy control) and between-
groups (SUD vs healthy control and GD vs healthy control).
Analyses
Seed-Based d Mapping
Our image-based meta-analyses were performed using the soft-
ware Seed-Based d Mapping (SDM, version 4.31, http://www
.sdmproject.com, formerly “Signed Differential Mapping”).
Seed-Based d Mapping allows us to perform voxel-based meta-
analyses using full statistical images as input
29,32
and has been
extensively validated in previous meta-analyses.
33-36
Seed-Based d Mapping recreates voxel-level maps of ef-
fect sizes (Hedge’s d) and their variances based on input T maps,
thereby substantially increasing the sensitivity of voxel-
based meta-analyses compared with coordinate-based
methods.
28,29
Additionally, SDM offers the possibility to have
both positive and negative values in the same map; this pro-
vides richer information and allows the computation of
standard meta-analytic measures such as between-study
heterogeneity. To evaluate cross-study convergence, a random-
effect model is applied in which each study is weighted by the
inverse of the sum of its variance plus the between-study vari-
ance. Thus, studies with larger sample sizes or lower variabil-
ity have a stronger contribution. Meta-analytic effect sizes are
then divided by their standard errors across studies, eventu-
ally leading to SDM Z maps. Given that the distribution of these
Z values typically deviates from normality, a null distribution
is empirically estimated using permutation statistics (ie, ran-
domizations of effect sizes across voxels). Previous simula-
tions have shown that a number as low as 20 permutations al-
ready leads to highly stable estimates.
29
To be on the safe side,
all the analyses reported were based on 50 permutations.
Finally, statistical thresholding is based on a voxel-wise
uncorrected P value less than .005. It has been shown that in
the context of presently used permutation statistics, such a
threshold is equivalent to a corrected P value of .05 and
provides an optimal balance between sensitivity and
specificity.
29
This threshold was complemented by a required
minimum cluster size of 10 voxels (approximately 80 mm
3
)
and a peak-level threshold of Z greater than 1 to further
reduce the possibility of false positives.
Whole-Brain Analyses
Primary analyses examined brain activity during reward an-
ticipation and reward outcome in all individuals showing ad-
dictive behaviors vs healthy control individuals. For reward
anticipation and reward outcome, included T maps were be-
tween-group comparisons of contrasts representing the an-
ticipation/outcome of a monetary win vs the anticipation/
outcome of a neutral, negative, or alternative event (see eTable
1intheSupplement for specific contrasts included per study).
To increase consistency across studies, some of the contrasts
included in the analyses are not those reported in the original
publication but additional contrasts obtained from the au-
thors. Secondary analyses examined the same contrasts sepa-
rately in individuals with SUD and individuals with GD (vs
healthy control individuals). Statistical analyses were re-
stricted to a custom gray-matter mask, following SDM recom-
mendations. Although some of the collected T maps did not
have full brain coverage (n = 5), all of them covered the key
areas of interest including the striatum and mPFC.
The robustness of whole-brain results was examined using
a jack-knife procedure, consisting of systematic repetitions of
the meta-analyses described in previous paragraphs after ex-
cluding 1 study at a time. In the Results section, we report the
number of overlapping jack-knife analyses as an index of rep-
licability of the results.
Region of Interest Analyses
Region of interest analyses were performed to examine the
contributions of individual studies and potential publication
biases more closely. Regions of interest were defined func-
tionally from whole brain maps and regional mean effect sizes,
and variances for individual studies were extracted using the
option integrated in SDM. These data were used to make for-
est plots and funnel plots illustrating striatal activation pat-
terns during reward anticipation and outcome. In the forest
plots, we categorized studies as a function of psychiatric co-
morbidities and phase of addiction to qualitatively assess
whether the results might be potentially driven by some of
these subgroups (see eMaterials in the Supplement for deci-
sion criteria regarding the categorization of studies).
Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction Original Investigation Research
jamapsychiatry.com (Reprinted) JAMA Psychiatry April 2017 Volume 74, Number 4 389
Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 08/26/2022

