scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Framing: Toward Clarification of a Fractured Paradigm

01 Dec 1993-Journal of Communication (Blackwell Publishing Ltd)-Vol. 43, Iss: 4, pp 51-58
TL;DR: Reaching this goal would require a more self-con- scious determination by communication scholars to plumb other fields and feed back their studies to outside researchers, and enhance the theoretical rigor of communication scholarship proper.
Abstract: deficient core knowledge, I propose that we turn an osten- sible weakness into a strength. We should identify our mission as bring- ing together insights and theories that would otherwise remain scattered in other disciplines. Because of the lack of interchange among the disci- plines, hypotheses thoroughly discredited in one field may receive wide acceptance in another. Potential research paradigms remain fractured, with pieces here and there but no comprehensive statement to guide re- search. By bringing ideas together in one location, communication can aspire to become a master discipline that synthesizes related theories and concepts and exposes them to the most rigorous, comprehensive state- ment and exploration. Reaching this goal would require a more self-con- scious determination by communication scholars to plumb other fields and feed back their studies to outside researchers. At the same time, such an enterprise would enhance the theoretical rigor of communication scholarship proper. The idea

Summary (2 min read)

Of Frames and Framing

  • To frame is to select some aspects of aperceived reality and make them more salient in a communicating text, in such a way as to promote aparticularproblem definition, causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recommendation for the item described.
  • A single sentence may perform more than one of these four framing functions, although many sentences in a text may perform none of them.
  • The cold war example also suggests that frames have at least four locations in the communication process: the communicator, the text, the receiver, and the culture.
  • The culture is the stock of commonly invoked frames; in fact, culture might be defined as the empirically demonstrable set of common frames exhibited in the discourse and thinking of most people in a social grouping.

How Frames Work

  • Frames highlight some bits of information about an item that is the subject of a communication, thereby elevating them in salience.
  • An increase in salience enhances the probability that receivers will perceive the information, discern meaning and thus process it, and store it in meniory (see Fiske & Taylor, 1991) .
  • Kahneman and Tversky (1984) offer perhaps the most widely cited recent example o f the power of framing and the way it operates by selecting and highlighting some features of reality while omitting others.
  • If Program D is adopted, there is a one-third probability that nobody will die and a two-thirds probability that 600 people will die" (Kahneman & Tversky, 1984, p. 343).
  • Often a potential counterframing of the subject is mostly or wholly absent from a text, although, to use this instance, an audience member with a strong civil liberties philosophy might reject mandatory testing even if the poll framed AIDS strictly in public health terms.

Frames in Political News

  • This portrait of framing has important implications for political communication.
  • Frames call attention to some aspects of reality while obscuring other elements, which might lead audiences to have different reactions.
  • Many news texts exhibit homogeneous framing at one level of analysis, yet competing frames at another.
  • The news frame included only two remedies, war now or sanctions now with war later, while problem definitions, causal analyses, and moral evaluations were homogeneous.
  • Unpublicized, the views could gain few adherents and generate little perceived or actual effect on public opinion, which meant elites felt no pressure to expand the frame so it included other treatments for Iraqi aggression, such as negotiation.

