scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Evidence for early morphological decomposition: Combining masked priming with magnetoencephalography

01 Nov 2011-Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (NIH Public Access)-Vol. 23, Iss: 11, pp 3366-3379
TL;DR: Data regarding the transitional probability from stem to affix in a post hoc comparison is presented, which suggests that this factor may modulate early morphological decomposition, particularly for opaque words.
Abstract: Are words stored as morphologically structured representations? If so, when during word recognition are morphological pieces accessed? Recent masked priming studies support models that assume early decomposition of (potentially) morphologically complex words. The electrophysiological evidence, however, is inconsistent. We combined masked morphological priming with magneto-encephalography (MEG), a technique particularly adept at indexing processes involved in lexical access. The latency of an MEG component peaking, on average, 220 msec post-onset of the target in left occipito-temporal brain regions was found to be sensitive to the morphological prime-target relationship under masked priming conditions in a visual lexical decision task. Shorter latencies for related than unrelated conditions were observed both for semantically transparent (cleaner-CLEAN) and opaque (corner-CORN) prime-target pairs, but not for prime-target pairs with only an orthographic relationship (brothel-BROTH). These effects are likely to reflect a prelexical level of processing where form-based representations of stems and affixes are represented and are in contrast to models positing no morphological structure in lexical representations. Moreover, we present data regarding the transitional probability from stem to affix in a post hoc comparison, which suggests that this factor may modulate early morphological decomposition, particularly for opaque words. The timing of a robust MEG component sensitive to the morphological relatedness of prime-target pairs can be used to further understand the neural substrates and the time course of lexical processing.

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Citations
More filters
01 Jan 1985
TL;DR: This paper discusses one aspect of the lexicon, namely its morphological organization, and describes this kind of lexicon system as most suited for describing suffixing languages with a comparatively high degree of agglutination.
Abstract: 1. In trod u otion In this paper, I w ill discuss one aspect of the lexicon, namely its morphological organization. For about two years I have been working with Koakenniemi's twolevel model (Koakenniemi 1983)i on a Polish two-level description. In this work, I have become more and more Interested in the formalism it s e l f , something that has tended to push work on the language description into the background. It seems to be the case that in moat concrete two-level descriptions, the rule component is forced to carry too heavy a burden in comparison with the lexicon, perhaps because the lexicon is very simple as to its implementation. Like many other lexical systems in computer applications i t is implemented as a tree, with a root node and leaves, from which the lexical entries eire retrieved when the analysts routine has traversed the tree. The twolevel lexicon la a bit more sophisticated than th is , however, in that there la not only one, but several lexicon trees, the so called minilexicons. The user links the mlnilexicona into a whole, moat often into a root lexicon and a number of su ffix lexicons. In Hockett'a terminology, we could apeak of an Itern-and-Arrangement (lA) model (Hockett 1958, pp 386ff). Elsewhere I have characterized this kind of lexicon system as most suited for describing suffixing languages with a comparatively high degree of agglutination (Borin 1985f p 35); This is mainly due to the fact that the system works according to what Blåberg (1984, p 6 l) aptly has termed the "forget-where-you-came-from"

261 citations

Book ChapterDOI
13 May 2013

183 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The data support a model of word recognition in which decomposition is attempted, and possibly utilized, for complex words containing bound roots as well as free word-stems, and find evidence of decomposition for both free stems and bound roots at the M170 stage in processing.
Abstract: We employ a single-trial correlational MEG analysis technique to investigate early processing in the visual recognition of morphologically complex words. Three classes of affixed words were presented in a lexical decision task: free stems (e.g., taxable), bound roots (e.g., tolerable), and unique root words (e.g., vulnerable, the root of which does not appear elsewhere). Analysis was focused on brain responses within 100-200 msec poststimulus onset in the previously identified letter string and visual word-form areas. MEG data were analyzed using cortically constrained minimum-norm estimation. Correlations were computed between activity at functionally defined ROIs and continuous measures of the words' morphological properties. ROIs were identified across subjects on a reference brain and then morphed back onto each individual subject's brain (n = 9). We find evidence of decomposition for both free stems and bound roots at the M170 stage in processing. The M170 response is shown to be sensitive to morphological properties such as affix frequency and the conditional probability of encountering each word given its stem. These morphological properties are contrasted with orthographic form features (letter string frequency, transition probability from one string to the next), which exert effects on earlier stages in processing (∼130 msec). We find that effects of decomposition at the M170 can, in fact, be attributed to morphological properties of complex words, rather than to purely orthographic and form-related properties. Our data support a model of word recognition in which decomposition is attempted, and possibly utilized, for complex words containing bound roots as well as free word-stems.