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Results
Included Studies and Sample Characteristics
The search resulted in 31 studies to be included in the meta-
analyses (Figure 1). We received data from 26 studies (84%).
One of these studies was excluded from the analyses because
of technical problems. The remaining 25 studies
17,37-60
were
included in the analyses; of these, 17 reported brain activa-
tion both during reward anticipation and outcome phases.
Combined, these studies included 643 individuals with ad-
dictive behaviors (mean (SD) age, 33.5 (8) years; 527 men
[82%]) and 609 unique healthy control individuals (mean
(SD) age, 33.2 (8.5) years; 469 men [77%]). Across studies, in-
dividuals with addictive behaviors and healthy control indi-
viduals did not differ in terms of age and sex. Study-specific
details including demographic information and type of
addicted populations are shown in the Table.
In total, 20 studies were included in the analyses of brain
activation during reward anticipation. Two of these studies in-
cluded 2 addicted populations that were each compared with
1 healthy control group, leading to a final number of 22 data
sets available for meta-analyses on reward anticipation. Twenty
studies were included in the analyses of brain activation dur-
ing reward outcome. Three of these studies included 2 ad-
dicted populations that were each compared with 1 healthy
control group, leading to a final number of 23 data sets avail-
able for meta-analyses on reward outcome. See eTable 2 in the
Supplement for the included studies and the number of par-
ticipants per analysis specifically.
Meta-analytic Brain Imaging Results
Whole-brain maps of the main results are available online at
http://neurovault.org/collections/1501.
Reward Anticipation Phase
We first examined group differences across all included stud-
ies, ie, across all addictions. The most striking group differ-
ence was observed in the bilateral striatum, in which individu-
als with addictive behaviors showed decreased responses
during reward anticipation compared with healthy control in-
dividuals (Figure 2A). Further whole-brain analyses revealed
that this pattern was present in individuals with SUD and in
individuals with GD compared with their respective control
groups when analyzed separately (Figure 2A). The extraction
of effect sizes within significant striatal voxels revealed no gross
abnormalities (Figure 3) except for 2 data sets presenting
relatively large effect sizes (see the Sensitivity Analyses sub-
section). Other group differences were visible outside of the
striatum including in the mPFC, anterior cingulate cortex,
amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex (see eTables 3 and eTable 4 in the Supplement). Z
maps showing brain activations separately for addicted and
matched healthy control groups are reported in eFigure 1 in
the Supplement.
Reward Outcome Phase
In contrast to the anticipation phase, individuals with addic-
tive behaviors showed enhanced responses in the ventral stria-
tum during reward outcome compared with healthy controls
(Figure 2B). The extraction of effect sizes revealed no gross ab-
normalities (Figure 4). Importantly, further analyses showed
that this pattern was mostly driven by individuals with SUD,
who showed markedly enhanced activity in the ventral stria-
tum (Figure 2B). In contrast, individuals with GD showed no
significant differences in the ventral striatum compared with
healthy control individuals, but decreased responses in the bi-
lateral dorsal striatum (Figure 2B). Other whole-brain group
differences, including findings in the orbitofrontal cortex, in-
sula, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, are reported in eTable
5 and eTable 6 in the Supplement. Z maps showing brain ac-
tivations separately for addicted and matched healthy con-
trol groups are reported in eFigure 2 in the Supplement.
Sensitivity Analyses
Using a jackknife procedure, we found that the striatal group
differences observed during reward anticipation and out-
come were replicated in virtually all jack-knife analyses, dem-
onstrating the robustness of our results (eFigure 3 in the Supple-
ment). We also reran our analyses after excluding the 2 data
sets
57
with the largest striatal effect sizes (Figures 3 and 4), but
the results remained qualitatively similar, suggesting that these
Figure 1. Flowchart Outlining the Selec tion Procedure of Studies
452 Studies identified
88 Full articles checked
31 Data requested
26 Data received
Included in reward
anticipation analyses
20 Studies
22 Datasets
526 Patients
505 Control
individuals
b
Included in reward
outcome analyses
20 Studies
23 Datasets
506 Patients
475 Control
individuals
b
1 Excluded because of problems
with data analyses
408 Excluded because title/
abstract unrelated to objective
58 Excluded for following reasons
a
30 Task design and analyses
9 No control group
6 Nondependent users
14 Age <18 y
8 At-risk group
1 Review article
2 Same dataset as in other
included study
1 Inclusion based
on reference
list of eligible
articles
a
Studies could be excluded for multiple reasons.
b
This number of control individuals reflects the number of unique control
individuals. Some studies included 2 different addicted groups (eg, cannabis
users and smokers) and 1 reference group of control individuals. Therefore, the
number of datasets is higher than the number of studies.
Research Original Investigation Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction
390 JAMA Psychiatry April 2017 Volume 74, Number 4 (Reprinted) jamapsychiatry.com
Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 08/26/2022