Benefits of a Consistent Concept of Framing

  • An understanding of frames helps illuminate many empirical and normative controversies, most importantly because the concept of framing di-rects their attention to the details of just how a communicated text exerts its power.
  • The example o f mass communication explored here suggests how a common understanding might help constitute framing as a research paradigm.
  • Journalists may follow the rules for "objective" reporting and yet convey a dominant framing of the news text that prevents most audience members from making a balanced assessment of a situation.
  • The major task of determining textual meaning should be to identify and describe frames; content analysis informed by a theory of framing would avoid treating all negative or positive terms or utterances as equally salient and influential.
  • Public opinion and normative democratic theory.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Framing: Toward Clarification
of
a
Fractured Paradigm
by Robert
M.
Entman, Northwestern University
In response
to
the proposition that communication lacks disciplinary sta-
tus because
of
deficient core knowledge,
I
propose that we turn an osten-
sible weakness into a strength. We should identify our mission as bring-
ing together insights and theories that would otherwise remain scattered
in other disciplines. Because
of
the lack
of
interchange among the disci-
plines, hypotheses thoroughly discredited in one field may receive wide
acceptance in another. Potential research paradigms remain fractured,
with pieces here and there but no comprehensive statement
to
guide re-
search. By bringing ideas together in one location, communication can
aspire
to
become a master discipline that synthesizes related theories and
concepts and exposes them to the most rigorous, comprehensive state-
ment and exploration. Reaching this goal would require a more self-con-
scious determination by communication scholars
to
plumb other fields
and feed back their studies
to
outside researchers.
At
the same time, such
an enterprise would enhance the theoretical rigor
of
communication
scholarship proper.
The idea
of
“framing” offers a case study
of
just the kind
of
scattered
conceptualization
I
have identified. Despite its omnipresence across the
social sciences and humanities, nowhere is there a general statement
of
framing theory that shows exactly how frames become embedded within
and make themselves manifest in a text,
or
how framing influences think-
ing. Analysis
of
this concept suggests how the discipline
of
communica-
tion might contribute something unique: synthesizing a key concept’s dis-
parate uses, showing how they invariably involve communication, and
constructing a coherent theory from them.
Whatever its specific
use,
the concept
of
framing consistently offers a
way
to
describe the power
of
a communicating text. Analysis
of
frames
il-
luminates the precise way in which influence over a human conscious-
ness is exerted by the transfer (or communication)
of
information from
Robert
M.
Entman
is
an associate professor
of
communication studies, journalism, and
po-
litical science and chair
of
the program in Communications, Media, and Public Policy at the
Center for Urban Affairs and Policy Research at Northwestern University, Evanston,
IL.
He
gratefully acknowledges the comments
of
students in his “Mass Communication and Demo-
cratic Theory” seminar, especially Andrew Rojecki.
Copyright
0
1993
Journal
of
Communication
43(4),
Autumn.
0021-9916/93/$5.00

Journal
of
Communzcatzon,
Antumn
199.3
one location-such as a speech, utterance, news report, or novel-to that
consciousness.
(A
representative list
of
classic and recent citations would
include: Edelman, 1993; Entman
&
Rojecki, 1993; Fiske
&
Taylor, 1991;
Gamson, 1992; Goffman, 1974; Graber,
1988;
Iyengar,
1991;
Kahneman
&
Tversky, 1984; Pan
&
Kosicki, 1993; Riker,
1986;
Snow
&
Benford,
1988;
Tuchman, 1978; White, 1987; Zaller,
1992.)
A
literature review suggests
that framing is often defined casually, with much left
to
an assumed tacit
understanding
of
reader and researcher. After all, the words
frame, fram-
ing,
and
.framework
are common outside of formal scholarly discourse,
and their connotation there is roughly the same. The goal here is
to
iden-
tify and make explicit common tendencies among the various uses
of
the
terms and
to
suggest a more precise and universal understanding
of
them.
Of
Frames
and
Framing
Framing essentially involves
selection
and
salience.
To frame is
to
select
some aspects
of
aperceived reality and make them more salient in a com-
municating text, in such a way as to promote aparticularproblem defini-
tion, causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recom-
mendation
for the item described. Typically frames diagnose, evaluate,
and prescribe, a point explored most thoroughly by Gamson (1992). An
example is the “cold war” frame that dominated
U.S.
news
of
foreign af-
fairs until recently. The cold war frame highlighted certain foreign
events-say, civil wars-as problems, identified their source (communist
rebels), offered moral judgments (atheistic aggression), and commended
particular solutions
(U.S.
support
for
the other side).
Frames, then,
dejineproblems-determine
what a causal agent is doing
with what costs and benefits, usually measured in terms
of
common cul-
tural values;
diagnose
causes-identify the forces creating the problem;
make moraljudgments-evaluate
causal agents and their effects; and
suggest remedies-offer
and justify treatments
for
the problems and pre-
dict their likely effects.
A
single sentence may perform more than one
of
these four framing functions, although many sentences in a text may per-
form none
of
them. And a frame in any particular text may not necessarily
include all four functions.
The cold war example also suggests that frames have at least four loca-
tions in the communication process: the communicator, the text, the re-
ceiver, and the culture.
Communicators
make conscious or unconscious
framing judgments in deciding what to say, guided by frames (often
called schemata) that organize their belief systems. The
text
contains
frames, which are manifested
by
the presence
or
absence
of
certain key-
words, stock phrases, stereotyped images, sources
of
information, and
sentences that provide thematically reinforcing clusters
of
facts
or
judg-
ments. The frames that guide the
receiver’s
thinking and conclusion may
or may not reflect the frames in the text and the framing intention
of
the
52

