TL;DR: The hypothesis that the cerebellum can be conceptualized as a relatively task-independent timing mechanism that is capable of representing temporal information ranging from a few milliseconds to an upper bound of a few seconds is reviewed.
Abstract: The ability of an animal to process temporal information has adaptive significance across different temporal ranges. The ability to encode and utilize temporal information allows an animal to predict and anticipate events. However, the time scales vary widely. The predictable event might be based on information that changes over relatively long periods such as a year or a day, or over periods comprising much shorter durations, events that change within a few minutes or milliseconds. Are there a single set of neural mechanisms that are essential for representing temporal information over these different scales? Despite the fact that numerous neural structures have been linked to successful performance on a variety of timing tasks, this question has received relatively little attention. In this chapter, we will focus on the role of the cerebellum in a variety of timing tasks. We will review the hypothesis that the cerebellum can be conceptualized as a relatively task-independent timing mechanism. An important feature of this hypothesis is that the range of the cerebellar timing system is assumed to be relatively restricted. Specifically, we assume that the cerebellum is capable of representing temporal information ranging from a few milliseconds to an upper bound of a few seconds. What remains unclear is whether the cerebellum is involved on tasks spanning longer durations. Cognitive processes such as attention and memory become clearly important here, and indeed, may dominate performance for longer intervals. The animal literature points to non-cerebellar structures as playing a critical role in these tasks and we will provide a brief review of this work. Finally, we will present the preliminary results from two experiments designed to directly test the hypothesis that the cerebellum's temporal capabilities are limited to relatively short durations.
These results required a reconccptualization of the domain of cerebellar function.
This structure has generally been linked to motor functions, or sensorimotor learning.
The fact that the patients were impaired on a purely perceptual task suggested that its domain should be specified in terms of a particular mental operation, namely the representation of the temporal relationships between events.
The authors have hypothesized that this computational capability is invoked across a wide range of tasks that require this form of representation.
2. The cerebellum and sensorimotor learning
It remains difficult to specif3.' the learning domain of the cerebellum (see Ivry, 1993) .
This hypothesis emphasizes the task domain of the cerebellum and focuses on the fact that the climbing fiber pathway provides a salient error signal for shaping appropriate skeletal responses.
By this way of thinking, the cerebellum is associated with NMR conditioning because this type of learning is only adaptive if it is appropriately timed (Kecle and lvr3'.
That is, learning an association and forming the temporal representation of that association can not be thought of as distinct.
Due to negative feedback loops and physiological processes such as slow IPSPs, other consequences of the mossy fiber activity may not be evident for hundreds of milliseconds.
3. Potential limitations of cerebellar timing
Second, behavioral studies in humans suggest that there may be a qualitative change around 2-4 scc in their capacity to represent temporal infommtion.
Below this duration, successive events are seen as belonging to a conunon temporally-defined group, regardless of whether this group has a rh}ethmic structure or lacks such organization.
Ps.vchophysical studies have also indicated an increase in the Weber fraction on duration discrimination tasks for intervals longer than 2 sec (Getty.
On the motor side, Mates et al. (1994) have shox~aa that when tapping with a periodic pacing signal, people shift from a predictive to an reactive mode as the target interval becomes longer.
For intervals less than 2-3 sec. the subjects' responses tended to anticipate the tones.
4. Animal models of temporal discrimination
To date, this animal research has ignored the cerebellum, perhaps because this structure has been assumed to be limited to the motor domain while these tasks focus on perceptual and memor), processes.
Moreover, while some studies have used stimuli that are less than 1 sec (Allan and Gibbon, 1991) , the maiority of this work has involved stimuli that are considerably longer, frequently ranging tip to 40 sec.
As noted above, the authors have hypothesized that the ccrcbcllar timing system is limited to relatively short durations.
The authors working model is that this timing process is relatively immune to cognitive influences.
The onset of a stimulus may automatically activate different sets of neurons, and mcmor), demands are minimal.
4.1. EXPERIMENT I
To obtain psychometric fi~nctions, the test phase included both the training durations and probe durations.
For the SR task, there were nine probe durations ranging from 200 to 850 ms, with seven of these durations falling between the endpoint values.
On 50% of the trials, one of the two training durations was selected and correct responses were reinforced.
On the other 50% of the trials, one of the nine probe durations was selected and no reward was possible.
The rats completed 36 sessions of the test phase prior to surgery.
4.1.2. Results And l)iscnssion.
The effect of task and the interaction were not significant.
In contrast, the consistency measure revealed a dissociation between the two tasks.
This result is in accord with the hypothesis that the temporal range of the cerebellum is limited to short intervals.
