scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Haptic Aftereffect of Curved Surfaces

01 Jan 1996-Perception (Pion)-Vol. 25, Iss: 1, pp 109-119
TL;DR: It is concluded that the haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces is an important effect that occurs almost instantaneously and lasts for an appreciable period.
Abstract: A haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces is demonstrated. Two spherical surfaces were presented sequentially to human subjects. They rested one hand on the first (conditioning) surface. After a fixed conditioning period they transferred their hand to the second (test) surface and judged whether the test surface was convex or concave. In experiment 1 the curvature of the conditioning surface was varied; the subject's judgment of convexity or concavity of the test surface was strongly shifted in the direction opposite to the curvature of the conditioning surface (negative aftereffect). Therefore, subjects judged a flat surface to be concave after being exposed to a convex surface. After a conditioning period of 5 s the shift was about 20% of the curvature of the conditioning surface. In experiment 2 the duration of the conditioning period was varied; the magnitude of the aftereffect could be described by a first-order integrator with a time constant of 2 s. In experiment 3 the time interval between the conditioning period and the touching of the second surface was varied; the magnitude of the aftereffect could be described by an exponential decay with a time constant of 40 s. It is concluded that the haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces is an important effect that occurs almost instantaneously and lasts for an appreciable period.

Summary (2 min read)

1 Introduction

  • Probably all modalities give rise to aftereffects.
  • Adaptation affects the perception of the stimulus itself.
  • They showed that tactile sensation produced by a weight resting on the skin disappeared when the rate at which the weight sank into the skin had decreased below a minimum rate required to stimulate.
  • If a rectangular inspection block is oriented with its long side horizontally and with its short side vertically, and if a subject alternately grasps this block horizontally and vertically, then the horizontal side of a subsequently presented square test block is perceived shorter than the vertical side (Walker 1977) .
  • The magnitude of the aftereffect was measured for several different periods of time during which the conditioning surface was touched (conditioning period).

2 Experiment 1

  • The results of this experiment can give us an idea of how haptic curvature information is processed.
  • They put their right hand under the curtain so that they could touch the stimuli without seeing them.
  • In order to measure the phenomenal flatness the authors presented each conditioning surface together with nine differently curved test surfaces.
  • All presentations were randomly distributed over seven sessions.

2.2 Results

  • The percentage of convex judgments is plotted against the curvature of the test surface.
  • The threshold, in this case the threshold of curvature detection, is inversely proportional to the steepness of the curve and corresponds to the difference between the values of the 50% and 85% points.
  • The stronger the curvature of the conditioning surface the larger the shift of the phenomenal flatness.
  • Phenomenal flatness depends thus rather critically and systematically on previous exposure to curved surfaces.

3.1 Method

  • The stimuli and the experimental setup were identical to those in experiment 1.
  • Three naive paid subjects participated in this experiment; one was a strongly left-handed male (MZ) and two were strongly right-handed males (TB and MH).
  • MH also participated in the first experiment.
  • The duration of the conditioning period was varied systematically: 2, 5, and 10 s. From pilot experiments this seemed to be the most interesting range, but the authors were also interested in shorter and longer durations.

3.2 Results

  • For the conditioning surfaces of 4 m-1 and 4 m-1 the phenomenal flatness is plotted against the duration of the conditioning period.
  • Because of the day-to-day variations, the values of the phenomenal flatness could not be compared directly.
  • The time t corresponds to the duration of the conditioning period.
  • Actually, the time constant is the time at which the magnitude of the aftereffect is 63% of its maximum Ab.
  • For all subjects the two time constants tb(4) and tb(-4) are not significantly different.

4 Experiment 3

  • When two surfaces are touched sequentially some finite time elapses between the removal of the hand from the first surface and the contact with the second surface.
  • This time interval could influence the magnitude of the aftereffect, although it is also conceivable that the aftereffect is maintained as long as the hand is not stimulated.
  • The authors did this by varying the length of the time interval between the conditioning period and the touching of the test surface.

4.1 Method

  • The stimuli and the experimental setup were identical to those in experiments 1 and 2.
  • Two naïve paid subjects participated in this experiment, MV (24 years) and MB (25 years).
  • The previous experiment had shown that after a conditioning period of 10 s the aftereffect did not (1) A second reason for subtracting this shift was that the magnitude of the aftereffect is not the value of the phenomenal flatness itself, but it is the shift of the phenomenal flatness compared with that in a condition without adaptation.
  • The time interval between the conditioning period and the touching of the test surface was varied: 5, 20, and 40 s.
  • During this interval subjects had to hold their hand in the air in a comfortable manner, but keep their fingers almost straight.