communicator. The
culture
is the stock
of
commonly invoked frames; in
fact, culture might be defined as the empirically demonstrable set
of
com-
mon frames exhibited in the discourse and thinking
of
most people in a
social grouping. Framing in a11 four locations includes similar functions:
selection and highlighting, and use
of
the highlighted elements
to
con-
struct an argument about problems and their causation, evaluation,
and/or solution.
How
Frames
Work
Frames highlight some bits
of
information about an item that is the sub-
ject
of
a communication, thereby elevating them in salience. The word
salience
itself needs
to
be defined:
It
means making
:i
piece
of
informa-
tion more noticeable, meaningful, or memorable
to
audiences. An in-
crease in salience enhances the probability that receivers will perceive
the information, discern meaning and thus process it, and store
it
in meni-
ory (see Fiske
&
Taylor,
1991).
Texts can make bits
of
information more salient by placement
or
repeti-
tion, or by associating them with culturally familiar symbols. However,
even a single nnillustrated appearance
of
a notion in an obscure part
of
the text can
be
highly salient,
if
it
comports with the existing schemata in
a receiver’s
belief
systems.
By
the same token, an idea emphasized in a
text can be difficult for receivers
to
notice, interpret,
or
remember
be-
cause
of
their existing schemata. For our purposes, schemata and closely
related concepts such as categories, scripts, or stereotypes connote men-
tally stored clusters
of
ideas that guide individuals’ processing
of
informa-
tion (see, e.g., Graber, 19881. Because salience is a product
of
the interac-
tion
of
texts and receivers, the presence
of
frames in the text, as detected
by researchers, does not guarantee their influence in audience thinking
(Entman, 1989; Graber, 1988).
Kahneman and Tversky
(1984)
offer perhaps the most widely cited re-
cent example
of
the power
of
framing and the way
it
operates by select-
ing and highlighting some features
of
reality while omitting others. The
authors asked experimental subjects the following:
Imagine thal the
1J.S.
is
preparing
for
the outbreak
of
an unusual Asian
disease, which is expected to kill
600people.
Two alternative programs
to combat the disease have beenproposed. Assume that the exact scien-
tz$c
estimates
of
the consequences qftheprograms are as,follows:
If
Pro-
gram
A
is adopted, 200people will
be
saved. Ifprogram
B
is adopted,
there is a one-thirdprobability that
GOOpeople
will be saved and a two-
thirdsprohahility that no people
will
he
saved. Which
of
the twopro-
grams wouldyou favor?(1984,
p.
243)
In this experiment,
72
percent
of
subjects chose Program
A;
28
percent
53
Toward
ClarzJicatzon
ofa
Fractured
Paradigm

,/ournal
of
Communication,
Autumn
199.3
chose Program B. In the next experiment,
identical options
to
treating the
same described situation were offered, but framed in terms
of
likely
deaths rather than likely lives saved:
“If
Program
C
is adopted,
400
people
will die.
If
Program
D
is adopted, there is a one-third probability that no-
body will die and a two-thirds probability that
600
people will die”
(Kahneman
&
Tversky,
1984,
p. 343). The percentages choosing the op-
tions were reversed by the framing. Program
C
was chosen by
22
percent,
though its twin Program
A
was selected by
72
percent; and Program
D
garnered
78
percent, while the identical Program B received only
28
per-
cent.
As
this example vividly illustrates, the frame determines whether most
people notice and how they understand and remember a problem, as
well as how they evaluate and choose
to
act upon
it.
The notion
of
fram-
ing thus implies that the frame has a common effect on large portions
of
the receiving audience, though
it
is not likely
to
have a universal effect
on
all.
Kahneman and Tversky’s experiments demonstrate that frames select
and call attention
to
particular aspects
of
the reality described, which log-
ically means that frames simultaneously direct attention away from other
aspects. Most frames are defined by what they omit as well as include,
and the omissions
of
potential problem definitions, explanations, evalua-
tions, and recommendations may be as critical as the inclusions in guid-
ing the audience.
tive description and omission
of
the features
of
a situation:
Edelman highlights the way frames exert their power through the selec-
The character, causes, and consequences of any phenomenon become
radically different as changes are made in what is prominently dis-
played, what is repressed and especially in how observations are classi-
jied.
.
.
.
(Uhe social world is
.
. .
a kaleidoscope ofpotential realities,
any
of
which can be readily evoked by altering the ways in which obser-
vations are framed and categorized.
(1993,
p.
232)
Receivers’ responses are clearly affected
if
they perceive and process in-
formation about one interpretation and possess little or incommensurable
data about alternatives. This
is
why exclusion
of
interpretations by frames
is as significant
to
outcomes as inclusion.
Sniderman, Brody, and Tetlock (1991) provide
a
clear instance
of
the
power
of
presence and absence in framing:
The effect of framing is toprime values differentially, establishing the
salience of the one
or
the other. (Thus].
. .
a majority of thepublic sup-
ports the rights ofpersons with
AIDS
when the issue is framed (in
a
sur-
vey question] to accentuate civil liberties considerations-and supports
. . .
mandatory testing when the issue is framed to accentuatepublic
health considerations.
(p.
52)
54