For the first two postsurgery sessions on the SR task, the consistency scores were -5.25 and -4.88.
Second, their animals received many more trials post-surger).,' than are commonly used in lcsion studies.
4.2. EXPERIMENT 2
Third, all sessions were four hours in duration and alternated by session between the two tasks.
Wc did not include any mixed sessions since an auditory stimulus was used for both tasks.
On each day, one group was tested on the SR task and the other group was tested on the ID task.
All animals received extensive training on the two tasks prior to surgeD'.
After a one-week recover3.' period, they were tested for an additional 26 sessions post-surgery.
4.2.2. Results And Discttssion.
Lesions (Ivry and Keele, 1989) . and included tests of both time production and time perception.
Rather than focus on particular tasks (e.g., reflex conditioning, motor control, perceptual processing), it can be useful to consider the operation performed by a structure and then explore how widely that operation is employed.
In the case of the cerebellum, it appears that its timing capability can help account for why this structure is essential for coordinated movement, speech production, sensorimotor learning, as well as certain perceptual functions (see Ivry, 1993) .
A timing system that is used for coordinating the actions of multiple joints or perceiving the velocity of a moving stimulus is unlikely to be useful for tasks that span many seconds or minutes.
Researchers utilizing tasks such as the peak procedure have suggested vet3' different evolutionary pressures for why rats might benefit from being able to determine when 40 see has elapsed.
TL;DR: The neurobiological properties of the basal ganglia, an area known to be necessary for interval timing and motor control, suggests that this set of structures act as a coincidence detector of cortical and thalamic input.
Abstract: Humans and other animals demonstrate the ability to perceive and respond to temporally relevant information with characteristic behavioral properties. For example, the response time distributions in peak-interval timing tasks are well described by Gaussian functions, and superimpose when scaled by the criterion duration. This superimposition has been referred to as the scalar property and results from the fact that the standard deviation of a temporal estimate is proportional to the duration being timed. Various psychological models have been proposed to account for such responding. These models vary in their success in predicting the temporal control of behavior as well as in the neurobiological feasibility of the mechanisms they postulate. A review of the major interval timing models reveals that no current model is successful on both counts. The neurobiological properties of the basal ganglia, an area known to be necessary for interval timing and motor control, suggests that this set of structures act as a coincidence detector of cortical and thalamic input. The hypothesized functioning of the basal ganglia is similar to the mechanisms proposed in the beat frequency timing model [R.C. Miall, Neural Computation 1 (1989) 359-371], leading to a reevaluation of its capabilities in terms of behavioral prediction. By implementing a probabilistic firing rule, a dynamic response threshold, and adding variance to a number of its components, simulations of the striatal beat frequency model were able to produce output that is functionally equivalent to the expected behavioral response form of peak-interval timing procedures.
791 citations
Cites background from "Exploring the domain of the cerebel..."
...[43]), as well as timing in the milliseconds range [39], thereby suggesting the existence of a unitary psychological and neurobiological interval timing mechanism....
TL;DR: It is argued that careful analysis of this literature provides evidence for separate neural timing systems associated with opposing task characteristics, the 'automatic' system draws mainly upon motor circuits and the 'cognitively controlled' system depends upon prefrontal and parietal regions.
Abstract: A recent review of neuroimaging data on time measurement argued that the brain activity seen in association with timing is not influenced by specific characteristics of the task performed. In contrast, we argue that careful analysis of this literature provides evidence for separate neural timing systems associated with opposing task characteristics. The ‘automatic’ system draws mainly upon motor circuits and the ‘cognitively controlled’ system depends upon prefrontal and parietal regions.
779 citations
Cites background from "Exploring the domain of the cerebel..."
...The cerebellum could also be involved [16;17], and shows particularly appropriate circuitry for the measurement of brief intervals [18-21]....
TL;DR: It is proposed that cerebellar dysfunction may induce deregulation of tonic thalamic tuning, which disrupts gating of the mnemonic temporal information generated in the basal ganglia through striato-thalamo-cortical loops.
Abstract: A rich tradition of normative psychophysics has identified two ubiquitous properties of interval timing: the scalar property, a strong form of Weber's law, and ratio comparison mechanisms. Finding the neural substrate of these properties is a major challenge for neurobiology. Recently, advances have been made in our understanding of the brain structures important for timing, especially the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. Surgical intervention or diseases of the cerebellum generally result in increased variability in temporal processing, whereas both clock and memory effects are seen for neurotransmitter interventions, lesions and diseases of the basal ganglia. We propose that cerebellar dysfunction may induce deregulation of tonic thalamic tuning, which disrupts gating of the mnemonic temporal information generated in the basal ganglia through striato-thalamo-cortical loops.