129 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jul 2019-Cortex
TL;DR: The view that learning to appreciate morphological relationships may be a vital part of acquiring a direct mapping between printed words and their meanings, represented in the ventral brain pathway of the reading network is developed.

91 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This work uses MEG to provide evidence for the temporally-differentiated stages of the Full Decomposition model, and demonstrates an early effect of derivational family entropy, corresponding to the stem lookup stage, and a late effect of a novel statistical measure, semantic coherence, which quantifies the gradient semantic well-formedness of complex words.

68 citations


Cites background from "Evidence for early morphological de..."

  • ...Taking advantage of the temporal resolution of MEG, here we separately examine both the hypothesized earlier derivational family entropy effect on lexical access for a stem, as well as the hypothesized later surface frequency effect on recombination of stem and affix....

    [...]

  • ...Using MEG, Lehtonen, Monahan, and Poeppel (2011) found similar effects for regular derived words at a latency of !...

    [...]

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Priming effects across conditions revealed the existence of a significant linear trend, with transparent items showing the greatest effects and orthographic items the smallest, suggesting that these priming effects vary as a function of morphological structure and semantic transparency.
Abstract: The role of semantics in the segmentation of morphologically complex words was examined using event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded to target words primed by semantically transparent (hunter–hunt,) opaque (corner–corn), and orthographically related (scandal–scan) masked primes. Behavioral data showed that only transparent items gave rise to priming. The ERP data showed both N250 and the N400 effects with transparent items generating greater priming than orthographic or opaque. Furthermore, priming effects across conditions revealed the existence of a significant linear trend, with transparent items showing the greatest effects and orthographic items the smallest, suggesting that these priming effects vary as a function of morphological structure and semantic transparency. The results are discussed in terms of a model of morphological processing.

135 citations


"Evidence for early morphological de..." refers background or methods or result in this paper

  • ...In contrast to Morris et al. (2007), Lavric et al. (2007) obtained results more consistent with the earlier behavioral masked priming effects found by Rastle et al. (2004) and Longtin et al. (2003)....

    [...]

  • ...In Morris et al. (2007), target words were preceded by primes with a 50-msec duration, followed by a 20-msec backward mask....

    [...]

  • ...Solomyak and Marantz (2010) suggest that the reason Morris et al. (2007) found graded effects rather early in the ERP signal was not because of early influence from semantics but due to the fact that their stimuli were not matched for TP....

    [...]

  • ...This division could explain the differential effects observed in these two studies (Morris et al., 2007, 2008) as well as the early graded topography effect of Lavric et al. (2007)....

    [...]

  • ...…N250 is composed of distinct subcomponents, the earlier phase reflecting prelexical orthographic processing and the later phase reflecting prelexical phonological processing (see also Grainger, Kiyonaga, & Holcomb, 2006) or the earliest indications of semantic influence (Morris et al., 2007, 2008)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A purely structural morphemic segmentation procedure operating in the early stages of visual word perception is supported, whose magnitude was similar to that observed in pairs with genuine morphological relationship and greater than that in the nonmorphological pairs.
Abstract: There is broad consensus that the visual word recognition system is sensitive to morphological structure (e.g., “hunter” = “hunt” + “er”). Moreover, it has been assumed that the analysis of morphologically complex words (e.g., “hunter”) occurs only if the meaning of the complex form can be derived from the meanings of its constituents (e.g., “hunt” and “er”). However, recent behavioral work using masked priming has suggested that morphological analysis can occur at an early, orthographic level, with little influence from semantics. The present investigation examined the neurophysiological correlates of masked priming in conditions of a genuine morphological relationship (e.g., “hunter”-“HUNT”), an apparent morphological relationship (“corner”-“CORN”), and no morphological relationship (“brothel”-“BROTH”). Neural priming was indexed by the reduction of the N400 ERP component associated with targets preceded by related primes, as compared to targets preceded by unrelated primes. The mere appearance of morphological structure (“corner”-“CORN”) resulted in robust behavioral and neural priming, whose magnitude was similar to that observed in pairs with genuine morphological relationship and greater than that in the nonmorphological pairs. The results support a purely structural morphemic segmentation procedure operating in the early stages of visual word perception.