Toumrd
Clanlfication
of
u
Fractured
Puradigm
The text
of
the survey question supplies most people with the considera-
tions they use when they respond
to
the issue
of
AIDS testing (Zaller,
1992).
Often a potential counterframing
of
the subject is mostly or wholly
absent from a text, although,
to
use this instance, an audience member
with a strong civil liberties philosophy might reject mandatory testing
even
if
the poll framed
AIDS
strictly in public health terms.
Frames in Political News
This portrait
of
framing has important implications
for
political communi-
cation. Frames call attention
to
some aspects
of
reality while obscuring
other elements, which might lead audiences to have different reactions.
Politicians seeking support are thus compelled
to
compete with each
other and with journalists over news frames (Entman,
1989;
Riker,
1986).
Framing in this light plays a major role in the exertion
of
political power,
and the frame in a news text is really the imprint
of
power-it registers
the identity
of
actors or interests that competed
to
dominate the text.
many news texts exhibit homogeneous framing at one level
of
analysis,
yet competing frames at another. Thus, in the pre-war debate over
U.S.
policy toward Iraq, there was a tacit consensus among
U.S.
elites not to
argue for such options as negotiation between Iraq and Kuwait. The news
frame included only two remedies, war now or sanctions now with war
(likely) later, while problem definitions, causal analyses, and moral evalu-
ations were homogeneous. Between the selected remedies, however,
framing was contested by elites, and news coverage offered different sets
of
facts and evaluations. The Iraq example reveals that the power
of
news
frames can be self-reinforcing. During the pre-war debate, any critique
transcending the remedies inside the frame (war
soon
versus more time
for sanctions) breached the bounds
of
acceptable discourse, hence was
unlikely
to
influence policy. By conventional journalistic standards, such
views were not newsworthy (Entman
&
Page, in press). Unpublicized, the
views could gain few adherents and generate little perceived or actual ef-
fect on public opinion, which meant elites
felt
no pressure
to
expand the
frame
so
it
included other treatments for Iraqi aggression, such as negoti-
ation. Relatedly, Gamson
(1992)
observes that a frame can exert great
so-
cial power when encoded in a term like
ajfirmatiue action.
Once a term
is widely accepted,
to
use another is
to
risk that target audiences will per-
ceive the communicator as lacking credibility-or
will
even fail
to
under-
stand what the communicator is talking about. Thus the power
of
a frame
can be as great as that
of
language itself.
Reflecting the play
of
power and boundaries
of
discourse over an issue,
Benefits
of
a
Consistent Concept
of
Framing
An understanding
of
frames helps illuminate many empirical and norma-
tive controversies, most importantly because the concept
of
framing di-
55

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A process model of framing is developed, identifying four key processes that should be addressed in future research: frame building, frame setting, individual-level processes of framing, and a feedback loop from audiences to journalists.
Abstract: Research on framing is characterized by theoretical and empirical vagueness. This is due, in part, to the lack of a commonly shared theoretical model underlying framing research. Conceptual problems translate into operational problems, limiting the comparability of instruments and results. In this paper I systematize the fragmented approaches to framing in political communication and integrate them into a comprehensive model. I classify previous approaches to framing research along two dimensions: the type of frame examined (media frames vs. audience frames) and the way frames are operationalized (independent variable or dependent variable). I develop a process model of framing, identifying four key processes that should be addressed in future research: frame building, frame setting, individual-level processes of framing, and a feedback loop from audiences to journalists.