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Table. Participant Characteristics
Source
Type of
Participants,
No. Age, Mean (SD)
Male Sex,
No. (%)
Addiction Diagnosis
and Severity
Active, Initial
Abstinence,
Prolonged Abstinence
Psychiatric
Comorbidity
a
Balodis et al,
37
2012
GD (14); HC
(14)
PG: 35.8 (11.7); HC:
37.1 (11.3)
PG: 10 (71); HC:
10 (71)
DSM-IV diagnosis GD Active gambling No current psychiatric
comorbidity (self-report)
Beck et al,
38
2009
ADI (19); HC
(19)
ADI: 41.8 (6.8); HC:
41.7 (9.0)
ADI: 19 (100); HC:
19 (100)
DSM-IV and ICD-10
diagnoses alcohol
dependence
Initial abstinence
alcohol
No current psychiatric
comorbidity (SCID)
Bjork et al,
39
2008
ADI (23); HC
(23)
ADI: 33.8 (9.1); HC:
34.7 (9.7)
ADI: 12 (52) ; HC:
12 (52)
DSM-IV diagnosis alcohol
dependence
Initial abstinence
alcohol
Psychiatric comorbidity
present (interview)
Bjork et al,
17
2012
ADI (29); HC
(23)
ADI: 30.9 (8.2); HC:
30.1 (5.9)
ADI: 15 (52); HC:
15 (52)
DSM-IV diagnosis alcohol
dependence
Initial abstinence
alcohol
Psychiatric comorbidity
present (interview)
Bustamante et
al,
40
2014
CoDI (17); HC
(18)
CoDI: 37.4 (8.2); HC:
37.5 (5.9)
CoDI: 17 (100);
HC: 18 (100)
DSM-IV diagnosis cocaine
dependence
Prolonged abstinence
cocaine
No current psychiatric
comorbidity (SCID)
Choi et al,
41
2012
GD (15); HC
(15)
GD: 27.9 (6.9); HC:
26.6 (4.3)
GD: 15 (100); HC:
15 (100)
DSM-IV diagnosis GD Active gambling No current psychiatric
comorbidity (SCID)
Cousijn et al,
42
2012
CaDI (32); HC
(41)
CaDI: 21.4 (2.3); HC:
22.2 (2.4)
CaDI: 21 (66); HC:
26 (63)
Heavy cannabis use ≥10
d/mo for ≥2 y
Active cannabis No current psychiatric
comorbidity (MINI)
Fauth-Bühler et
al,
43
2014
GD (80); HC
(89)
GD: 37.4 (9.1); HC:
36.2 (9.4)
GD: 80 (100); HC:
89 (100)
DSM-IV diagnosis GD Mix gambling Psychiatric comorbidity
not excluded (SCID)
Filbey et al,
44
2013
CaDI (59); HC
(27)
CaDI: 23.5 (6.4); HC:
30.3 (10.1)
CaDI:46 (78); HC:
5 (18)
DSM-IV diagnosis cannabis
dependence (58%);
DSM-IV diagnosis cannabis
abuse (10%); all: heavy
cannabis use; ≥4 d/wk for
≥6 mo
Initial abstinence
cannabis
Psychiatric comorbidity
not excluded (SCID)
Goldstein et
al,
45
2007
CoDI (16); HC
(13)
CoDI: 42.8 (4.6); HC:
37.6 (6.8)
CoDI: 12 (75); HC:
9 (69)
DSM-IV diagnosis cocaine
dependence (56%);
DSM-IV diagnosis cocaine
abuse (38%); DSM-IV
diagnosis past
poly-substance abuse (6%)
Mix cocaine Psychiatric comorbidity
not excluded (interview)
Hägele et al,
46
2014
ADI (26); HC
(54)
ADI: 43.3 (7.0); HC:
37.7 (11.1)
ADI: 25 (96); HC:
41 (76)
DSM-IV and ICD-10
diagnoses alcohol
dependence
Mix alcohol No current psychiatric
comorbidity (SCID)
Jansma et al,
47
2013
NDI (10); HC
(11)
NDI: 21.2 (2.5); HC:
25.6 (7)
NDI: 10 (100); HC:
11 (100)
At least 10 cigarettes per
d; FTND mean (SD):
4.3 (0.95)
Active nicotine, initial
abstinence cannabis