TL;DR: Two subcortical structures, the cerebellum and basal ganglia, play a critical role in the timing of both movement and perception and are examined from both a neurological and a computational perspective.
Abstract: The representation of temporal information can be examined from both a neurological and a computational perspective. Recent evidence suggests that two subcortical structures, the cerebellum and basal ganglia, play a critical role in the timing of both movement and perception. At a computational level, models of an internal clock have been developed in which timing is based on either endogenous oscillatory processes or distributed interval-based representations derived from relatively slow physiological processes.
TL;DR: FMRI is used to isolate differences between the brain networks which measure 0.6 and 3s in a temporal discrimination task with visual discrimination for control, suggesting that distinct components are used for the two durations.
Abstract: The possibility that different neural systems are used to measure temporal durations at the sub-second and several second ranges has been supported by pharmacological manipulation, psychophysics, and neural network modelling. Here, we add to this literature by using fMRI to isolate differences between the brain networks which measure 0.6 and 3 s in a temporal discrimination task with visual discrimination for control. We observe activity in bilateral insula and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and in right hemispheric pre-supplementary motor area, frontal pole, and inferior parietal cortex during measurement of both intervals, suggesting that these regions constitute a system used in temporal discrimination at both ranges. The frontal operculum, left cerebellar hemisphere and middle and superior temporal gyri, all show significantly greater activity during measurement of the shorter interval, supporting the hypotheses that the motor system is preferentially involved in the measurement of sub-second intervals, and that auditory imagery is preferentially used during measurement of the same. Only a few voxels, falling in the left posterior cingulate and inferior parietal lobe, are more active in the 3 s condition. Overall, this study shows that although many brain regions are used for the measurement of both sub- and supra-second temporal durations, there are also differences in activation patterns, suggesting that distinct components are used for the two durations.
418 citations
Cites background from "Exploring the domain of the cerebel..."
...distraction of attention in dual task scenarios ( Rammsayer & Lima, 1991) can have differential influence (but see Macar, Grondin, & Casini, 1994), while lesions to specific brain areas elicit differential impairments ( Clarke, Ivry, Grinband, Roberts, & Shimizu, 1996 )....
TL;DR: This paper presents a meta-analyses of the determinants of earthquake-triggered landsliding in the Czech Republic over a period of 18 months in order to establish a probabilistic framework for estimating the intensity of the earthquake.
Abstract: Preface. Acknowledgements. Introduction. References. List of Structures. Index of Abbreviations. Diagrams.
TL;DR: The results suggest that the domain of the cerebellar timing process is not limited to the motor system, but is employed by other perceptual and cognitive systems when temporally predictive computations are needed.
Abstract: This study investigated the effects of different types of neurological deficits on timing functions. The performance of Parkinson, cerebellar, cortical, and peripheral neuropathy patients was compared to age-matched control subjects on two separate measures of timing functions. The first task involved the production of timed intervals in which the subjects attempted to maintain a simple rhythm. The second task measured the subjects' perceptual ability to discriminate between small differences in the duration of two intervals.
The primacy of the cerebellum in timing functions was demonstrated by the finding that these were the only patients who showed a deficit in both the production and perception of timing tasks. The cerebellar group was found to have increased variability in performing rhythmic tapping and they were less accurate than the other groups in making perceptual discriminations regarding small differences in duration. Critically, this perceptual deficit appears to be specific to the perception of time since the cerebellar patients were unaffected in a control task measuring the perception of loudness.
It is argued that the operation of a timing mechanism can be conceptualized as an isolable component of the motor control system. Furthermore, the results suggest that the domain of the cerebellar timing process is not limited to the motor system, but is employed by other perceptual and cognitive systems when temporally predictive computations are needed.
TL;DR: Probably applications of this new understanding of the neural bases of learning and memory range from education to the treatment of learning disabilities to the design of new artificial intelligence systems.
Abstract: Study of the neurobiology of learning and memory is in a most exciting phase. Behavioral studies in animals are characterizing the categories and properties of learning and memory; essential memory trace circuits in the brain are being defined and localized in mammalian models; work on human memory and the brain is identifying neuronal systems involved in memory; the neuronal, neurochemical, molecular, and biophysical substrates of memory are beginning to be understood in both invertebrate and vertebrate systems; and theoretical and mathematical analysis of basic associative learning and of neuronal networks in proceeding apace. Likely applications of this new understanding of the neural bases of learning and memory range from education to the treatment of learning disabilities to the design of new artificial intelligence systems.