scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Evidence for Racial Prejudice at the Implicit Level and Its Relationship With Questionnaire Measures

01 Feb 1997-Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (American Psychological Association)-Vol. 72, Iss: 2, pp 262-274
TL;DR: The magnitude of this implicit prejudice effect correlated reliably with participants' scores on explicit racial attitude measures, indicating that people's spontaneous stereotypic associations are consistent with their more controlled responses.
Abstract: The content of spontaneously activated racial stereotypes among White Americans and the relation of this to more explicit measures of stereotyping and prejudice were investigated. Using a semantic priming paradigm, a prime was presented outside of conscious awareness (BLACK or WHITE), followed by a target stimulus requiring a word-nonword decision. The target stimuli included attributes that varied in valence and stereotypicality for Whites and African Americans. Results showed reliable stereotyping and prejudice effects: Black primes resulted in substantially stronger facilitation to negative than positive stereotypic attributes, whereas White primes facilitated positive more than negative stereotypic traits. The magnitude of this implicit prejudice effect correlated reliably with participants' scores on explicit racial attitude measures, indicating that people's spontaneous stereotypic associations are consistent with their more controlled responses.
Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute when instructions oblige highly associated categories to share a response key, and performance is faster than when less associated categories share a key.
Abstract: An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute. The 2 concepts appear in a 2-choice task (e.g., flower vs. insect names), and the attribute in a 2nd task (e.g., pleasant vs. unpleasant words for an evaluation attribute). When instructions oblige highly associated categories (e.g., flower + pleasant) to share a response key, performance is faster than when less associated categories (e.g., insect + pleasant) share a key. This performance difference implicitly measures differential association of the 2 concepts with the attribute. In 3 experiments, the IAT was sensitive to (a) near-universal evaluative differences (e.g., flower vs. insect), (b) expected individual differences in evaluative associations (Japanese + pleasant vs. Korean + pleasant for Japanese vs. Korean subjects), and (c) consciously disavowed evaluative differences (Black + pleasant vs. White + pleasant for self-described unprejudiced White subjects).

9,731 citations

Book
01 Jan 1999
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors present a theory of intergroup relations from visiousness to viciousness, and the psychology of group dominance, as well as the dynamics of the criminal justice system.
Abstract: Part I. From There to Here - Theoretical Background: 1. From visiousness to viciousness: theories of intergroup relations 2. Social dominance theory as a new synthesis Part II. Oppression and its Psycho-Ideological Elements: 3. The psychology of group dominance: social dominance orientation 4. Let's both agree that you're really stupid: the power of consensual ideology Part III. The Circle of Oppression - The Myriad Expressions of Institutional Discrimination: 5. You stay in your part of town and I'll stay in mine: discrimination in the housing and retail markets 6. They're just too lazy to work: discrimination in the labor market 7. They're just mentally and physically unfit: discrimination in education and health care 8. The more of 'them' in prison, the better: institutional terror, social control and the dynamics of the criminal justice system Part IV. Oppression as a Cooperative Game: 9. Social hierarchy and asymmetrical group behavior: social hierarchy and group difference in behavior 10. Sex and power: the intersecting political psychologies of patriarchy and empty-set hierarchy 11. Epilogue.

3,970 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A review of 122 research reports (184 independent samples, 14,900 subjects) found average r =.274 for prediction of behavioral, judgment, and physiological measures by Implicit Association Test (IAT) measures as mentioned in this paper.
Abstract: This review of 122 research reports (184 independent samples, 14,900 subjects) found average r = .274 for prediction of behavioral, judgment, and physiological measures by Implicit Association Test (IAT) measures. Parallel explicit (i.e., self-report) measures, available in 156 of these samples (13,068 subjects), also predicted effectively (average r = .361), but with much greater variability of effect size. Predictive validity of self-report was impaired for socially sensitive topics, for which impression management may distort self-report responses. For 32 samples with criterion measures involving Black-White interracial behavior, predictive validity of IAT measures significantly exceeded that of self-report measures. Both IAT and self-report measures displayed incremental validity, with each measure predicting criterion variance beyond that predicted by the other. The more highly IAT and self-report measures were intercorrelated, the greater was the predictive validity of each.