134 citations


"Evidence for early morphological de..." refers background or result in this paper

  • ...In contrast to Morris et al. (2007), Lavric et al. (2007) obtained results more consistent with the earlier behavioral masked priming effects found by Rastle et al. (2004) and Longtin et al. (2003)....

    [...]

  • ...Effects roughly consistent with the majority of the behavioral masked priming literature (and the current MEG study) were, however, found in Lavric et al. (2007) in the N400 component (at 380– 500 msec) and in the early phase of the N250 component (at 200–250 msec) in Morris et al. (2008)....

    [...]

  • ...Because of these contrasting ERP results—in particular, the differences in the early components between the findings in Morris et al. (2008) and Lavric et al. (2007)—the temporal nature of early decomposition and its electrophysiological correlates remains unresolved....

    [...]

  • ...This division could explain the differential effects observed in these two studies (Morris et al., 2007, 2008) as well as the early graded topography effect of Lavric et al. (2007)....

    [...]

  • ...Moreover, Lavric et al. (2007) and Morris et al. (2007) found priming effects in the N400 time-window....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Findings from three masked priming lexical decision experiments support the view that morphological decomposition operates at an early stage of visual word recognition.

130 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The data support a model of word recognition in which decomposition is attempted, and possibly utilized, for complex words containing bound roots as well as free word-stems, and find evidence of decomposition for both free stems and bound roots at the M170 stage in processing.
Abstract: We employ a single-trial correlational MEG analysis technique to investigate early processing in the visual recognition of morphologically complex words. Three classes of affixed words were presented in a lexical decision task: free stems (e.g., taxable), bound roots (e.g., tolerable), and unique root words (e.g., vulnerable, the root of which does not appear elsewhere). Analysis was focused on brain responses within 100-200 msec poststimulus onset in the previously identified letter string and visual word-form areas. MEG data were analyzed using cortically constrained minimum-norm estimation. Correlations were computed between activity at functionally defined ROIs and continuous measures of the words' morphological properties. ROIs were identified across subjects on a reference brain and then morphed back onto each individual subject's brain (n = 9). We find evidence of decomposition for both free stems and bound roots at the M170 stage in processing. The M170 response is shown to be sensitive to morphological properties such as affix frequency and the conditional probability of encountering each word given its stem. These morphological properties are contrasted with orthographic form features (letter string frequency, transition probability from one string to the next), which exert effects on earlier stages in processing (∼130 msec). We find that effects of decomposition at the M170 can, in fact, be attributed to morphological properties of complex words, rather than to purely orthographic and form-related properties. Our data support a model of word recognition in which decomposition is attempted, and possibly utilized, for complex words containing bound roots as well as free word-stems.

129 citations


"Evidence for early morphological de..." refers background or methods or result in this paper

  • ...…has been suggested to affect the ease or difficulty of decomposition (Hay, 2001), and recent MEG studies by Lewis, Solomyak, and Marantz (2011) and Solomyak and Marantz (2010) found that the left hemisphere M170 component, which may be related to the present peak, was sensitive to this factor in…...

    [...]

  • ...In line with Solomyak and Marantz (2010), the lemma TP was defined as the ratio of each wordʼs surface frequency to its cumulative stem frequency (lemma frequency)....

    [...]

  • ...Solomyak and Marantz (2010) suggest that the reason Morris et al. (2007) found graded effects rather early in the ERP signal was not because of early influence from semantics but due to the fact that their stimuli were not matched for TP....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Five representative examples of recent MEG studies in the laboratory that demonstrate the usefulness of whole-head magnetoencephalography in investigations of spatiotemporal dynamics of cortical signal processing are presented.
Abstract: Rapid progress in effective methods to image brain functions has revolutionized neuroscience. It is now possible to study noninvasively in humans neural processes that were previously only accessible in experimental animals and in brain-injured patients. In this endeavor, positron emission tomography has been the leader, but the superconducting quantum interference device-based magnetoencephalography (MEG) is gaining a firm role, too. With the advent of instruments covering the whole scalp, MEG, typically with 5-mm spatial and 1-ms temporal resolution, allows neuroscientists to track cortical functions accurately in time and space. We present five representative examples of recent MEG studies in our laboratory that demonstrate the usefulness of whole-head magnetoencephalography in investigations of spatiotemporal dynamics of cortical signal processing.

114 citations