3,345 citations


Cites background from "Framing: Toward Clarification of a ..."

  • ...Entman (1993) referred to framing as “a scattered conceptualization” (p.␣ 51), with previous studies lacking clear conceptual definitions and relying on context-specific, rather than generally applicable operationalizations....

    [...]

  • ...The presentation of operationalizations and empirical results shows that the fractured paradigm to which Entman (1993) referred still exists....

    [...]

  • ...Entman (1993) offered a more detailed explanation of how media provide audiences with schemas for interpreting events....

    [...]

  • ...Pan and Kosicki (1993) and Entman (1993), for example, conceptually predicted a link between media and audience frames, but provided only exploratory analyses of media frames....

    [...]

  • ...Individual frames are defined as “mentally stored clusters of ideas that guide individuals’ processing of information” (Entman, 1993, p. 53)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A recent special issue of the Journal of Communication is devoted to theoretical explanations of news framing, agenda setting, and priming effects as mentioned in this paper, which examines if and how the three models are related and what potential relationships between them tell theorists and researchers about the effects of mass media.
Abstract: This special issue of Journal of Communication is devoted to theoretical explanations of news framing, agenda setting, and priming effects. It examines if and how the three models are related and what potential relationships between them tell theorists and researchers about the effects of mass media. As an introduction to this effort, this essay provides a very brief review of the three effects and their roots in media-effects research. Based on this overview, we highlight a few key dimensions along which one can compare, framing, agenda setting, and priming. We conclude with a description of the contexts within which the three models operate, and the broader implications that these conceptual distinctions have for the growth of our discipline. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00326.x In 1997, Republican pollster Frank Luntz sent out a 222-page memo called ‘‘Language of the 21st century’’ to select members of the U.S. Congress. Parts of the memo soon spread among staffers, members of Congress, and also journalists. Luntz’s message was simple: ‘‘It’s not what you say, it’s how you say it’’ (Luntz, in press). Drawing on various techniques for real-time message testing and focus grouping, Frank Luntz had researched Republican campaign messages and distilled terms and phrases that resonated with specific interpretive schemas among audiences and therefore helped shift people’s attitudes. In other words, the effect of the messages was not a function of content differences but of differences in the modes of presentation. The ideas outlined in the memo were hardly new, of course, and drew on decades of existing research in sociology (Goffman, 1974), economics (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), psychology (Kahneman & Tversky, 1984), cognitive linguistics (Lakoff, 2004), and communication (Entman, 1991; Iyengar, 1991). But Frank Luntz was the first professional pollster to systematically use the concept of framing as a campaign tool. The Democratic Party soon followed and George Lakoff published Don’t Think of an

2,365 citations


Cites background from "Framing: Toward Clarification of a ..."

  • ...Our field, unfortunately, has largely skipped the first step and rushed ahead to the second step, which explains much of the conceptual and terminological confusions that so many scholars have written about (e.g., Entman, 1993; Pan & Kosicki, 1993; Scheufele, 1999)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The authors investigated the prevalence of five news frames identified in earlier studies on framing and framing effects: attribution of responsibility, conflict, human interest, economic consequences, and morality, and found that the use of news frames depended on both the type of outlet and the topic most significant differences were not between media (television vs the press) but between sensationalist vs serious types of news outlets.
Abstract: We investigated the prevalence of 5 news frames identified in earlier studies on framing and framing effects: attribution of responsibility, conflict, human interest, economic consequences, and morality We content analyzed 2,601 newspaper stories and 1,522 television news stories in the period surrounding the Amsterdam meetings of European heads of state in 1997 Our results showed that, overall, the attribution of responsibility frame was most commonly used in the news, followed by the conflict, economic consequences, human interest, and morality frames, respectively The use of news frames depended on both the type of outlet and the type of topic Most significant differences were not between media (television vs the press) but between sensationalist vs serious types of news outlets Sober and serious newspapers and television news programs more often used the responsibility and conflict frames in the presentation of news, whereas sensationalist outlets more often used the human interest frame