2,690 citations

DOI
10 Oct 2018
TL;DR: The authors reviewed major meta-analyses of the attitude-behavior relation and found that general attitudes toward policies, people, institutions, and events correlate well with general behavioral patterns but not with specific behaviors.
Abstract: Work on general attitudes has drawn attention to the roles of attitude accessibility, controlled versus automatic information processing, and biases in information processing produced by automatically activated general attitudes towards objects. However, early failures to demonstrate strong attitude-behavior relations were shown to be attributable to incompatibility in the level of generality at which these variables were assessed. General attitudes toward policies, people, institutions, and events correlate well with general behavioral patterns but not with specific behaviors. Predicting specific actions requires a measure of attitude toward the behavior itself, as in the reasoned action approach, which takes specific behavior as its starting point and identifies intentions, attitudes, norms, and perceived behavioral control as important determinants. In addition to discussing those topics, this chapter reviews major meta-analyses of the attitude-behavior relation.

2,589 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This work reviews research that has utilized implicit measures across several domains, including attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes, and discusses their predictive validity, their interrelations, and the mechanisms presumably underlying their operation.
Abstract: Behavioral scientists have long sought measures of important psychological constructs that avoid response biases and other problems associated with direct reports. Recently, a large number of such indirect, or “implicit,” measures have emerged. We review research that has utilized these measures across several domains, including attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes, and discuss their predictive validity, their interrelations, and the mechanisms presumably underlying their operation. Special attention is devoted to various priming measures and the Implicit Association Test, largely due to their prevalence in the literature. We also attempt to clarify several unresolved theoretical and empirical issues concerning implicit measures, including the nature of the underlying constructs they purport to measure, the conditions under which they are most likely to relate to explicit measures, the kinds of behavior each measure is likely to predict, their sensitivity to context, and the construct's potential for c...

2,433 citations


Cites background from "Evidence for Racial Prejudice at th..."

  • ...5This distinction would not apply for priming measures that involve the subliminal presentation of the words “black” or “white” as primes (e.g., Wittenbrink et al. 1997). candy bars (a preference that was not apparent on an explicit measure of liking), these researchers noted: “In our society,…...

    [...]

  • ...…2001, Rudman & Glick 2001, Devine et al. 2002, Dovidio et al. 2002), although there are occasional reports of significant correlations (e.g., McConnell & Liebold 2001, with respect to the IAT; Lepore & Brown 1997; Kawakami et al. 1998; Wittenbrink et al. 1997, with respect to a priming measure)....

    [...]

  • ...The research has involved a variety of domains, including attitudes (e.g., Fazio et al. 1995, Greenwald et al. 1998), stereotypes (e.g., Wittenbrink et al. 1997, Nosek et al. 2002a), self-esteem (Hetts et al. 1999, Bosson et al. 2000, Koole et al. 2001, Rudman et al. 2001b), close relationships…...

    [...]

References
More filters
Book
01 Jan 1935
TL;DR: In this paper, Neuberg and Heine discuss the notion of belonging, acceptance, belonging, and belonging in the social world, and discuss the relationship between friendship, membership, status, power, and subordination.
Abstract: VOLUME 2. Part III: The Social World. 21. EVOLUTIONARY SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY (Steven L. Neuberg, Douglas T. Kenrick, and Mark Schaller). 22. MORALITY (Jonathan Haidt and Selin Kesebir). 23. AGGRESSION (Brad J. Bushman and L. Rowell Huesmann). 24. AFFILIATION, ACCEPTANCE, AND BELONGING: THE PURSUIT OF INTERPERSONAL CONNECTION (Mark R. Leary). 25. CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS (Margaret S. Clark and Edward P. Lemay, Jr.). 26. INTERPERSONAL STRATIFICATION: STATUS, POWER, AND SUBORDINATION (Susan T. Fiske). 27. SOCIAL CONFLICT: THE EMERGENCE AND CONSEQUENCES OF STRUGGLE AND NEGOTIATION (Carsten K. W. De Dreu). 28. INTERGROUP RELATIONS 1(Vincent Yzerbyt and Stephanie Demoulin). 29. INTERGROUP BIAS (John F. Dovidio and Samuel L. Gaertner). 30. SOCIAL JUSTICE: HISTORY, THEORY, AND RESEARCH (John T. Jost and Aaron C. Kay). 31. INFLUENCE AND LEADERSHIP (Michael A. Hogg). 32. GROUP BEHAVIOR AND PERFORMANCE (J. Richard Hackman and Nancy Katz). 33. ORGANIZATIONAL PREFERENCES AND THEIR CONSEQUENCES (Deborah H. Gruenfeld and Larissa Z. Tiedens). 34. THE PSYCHOLOGICAL UNDERPINNINGS OF POLITICAL BEHAVIOR (Jon A. Krosnick, Penny S. Visser, and Joshua Harder). 35. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LAW (Margaret Bull Kovera and Eugene Borgida). 36. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LANGUAGE: WORDS, UTTERANCES, AND CONVERSATIONS (Thomas Holtgraves). 37. CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY (Steven J. Heine). AUTHOR INDEX. SUBJECT INDEX.