No current psychiatric
comorbidity (MINI)
Jia et al,
48
2011 CoDI (20); HC
(20)
CoDI: 38.6 (9.3); HC:
35.3 (10.2)
CoDI: 12 (60); HC:
12 (60)
DSM-IV diagnosis cocaine
dependence
Mix cocaine Psychiatric comorbidity
present (SCID)
Martin et al,
49
2014
NDI (16); HC
(17)
NDI: 31.4 (9.8); HC:
33.7 (10.3)
NDI: 6 (38); HC:
8 (47)
At least 10 cigarettes per
d; FTND mean (SD):
3.6 (1.9)
Active nicotine No current psychiatric
comorbidity (self-report)
Miedl et al,
50
2010
GD (12); OG
(12)
GD: 39.5 (9.3); OG:
33.4 (8.0)
GD: 12 (100); OG:
12 (100)
DSM-IV diagnosis problem
gambling (42%); DSM-IV
diagnosis GD (58%)
Active gambling No current psychiatric
comorbidity (assessment
unknown)
Nestor et al,
51
2010
CaDI (14); HC
(14)
CaDI: 22.1 (4.5); HC:
23.1 (4.5)
CaDI: 12 (86); HC:
11 (79)
Heavy cannabis use 5-7
d/wk for ≥2 y
Mix cannabis No current psychiatric
comorbidity (assessment
unknown)
Patel et al,
52
2013
CoDI (42); HC
(47)
CoDI: 38.5 (7.1); HC:
34.6 (9.0)
CoDI: 24 (58); HC:
26 (56)
DSM-IV diagnosis cocaine
dependence
Active cocaine No current psychiatric
comorbidity (SCID)
Romanczuk-
Seiferth et al,
53
2015
GD (18); ADI
(15); HC (17)
GD: 33.6 (9.5); ADI:
45.4 (10.2); HC:
37.4 (11.8)
GD: 18 (100); ADI:
15 (100); HC:
17 (100)
GD: DSM-IV and ICD-10
diagnosis GD; ADI: DSM-IV
and ICD-10 diagnosis
alcohol dependence
Active gambling,
initial abstinence
alcohol
No current psychiatric
comorbidity (interview)
Rose et al,
54
2014
NDI (28); HC
(28)
NDI: 32.7 (10.0);
HC: 30.1 (7.8)
NDI: 13 (46); HC:
16 (57)
18-40 cigarettes per d;
FTND mean (range):
5.9 (3-9)
Active nicotine No current psychiatric
comorbidity (assessment
unknown)
De Ruiter et al,
55
2009
GD (19); NDI
(19); HC (19)
GD: 34.3 (9.4); NDI:
34.8 (9.8); HC:
34.1 (9.3)
GD: 19 (100); NDI:
19 (100); HC:
19 (100)
GD: All in treatment for
gambling problems; 79%
lifetime DSM-IV diagnosis
GD; NDI: at least 15
cigarettes per d; FTND
mean (SD): 5.1 (1.5)
Initial abstinence
gambling, initial
abstinence nicotine
GD: Psychiatric
comorbidity present
NDI: No current
psychiatric comorbidity
(DIS)
Sescousse et
al,
56
2013
GD (18); HC
(20)
GD: 34.1 (11.6); HC:
31.0 (7.3)
GD: 18 (100); HC:
20 (100)
DSM-IV diagnosis GD Active gambling No current psychiatric
comorbidity (structured
interview)
Van Hell et al,
57
2010
CaDI (14); NDI
(14); HC (13)
CaDI: 24 (4.4); NDI:
25 (4.5); HC:
24 (2.7)
CaDI: 13 (93);
NDI: 11 (79); HC:
11 (85)
CaDI: Heavy cannabis use
≥150 joints last year and
≥1500 joints lifetime; NDI:
≥5 cigarettes per d
(mean = 13)
Initial abstinence
cannabis, active
nicotine
No current psychiatric
comorbidity (interview)
(continued)
Disruption of Reward Processing in Addiction Original Investigation Research
jamapsychiatry.com (Reprinted) JAMA Psychiatry April 2017 Volume 74, Number 4 391
Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ on 08/26/2022