2,006 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper examined how local television news outlets framed a specific, dramatic event: a demonstration and rally by the Ku Klux Klan (KKK) in a small Ohio city, and explored the effect of alternative news frames for this event on tolerance for KKK activities.
Abstract: public controversy. In the present research, we examine how local television news outlets framed a specific, dramatic event: a demonstration and rally by the Ku Klux Klan (KKK) in a small Ohio city. We explore the effect of alternative news frames for this event on tolerance for KKK activities. We also test contrasting hypotheses about how viewers psychologically process news frames, and how such frames ultimately affect viewers' thoughts about political controversies.

1,322 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper integrated the insights generated by framing, priming, and agenda-setting research through a systematic effort to conceptualize and understand their larger implications for political power and democracy, and proposed improved measures of slant and bias.
Abstract: This article proposes integrating the insights generated by framing, priming, and agenda-setting research through a systematic effort to conceptualize and understand their larger implications for political power and democracy. The organizing concept is bias, that curiously undertheorized staple of public discourse about the media. After showing how agenda setting, framing and priming fit together as tools of power, the article connects them to explicit definitions of news slant and the related but distinct phenomenon of bias. The article suggests improved measures of slant and bias. Properly defined and measured, slant and bias provide insight into how the media influence the distribution of power: who gets what, when, and how. Content analysis should be informed by explicit theory linking patterns of framing in the media text to predictable priming and agenda-setting effects on audiences. When unmoored by such underlying theory, measures and conclusions of media bias are suspect. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00336.x This article proposes integrating the insights generated by framing, priming, and agenda-setting research through a new, systematic effort to conceptualize and understand their implications for political power. The organizing concept is bias, that curiously undertheorized staple of public discourse about the media. With all the heat and attention it incites among activists and ordinary citizens, bias is yet to be defined clearly, let alone received much serious empirical attention (Niven, 2002). The term seems to take on three major meanings. Sometimes, it is applied to news that purportedly distorts or falsifies reality (distortion bias), sometimes to news that favors one side rather than providing equivalent treatment to both sides in a political conflict (content bias), and sometimes to the motivations and mindsets of journalists who allegedly produce the biased content (decision-making bias). This essay argues that we can make bias a robust, rigorous, theory-driven, and productive research concept by abandoning the first use while deploying new, more precisely delineated variants of the second and third. Depending on specific research objectives, the distinctions among these three concepts can be crucial (Scheufele, 2000). The present article suggests that parsimonious

1,311 citations


Cites background from "Framing: Toward Clarification of a ..."

  • ...Fully developed frames typically perform four functions: problem definition, causal analysis, moral judgment, and remedy promotion (Entman, 1993, 2004)....

    [...]

References
More filters
Book
25 Sep 2000
TL;DR: In this paper, the cognitive and psychophysical determinants of choice in risky and risk- less contexts are discussed, and the relation between decision values and experience values is discussed, as well as an approach to risky choice that sketches an approach for decision making that can be seen as the acceptance of a gamble that can yield various outcomes with different probabilities.
Abstract: We discuss the cognitive and the psy- chophysical determinants of choice in risky and risk- less contexts. The psychophysics of value induce risk aversion in the domain of gains and risk seeking in the domain of losses. The psychophysics of chance induce overweighting of sure things and of improbable events, relative to events of moderate probability. De- cision problems can be described or framed in multiple ways that give rise to different preferences, contrary to the invariance criterion of rational choice. The pro- cess of mental accounting, in which people organize the outcomes of transactions, explains some anomalies of consumer behavior. In particular, the acceptability of an option can depend on whether a negative outcome is evaluated as a cost or as an uncompensated loss. The relation between decision values and experience values is discussed. Making decisions is like speaking prose—people do it all the time, knowingly or unknowingly. It is hardly surprising, then, that the topic of decision making is shared by many disciplines, from mathematics and statistics, through economics and political science, to sociology and psychology. The study of decisions ad- dresses both normative and descriptive questions. The normative analysis is concerned with the nature of rationality and the logic of decision making. The de- scriptive analysis, in contrast, is concerned with peo- ple's beliefs and preferences as they are, not as they should be. The tension between normative and de- scriptive considerations characterizes much of the study of judgment and choice. Analyses of decision making commonly distin- guish risky and riskless choices. The paradigmatic example of decision under risk is the acceptability of a gamble that yields monetary outcomes with specified probabilities. A typical riskless decision concerns the acceptability of a transaction in which a good or a service is exchanged for money or labor. In the first part of this article we present an analysis of the cog- nitive and psychophysical factors that determine the value of risky prospects. In the second part we extend this analysis to transactions and trades. Risky Choice Risky choices, such as whether or not to take an umbrella and whether or not to go to war, are made without advance knowledge of their consequences. Because the consequences of such actions depend on uncertain events such as the weather or the opponent's resolve, the choice of an act may be construed as the acceptance of a gamble that can yield various out- comes with different probabilities. It is therefore nat- ural that the study of decision making under risk has focused on choices between simple gambles with monetary outcomes and specified probabilities, in the hope that these simple problems will reveal basic at- titudes toward risk and value. We shall sketch an approach to risky choice that