13,453 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The only truly comprehensive advanced level textbook designed for courses in the pscyhology of attitudes and related studies in attitude measurement, social cognition is as mentioned in this paper, which contains a comprehensive coverage of classic and modern research and theory.
Abstract: This is the only truly comprehensive advanced level textbook in the past 20 years designed for courses in the pscyhology of attitudes and related studies in attitude measurement, social cognition. Written by two of the most distinguished scholars in the field, its comprehensive coverage of classic and modern research and theory is unsurpassed.

7,753 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors (1977) in a series of experiments and demonstrated the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search.
Abstract: Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors (1977) in a series of experiments. The studies (a) demonstrate the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search; (b) trace the course of the

7,032 citations


Additional excerpts

  • ...Jacoby, Lindsay, & Toth, 1992; Posner, 1978; Roediger, 1990; Schacter, 1987; Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977; Uleman & Bargh, 1989)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, a theoretical model based on the dissociation ofantomatic and controlled processes involved in prejudice was proposed, which suggests that the stereotype is automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of the stereotyped group and that Iow-prejudiee responses require controlled inhibition of the automatically activated stereotype.
Abstract: University of Wisconsin--Madis on Three studies tested basic assumptions derived from a theoretical model based on the dissociation ofantomatic and controlled processes involved in prejudice. Study I supported the model's assumption that high- and low-prejudice persons are equally knowledgeable of the cultural stereotype. The model suggests that the stereotype is automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of the stereotyped group and that Iow-prejudiee responses require controlled inhibition of the automatically activated stereotype. Study 2, which examined the effects of automarie stereotype activation on the evaluation of ambiguous stereotype-relevant behaviors performed by a race-unspecified person, suggested that when subjects' ability to consciously monitor stereotype activation is precluded, both high- and low-prejudice subjects produce stereotype-congruent evaluations of ambiguous behaviors. Study 3 examined high- and low-prejudice subjects' responses in a consciously directed thought-listing task. Consistent with the model, only low-prejudice subjects inhibited the automatically activated stereotype-congruent thoughts and replaced them with thoughts reflecting equality and negations of the stereotype. The relation between stereotypes and prejudice and implications for prejudice reduction are discussed.

5,300 citations


"Evidence for Racial Prejudice at th..." refers background in this paper

  • ...More recently, a rather different explanation has been suggested, according to which a person may, at the same time, hold positive attitudes toward a social group and nevertheless be influenced by negative group stereotypes (Devine, 1989)....

    [...]