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An updated version of the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model is proposed, which is argued to be valid for several types of addictive behaviors, such as gambling, gaming, buying-shopping, and compulsive sexual behavior disorders.

615 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Meta-analytic data is presented revealing distinct subregions within the vmPFC that correspond to each of these three functions, as well as the associations between these subregion and specific psychiatric disorders (depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, addiction, social anxiety disorder, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder).

503 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
06 Jun 2018-Neuron
TL;DR: Whereas the salience and executive networks showed impairments throughout the addiction cycle, the reward network was dysregulated at later stages of abuse and effects were similar in alcohol, cannabis, and stimulant addiction.

302 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An Activation Likelihood Estimation meta‐analysis of 50 fMRI studies, which used the Monetary Incentive Delay Task (MIDT), to identify which brain regions are implicated in the anticipation of rewards, anticipation of losses, and the receipt of reward, helped clarify the neural substrates of the different phases of reward and loss processing.
Abstract: The processing of rewards and losses are crucial to everyday functioning. Considerable interest has been attached to investigating the anticipation and outcome phases of reward and loss processing, but results to date have been inconsistent. It is unclear if anticipation and outcome of a reward or loss recruit similar or distinct brain regions. In particular, while the striatum has widely been found to be active when anticipating a reward, whether it activates in response to the anticipation of losses as well remains ambiguous. Furthermore, concerning the orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal regions, activation is often observed during reward receipt. However, it is unclear if this area is active during reward anticipation as well. We ran an Activation Likelihood Estimation meta-analysis of 50 fMRI studies, which used the Monetary Incentive Delay Task (MIDT), to identify which brain regions are implicated in the anticipation of rewards, anticipation of losses, and the receipt of reward. Anticipating rewards and losses recruits overlapping areas including the striatum, insula, amygdala and thalamus, suggesting that a generalised neural system initiates motivational processes independent of valence. The orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal regions were recruited only during the reward outcome, likely representing the value of the reward received. Our findings help to clarify the neural substrates of the different phases of reward and loss processing, and advance neurobiological models of these processes.

272 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Findings support a broader interpretation of the social motivation hypothesis of ASD whereby general atypical reward processing encompasses social reward, nonsocial reward, and perhaps restricted interests.
Abstract: Importance The social motivation hypothesis posits that individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) find social stimuli less rewarding than do people with neurotypical activity. However, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of reward processing have yielded mixed results. Objectives To examine whether individuals with ASD process rewarding stimuli differently than typically developing individuals (controls), whether differences are limited to social rewards, and whether contradictory findings in the literature might be due to sample characteristics. Data Sources Articles were identified in PubMed, Embase, and PsycINFO from database inception until June 1, 2017. Functional MRI data from these articles were provided by most authors. Study Selection Publications were included that provided brain activation contrasts between a sample with ASD and controls on a reward task, determined by multiple reviewer consensus. Data Extraction and Synthesis When fMRI data were not provided by authors, multiple reviewers extracted peak coordinates and effect sizes from articles to recreate statistical maps using seed-baseddmapping software. Random-effects meta-analyses of responses to social, nonsocial, and restricted interest stimuli, as well as all of these domains together, were performed. Secondary analyses included meta-analyses of wanting and liking, meta-regression with age, and correlations with ASD severity. All procedures were conducted in accordance with Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology guidelines. Main Outcomes and Measures Brain activation differences between groups with ASD and typically developing controls while processing rewards. All analyses except the domain-general meta-analysis were planned before data collection. Results The meta-analysis included 13 studies (30 total fMRI contrasts) from 259 individuals with ASD and 246 controls. Autism spectrum disorder was associated with aberrant processing of both social and nonsocial rewards in striatal regions and increased activation in response to restricted interests (social reward, caudate cluster:d = −0.25 [95% CI, −0.41 to −0.08]; nonsocial reward, caudate and anterior cingulate cluster:d = −0.22 [95% CI, −0.42 to −0.02]; restricted interests, caudate and nucleus accumbens cluster:d = 0.42 [95% CI, 0.07 to 0.78]). Conclusions and Relevance Individuals with ASD show atypical processing of social and nonsocial rewards. Findings support a broader interpretation of the social motivation hypothesis of ASD whereby general atypical reward processing encompasses social reward, nonsocial reward, and perhaps restricted interests. This meta-analysis also suggests that prior mixed results could be driven by sample age differences, warranting further study of the developmental trajectory for reward processing in ASD.