6,015 citations

Book
28 Aug 1992
TL;DR: Zaller as discussed by the authors developed a comprehensive theory to explain how people acquire political information from elites and the mass media and convert it into political preferences, and applied this theory to the dynamics of public opinion on a broad range of subjects, including domestic and foreign policy, trust in government, racial equality, and presidential approval, as well as voting behaviour in U.S. House, Senate and presidential elections.
Abstract: In this 1992 book John Zaller develops a comprehensive theory to explain how people acquire political information from elites and the mass media and convert it into political preferences. Using numerous specific examples, Zaller applies this theory to the dynamics of public opinion on a broad range of subjects, including domestic and foreign policy, trust in government, racial equality, and presidential approval, as well as voting behaviour in U.S. House, Senate, and presidential elections. The thoery is constructed from four basic premises. The first is that individuals differ substantially in their attention to politics and therefore in their exposure to elite sources of political information. The second is that people react critically to political communication only to the extent that they are knowledgeable about political affairs. The third is that people rarely have fixed attitudes on specific issues; rather, they construct 'preference statements' on the fly as they confront each issue raised. The fourth is that, in constructing these statements, people make the greatest use of ideas that are, for various reasons, the most immediately salient to them. Zaller emphasizes the role of political elites in establishing the terms of political discourse in the mass media and the powerful effect of this framing of issues on the dynamics of mass opinion on any given issue over time.

5,393 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, news discourse is conceived as a sociocognitive process involving all three players: sources, journalists, and audience members operating in the universe of shared culture and on the basis of socially defined roles.
Abstract: In the American political process, news discourse concerning public policy issues is carefully constructed. This occurs in part because both politicians and interest groups take an increasingly proactive approach to amplify their views of what an issue is about However, news media also play an active role in framing public policy issues. Thus, in this article, news discourse is conceived as a sociocognitive process involving all three players: sources, journalists, and audience members operating in the universe of shared culture and on the basis of socially defined roles. Framing analysis is presented as a constructivist approach to examine news discourse with the primary focus on conceptualizing news texts into empirically operationalizable dimensions—syntactical, script, thematic, and rhetorical structures—so that evidence of the news media's framing of issues in news texts may be gathered. This is considered an initial step toward analyzing the news discourse process as a whole. Finally, an ex...

1,764 citations

Book
30 Aug 1991
TL;DR: The role of heuristics in political reasoning is discussed in this paper, where a theory sketch of the role of the heuristic in reasoning is presented, along with a discussion of the principle-policy puzzle of American racial attitudes.
Abstract: List of tables and figures Preface 1. Introduction: major themes 2. The role of heuristics in political reasoning: a theory sketch 3. Values under pressure: AIDS and civil liberties 4. The principle-policy puzzle: the paradox of American racial attitudes 5. Reasoning chains 6. The likability heuristic 7. Democratic values and mass publics 8. Ideological reasoning 9. Information and electoral choice 10. Stability and change in party identification: presidential to off-years 11. The American dilemma: the role of law as a persuasive symbol 12. Ideology and issue persuasibility: dynamics of racial policy attitudes 13. The new racism and the American ethos 14. Retrospect and prospect Notes Bibliography Index