  • ...To date, most of the work concerned with this distinction has focused on demonstrating that stereotypic knowledge in general, and racial stereotypes in particular, may in fact be activated effortlessly and influence subsequent judgments unbeknownst to the perceiver (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Devine, 1989; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin, 1983; D. T. Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae, Stangor, & Milne, 1994; Perdue & Gurtman, 1990). Much less work exists that directly assesses the relationship between implicit and explicit measures of stereotyping and prejudice. Perhaps the best known study that has examined this relationship is Devine's (1989) work, in which she argued that all White Americans know of and automatically activate the culturally shared negative stereotype of African Americans. People low in prejudice must then invoke their own personal (more positive) beliefs regarding African Americans by means of a controlled process. The implications of this argument are that there are no individual differences in the associations that are automatically activated by the category Blacks and, therefore, that the degree of prejudice evidenced on an implicit, or automatic, task will be uncorrelated with the degree of prejudice exhibited on an explicit, or controlled task. To test this "dissociation" argument, Devine first showed that participants high and low in prejudice (determined by scores on the Modern Racism Scale [MRS]; McConahay, Hardee, & Batts, 1981 ) differed in the relative numbers of positive to negative qualities they listed as characteristic of African Americans. That is, the MRS predicted differences in explicitly generated lists of valenced attributes. Next, in Devine's (1989) study, participants were subliminally exposed to words associated with the target group, African Americans, such as busing, oppression, slavery, jazz, and basketball Some participants saw a large number of these words, and some only a few. Then participants read about and interpreted the behavior of an ambiguously hostile individual, whose ethnicity was not specified. If a large number of African American-related words had been seen in the subliminal priming task, the ambiguous target was judged to be more hostile than if a small number of such words were seen. Importantly, the extent to which this was true did not depend on the participants' scores on the MRS. Thus, Devine (1989) concluded that implicit stereotyping had occurred and that it had occurred to the same degree for all participants, regardless of their level of explicit prejudice. We have two primary concerns with this study. First, although in the subliminal task, Devine (1989) was careful to avoid words that were direct semantic associates of hostile, many of the words presented were at least indirectly related to the concept "hostility." Busing, oppression, nigger, ghetto, slavery, and prejudice are certainly linked in memory to knowledge of hostility. To the extent that this task activated the general concept of hostility (not as it relates specifically to the stereotypic qualities of African Americans, but simply as a general concept, primed by, for example, busing and oppression), then it is reasonable to expect both that the ambiguous target would be seen as hostile and that this would not depend on MRS scores. Hostility had been primed for all participants (as a general construct), thereby masking any individual differences in the strength of association of hostility to the group African Americans. A second problem is that the ethnicity of the ambiguous target was left unspecified, and thus, the results are arguably a demonstration of implicit stereotyping, given that the target was not specifically identified as a member of the group (and the default assumption would presumably be that the target was White). Thus, a fair evaluation of the dissociation hypothesis proposed by Devine (1989) would require a task that more directly measures the degree of association of stereotypic attributes with the category African Americans in an implicit manner and that tests whether this is related to explicitly measured stereotypes. In fact, not only would such a task be useful to examine whether controlled responses are indeed uncorrelated with what is activated at the automatic level, but it would at the same time allow us to explore the issue of strategic manipulation of the responses in the Judd et al. (1995) studies. A different procedure to get at spontaneous activation of stereotypic knowledge has been developed by Dovidio, Gaertner, and their colleagues (Dovidio, Evans, & Tyler, 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin, 1983). This task is a variation on Meyer and Schvaneveldt's (1971 ) classic procedure demonstrating semantic priming effects. In the Meyer and Schvaneveldt procedure, participants first see a single word, the prime (e.g., BREAD) and then are presented with a letter string, the target (e.g., BLrrrEa), to which they have to respond with, for example, a wordnonword judgment. Response latencies are facilitated by semantic associations between the prime and the target stimulus. A common explanation for this by now well established finding (for a review, see Neely, 1991 ) derives from the concept of spreading activation. Presumably, activation of the prime spreads to semantically related concepts and thus reduces the time required for the activation of related targets to reach recognition threshold (cf. Neely, 1977; Posner & Snyder, 1975). Dovidio et al. (1986) used similar reasoning when asking their participants about what attributes they associated with White Americans and African Americans....

    [...]

  • ...To date, most of the work concerned with this distinction has focused on demonstrating that stereotypic knowledge in general, and racial stereotypes in particular, may in fact be activated effortlessly and influence subsequent judgments unbeknownst to the perceiver (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Devine, 1989; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin, 1983; D. T. Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae, Stangor, & Milne, 1994; Perdue & Gurtman, 1990)....

    [...]

  • ...To date, most of the work concerned with this distinction has focused on demonstrating that stereotypic knowledge in general, and racial stereotypes in particular, may in fact be activated effortlessly and influence subsequent judgments unbeknownst to the perceiver (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Devine, 1989; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin, 1983; D. T. Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae, Stangor, & Milne, 1994; Perdue & Gurtman, 1990). Much less work exists that directly assesses the relationship between implicit and explicit measures of stereotyping and prejudice. Perhaps the best known study that has examined this relationship is Devine's (1989) work, in which she argued that all White Americans know of and automatically activate the culturally shared negative stereotype of African Americans. People low in prejudice must then invoke their own personal (more positive) beliefs regarding African Americans by means of a controlled process. The implications of this argument are that there are no individual differences in the associations that are automatically activated by the category Blacks and, therefore, that the degree of prejudice evidenced on an implicit, or automatic, task will be uncorrelated with the degree of prejudice exhibited on an explicit, or controlled task. To test this "dissociation" argument, Devine first showed that participants high and low in prejudice (determined by scores on the Modern Racism Scale [MRS]; McConahay, Hardee, & Batts, 1981 ) differed in the relative numbers of positive to negative qualities they listed as characteristic of African Americans. That is, the MRS predicted differences in explicitly generated lists of valenced attributes. Next, in Devine's (1989) study, participants were subliminally exposed to words associated with the target group, African Americans, such as busing, oppression, slavery, jazz, and basketball Some participants saw a large number of these words, and some only a few. Then participants read about and interpreted the behavior of an ambiguously hostile individual, whose ethnicity was not specified. If a large number of African American-related words had been seen in the subliminal priming task, the ambiguous target was judged to be more hostile than if a small number of such words were seen. Importantly, the extent to which this was true did not depend on the participants' scores on the MRS. Thus, Devine (1989) concluded that implicit stereotyping had occurred and that it had occurred to the same degree for all participants, regardless of their level of explicit prejudice. We have two primary concerns with this study. First, although in the subliminal task, Devine (1989) was careful to avoid words that were direct semantic associates of hostile, many of the words presented were at least indirectly related to the concept "hostility." Busing, oppression, nigger, ghetto, slavery, and prejudice are certainly linked in memory to knowledge of hostility. To the extent that this task activated the general concept of hostility (not as it relates specifically to the stereotypic qualities of African Americans, but simply as a general concept, primed by, for example, busing and oppression), then it is reasonable to expect both that the ambiguous target would be seen as hostile and that this would not depend on MRS scores. Hostility had been primed for all participants (as a general construct), thereby masking any individual differences in the strength of association of hostility to the group African Americans. A second problem is that the ethnicity of the ambiguous target was left unspecified, and thus, the results are arguably a demonstration of implicit stereotyping, given that the target was not specifically identified as a member of the group (and the default assumption would presumably be that the target was White). Thus, a fair evaluation of the dissociation hypothesis proposed by Devine (1989) would require a task that more directly measures the degree of association of stereotypic attributes with the category African Americans in an implicit manner and that tests whether this is related to explicitly measured stereotypes. In fact, not only would such a task be useful to examine whether controlled responses are indeed uncorrelated with what is activated at the automatic level, but it would at the same time allow us to explore the issue of strategic manipulation of the responses in the Judd et al. (1995) studies....