175 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
19 Apr 2000-JAMA
TL;DR: A checklist contains specifications for reporting of meta-analyses of observational studies in epidemiology, including background, search strategy, methods, results, discussion, and conclusion should improve the usefulness ofMeta-an analyses for authors, reviewers, editors, readers, and decision makers.
Abstract: ObjectiveBecause of the pressure for timely, informed decisions in public health and clinical practice and the explosion of information in the scientific literature, research results must be synthesized. Meta-analyses are increasingly used to address this problem, and they often evaluate observational studies. A workshop was held in Atlanta, Ga, in April 1997, to examine the reporting of meta-analyses of observational studies and to make recommendations to aid authors, reviewers, editors, and readers.ParticipantsTwenty-seven participants were selected by a steering committee, based on expertise in clinical practice, trials, statistics, epidemiology, social sciences, and biomedical editing. Deliberations of the workshop were open to other interested scientists. Funding for this activity was provided by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.EvidenceWe conducted a systematic review of the published literature on the conduct and reporting of meta-analyses in observational studies using MEDLINE, Educational Research Information Center (ERIC), PsycLIT, and the Current Index to Statistics. We also examined reference lists of the 32 studies retrieved and contacted experts in the field. Participants were assigned to small-group discussions on the subjects of bias, searching and abstracting, heterogeneity, study categorization, and statistical methods.Consensus ProcessFrom the material presented at the workshop, the authors developed a checklist summarizing recommendations for reporting meta-analyses of observational studies. The checklist and supporting evidence were circulated to all conference attendees and additional experts. All suggestions for revisions were addressed.ConclusionsThe proposed checklist contains specifications for reporting of meta-analyses of observational studies in epidemiology, including background, search strategy, methods, results, discussion, and conclusion. Use of the checklist should improve the usefulness of meta-analyses for authors, reviewers, editors, readers, and decision makers. An evaluation plan is suggested and research areas are explored.

17,663 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The Global Burden of Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors Study 2010 (GBD 2010) as discussed by the authors was used to estimate the burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders in terms of disability-adjusted life years (DALYs), years of life lost to premature mortality (YLLs), and years lived with disability (YLDs).

4,753 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
24 Apr 1987-Science
TL;DR: In this paper, three dimensions of personality have been described that may reflect individual differences in brain systems modulating the activation, maintenance, and inhibition of behavioral responses to the effects of alcohol and other environmental stimuli.
Abstract: Clinical, genetic, and neuropsychopharmacological studies of developmental factors in alcoholism are providing a better understanding of the neurobiological bases of personality and learning. Studies of the adopted-away children of alcoholics show that the predisposition to initiate alcohol-seeking behavior is genetically different from susceptibility to loss of control after drinking begins. Alcohol-seeking behavior is a special case of exploratory appetitive behavior and involves different neurogenetic processes than do susceptibility to behavioral tolerance and dependence on the antianxiety or sedative effects of alcohol. Three dimensions of personality have been described that may reflect individual differences in brain systems modulating the activation, maintenance, and inhibition of behavioral responses to the effects of alcohol and other environmental stimuli. These personality traits distinguish alcoholics with different patterns of behavioral, neurophysiological, and neuropharmacological responses to alcohol.

2,246 citations

Book
01 Jan 2002
TL;DR: The latest edition of this groundbreaking atlas gives shape and meaning to statistics about tobacco use and control, revealing similarities and differences between countries, and exposing the behaviour of the tobacco companies.
Abstract: By 2025, tobacco will be the leading cause of death and disability worldwide, killing more than 10 million people every year. If nothing is done between now and then, tobacco will kill 650 million smokers. The latest edition of this groundbreaking atlas gives shape and meaning to statistics about tobacco use and control. Full-colour maps and graphics illustrate in a clear and accessible format the wide range of tobacco issues, revealing similarities and differences between countries, and exposing the behaviour of the tobacco companies. It also examines solutions and predicts the future course of the epidemic. Topics include: History of tobacco; Prevalence and consumption; Health risks and mortality; Economic costs; The tobacco industry; Legal regulations and limitations; Health education and quitting; The Framework Convention of Tobacco Control; and The future of the epidemic.

2,080 citations