1,512 citations

Book
01 Jan 1986
TL;DR: Riker as discussed by the authors discusses the feature of politics that all of the manipulators exploited and sketches out the new political theory that explains why manipulation works the way it does, which is a useful and entertaining informal essay on political tactics that will have direct utility in the classroom.
Abstract: In twelve entertaining stories from history and current events, a noted political scientist and game theorist shows us how some of our heroes we as well as ordinary folk have manipulated their opponents in order to win political advantage. The stories come from many times and places, because manipulation of people by other people is universal: from the Roman Senate through the Constitutional Convention of 1787, to the Congress, state legislatures, and city councils of twentieth-century America. The results of manipulation are not trivial, as we see, for example, in Riker's account of Lincoln's outmaneuvering of Douglas in their debates and in his description of the parliamentary trick that defeated the Equal Rights Amendment only six years ago in the Virginia Senate. The tales can be enjoyed by anyone. For the scholar, they are held together by a concluding chapter in which Riker discusses the feature of politics that all of the manipulators exploited and sketches out the new political theory that explains why manipulation works the way it does. Preface Lincoln at Freeport Chauncey DePew and the Seventeenth Amendment The Flying Club Gouverneur Morris in the Philadelphia Convention Heresthetic in Fiction Camouflaging the Gerrymander Pliny the Younger on Parliamentary Law Trading Votes at the Constitutional Convention How to Win on a Roll Call by Not Voting Warren Magnuson and Nerve Gas Exploiting the Powell Amendment Reed and Cannon Conclusion "A useful and entertaining informal essay on political tactics that will have direct utility in the classroom."-Douglas W. Rae, Yale University William H. Riker is Wilson Professor of Political Science at the University of Rochester and a former president of the American Political Science Association. He is the author of numerous books, including Theory of Political Coalitions, a classic in the field.

1,145 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (13)
Q1. What contributions have the authors mentioned in the paper "Framing: toward clarification of a fractured paradigm" ?

Reaching this goal would require a more self-conscious determination by communication scholars to plumb other fields and feed back their studies to outside researchers. The idea of “ framing ” offers a case study of just the kind of scattered conceptualization I have identified. Analysis o f this concept suggests how the discipline of communication might contribute something unique: synthesizing a key concept ’ s disparate uses, showing how they invariably involve communication, and constructing a coherent theory from them. 

Texts can make bits of information more salient by placement o r repetition, or by associating them with culturally familiar symbols. 

The major task o f determining textual meaning should be to identify and describe frames; content analysis informed by a theory of framing would avoid treating all negative or positive terms or utterances as equally salient and influential. 

If Program D is adopted, there is a one-third probability that nobody will die and a two-thirds probability that 600 people will die” (Kahneman & Tversky, 1984, p. 343). 

Framing in this light plays a major role in the exertion of political power, and the frame in a news text is really the imprint of power-it registers the identity of actors or interests that competed to dominate the text. 

In Zaller’s (1992) account, framing appears to be a central power in the democratic process, for political elites control the framing of issues. 

Unguided by a framing paradigm, content analysis may often yield data that misrepresent the media messages that most audience members are actually picking up. 

Approving the option with 60 percent support is not axiomatically the most democratic response because of the cyclical majority problem (Riker, 19861, which makes majority rule among several complex options mathematically impossible. 

Because salience is a product of the interaction of texts and receivers, the presence of frames in the text, as detected by researchers, does not guarantee their influence in audience thinking (Entman, 1989; Graber, 1988). 

the views could gain few adherents and generate little perceived or actual effect on public opinion, which meant elites felt no pressure to expand the frame so it included other treatments for Iraqi aggression, such as negotiation. 

During the pre-war debate, any critique transcending the remedies inside the frame (war soon versus more time for sanctions) breached the bounds of acceptable discourse, hence was unlikely to influence policy. 

The framing paradigm could be applied with similar benefits to the study of public opinion and voting behavior in political science; to cognitive studies in social psychology; or to class, gender, and race research in cultural studies and sociology, to name a few. 

Just as important, attempting to determine which of the differently framed opinions is the closest to the public’s “real” sentiments appears futile, because it would require agreement among contending elites and citizens on which frame was most accurate, fair, complete, and so forth.