    [...]

  • ...To date, most of the work concerned with this distinction has focused on demonstrating that stereotypic knowledge in general, and racial stereotypes in particular, may in fact be activated effortlessly and influence subsequent judgments unbeknownst to the perceiver (Banaji & Greenwald, 1995; Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Devine, 1989; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin, 1983; D. T. Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae, Stangor, & Milne, 1994; Perdue & Gurtman, 1990). Much less work exists that directly assesses the relationship between implicit and explicit measures of stereotyping and prejudice. Perhaps the best known study that has examined this relationship is Devine's (1989) work, in which she argued that all White Americans know of and automatically activate the culturally shared negative stereotype of African Americans. People low in prejudice must then invoke their own personal (more positive) beliefs regarding African Americans by means of a controlled process. The implications of this argument are that there are no individual differences in the associations that are automatically activated by the category Blacks and, therefore, that the degree of prejudice evidenced on an implicit, or automatic, task will be uncorrelated with the degree of prejudice exhibited on an explicit, or controlled task. To test this "dissociation" argument, Devine first showed that participants high and low in prejudice (determined by scores on the Modern Racism Scale [MRS]; McConahay, Hardee, & Batts, 1981 ) differed in the relative numbers of positive to negative qualities they listed as characteristic of African Americans. That is, the MRS predicted differences in explicitly generated lists of valenced attributes. Next, in Devine's (1989) study, participants were subliminally exposed to words associated with the target group, African Americans, such as busing, oppression, slavery, jazz, and basketball Some participants saw a large number of these words, and some only a few. Then participants read about and interpreted the behavior of an ambiguously hostile individual, whose ethnicity was not specified. If a large number of African American-related words had been seen in the subliminal priming task, the ambiguous target was judged to be more hostile than if a small number of such words were seen. Importantly, the extent to which this was true did not depend on the participants' scores on the MRS. Thus, Devine (1989) concluded that implicit stereotyping had occurred and that it had occurred to the same degree for all participants, regardless of their level of explicit prejudice. We have two primary concerns with this study. First, although in the subliminal task, Devine (1989) was careful to avoid words that were direct semantic associates of hostile, many of the words presented were at least indirectly related to the concept "hostility." Busing, oppression, nigger, ghetto, slavery, and prejudice are certainly linked in memory to knowledge of hostility. To the extent that this task activated the general concept of hostility (not as it relates specifically to the stereotypic qualities of African Americans, but simply as a general concept, primed by, for example, busing and oppression), then it is reasonable to expect both that the ambiguous target would be seen as hostile and that this would not depend on MRS scores. Hostility had been primed for all participants (as a general construct), thereby masking any individual differences in the strength of association of hostility to the group African Americans. A second problem is that the ethnicity of the ambiguous target was left unspecified, and thus, the results are arguably a demonstration of implicit stereotyping, given that the target was not specifically identified as a member of the group (and the default assumption would presumably be that the target was White). Thus, a fair evaluation of the dissociation hypothesis proposed by Devine (1989) would require a task that more directly measures the degree of association of stereotypic attributes with the category African Americans in an implicit manner and that tests whether this is related to explicitly measured stereotypes....

    [...]