4.2 Results

  • In figure 6 the influence of the time interval is shown for subject MV.
  • For the conditioning surfaces of 4 m-1 and 4 m-1 the phenomenal flatness is plotted against the length of the time interval.
  • This aftereffect of curved surfaces is perhaps surprising because most people tend to believe that the authors need only put their hand on an object to obtain a veridical impression of its shape.
  • On the other hand, many different kinds of aftereffects occur in other modalities.
  • If the underlying mechanisms responsible for the aftereffect behave like a simple first-order system the authors would expect the time constant to be the same for all conditioning periods.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Perception, 1996, volume 25, pages 109-119
Haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces
Ingrid M L C Vogels, Astrid M L Kappers, Jan J
Koenderink
Helmholtz lnstituut, Princetonplein 5, 3584 CC Utrecht, The
Netherlands
Received 15 May 1995, in revised form 14 November 1995
Abstract. A haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces
is demonstrated. Two spherical surfaces were
presented sequentially to human subjects. They rested one hand on
the first (conditioning) surface.
After a fixed conditioning period they transferred their hand to
the second (test) surface and judged
whether the test surface was convex or concave. In experiment I
the curvature of the conditioning
surface was varied; the subject's judgment of convexity or concavity
of the test surface was strongly
shifted in the direction opposite to the curvature of the
conditioning surface (negative aftereffect).
Therefore, subjects judged a flat surface to be concave after being
exposed to a convex surface. After a
conditioning period of 5 s the shift was about 20% of the curvature
of the conditioning surface.
In experiment 2 the duration of the conditioning period was
varied; the magnitude of the aftereffect
could be described by a first-order integrator with a time constant
of 2 s. In experiment 3 the time
interval between the conditioning period and the touching of
the second surface was varied; the
magnitude of the aftereffect could be described by an exponential decay
with a time constant of 40 s.
It is concluded that the haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces is an
important effect that occurs almost
instantaneously and lasts for an appreciable period.
1 Introduction
Probably all modalities give rise to aftereffects. Gibson (1937) was one
of the first to
emphasise this similarity between the senses. In his paper he
described several exam-
ples of visual, tactual, and gustatory stimuli which lead to
adaptation with negative
aftereffects. Adaptation and aftereffect, both of which are caused
by prolonged exposure
to a constant stimulus, are in fact two
distinct phenomena. Adaptation affects the
perception of the stimulus itself. Aftereffects are defined as a
change of the 'physical
phenomenal correspondence' of the stimulus dimension
occurring after prolonged
stimulation. The term adaptation is often used as a synonym
for various ill-defined
terms such as normalisation, satiation,
habituation, or fatigue. Such careless usage
often leads to unfortunate misunderstandings. For instance,
Gibson (1937) assumed
that adaptation and aftereffect are two facets of a
single process, whereas Colheart
(1971) argued that they may be quite separate effects. In
this paper we use the men-
tioned conventions.
In modalities such as vision or hearing, adaptation
and aftereffects have been
investigated intensively. In haptic perception, which relies on both
the cutaneous sense
and kinesthesis, these phenomena have received
considerably less attention, although
they have already been known phenomenologically for a
long time. Here a number of
examples of studied haptic aftereffects is presented. We
certainly do not aim to be
complete.
It is well known that tepid water feels cold to a hand
previously exposed to hot
water. Abbott (1914) mentioned that the phenomenal
'neutral' temperature appears to
be so adaptable that it can be raised as high as 39
°C, whereupon 37 °C feels cold
and it can fall as low as 11 °C, whereupon 12 °C feels warm.
Thalman (1922) applied
a cord of rough material to the
underside of the bare forearm of human subjects.
The cord moved in a constant direction and with a constant
velocity. As soon as the
movement stopped, subjects perceived a movement
in the opposite direction both
when the cord was removed from the arm and when the cord
remained in contact with

110
IMLC Vogels, A M L Kappers, J J Koenderink
the arm. Wohlgemuth (1911) and Hazlewood (1971) failed
to find evidence in support
of a tactile movement aftereffect. Recently, Hollins
and Favorov (1994) demonstrated
that the texture of the surface applied to the skin might
determine the occurrence or
nonoccurrence of the aftereffect. When subjects cupped their hand around
a moving
drum which was covered with a smooth microtexture
combined with a square wave
of low spatial frequency, the aftereffect
was especially effective. The vividness and
duration of the aftereffect increased
over the range of adaptation durations explored
(30 180 s). Gibson (1933) found that after 3 min of
moving the fingers over a curved
cardboard edge a straight edge felt curved in the opposite
direction. Nafe and Wagoner
(1941) investigated pressure adaptation. They showed
that tactile sensation produced
by a weight resting on the skin disappeared when the
rate at which the weight sank
into the skin had decreased below
a minimum rate required to stimulate. The removal
of the weight produced a clear tactile sensation.
Koehler and Dinnerstein (1947)
showed that when the width of a test bar felt with
one hand had to be matched with
the other hand, the matching depended
on a satiation bar previously felt with the
tested hand. A satiation object smaller than the
test object enlarged the matched width
of the test bar; a wider satiation object had the opposite
effect. Gibson (1963) investi-
gated the aftereffect of the perception of
vergence. He found that two parallel palm
boards felt divergent after they had been held
convergent for some time, and felt
convergent after being held divergent. In order to make the surfaces feel
parallel they
had to be rotated over about 50 to 10°, according
to the amount of convergence or
divergence of the hand during adaptation. After
an adaptation period of 12 s the
aftereffect did not show any increase with time and after
a so-called recovery period of
56 s the aftereffect still had no tendency to decrease. Hahn
(1966) investigated vibro-
tactile adaptation by two different methods. In the first
method he measured the
absolute threshold, which is the just-noticeable amplitude of
the sinusoidal vibration.
In the second method he measured the subjective magnitude,
by matching the sensa-
tion in the adapted finger to that in the contralateral finger.
Increasing the time of
vibrotactile stimulation resulted in an increase of the threshold
and a decrease of the
subjective magnitude. Both methods showed the
same temporal course of adaptation.
Adaptation continued to increase after a period of 20 min.
Not only simple aftereffects, like previous examples, but also
contingent aftereffects
have been demonstrated in the haptic modality In contingent
aftereffects, a correlation
between two stimulus dimensions is established during
an inspection period, and
during a subsequent test period the
appearance of an object on one dimension is
dependent on the location on the other dimension. If
a narrow inspection block,
located on the subject's left, and a wide block, located
on the right, are alternately
grasped by a subject between the fingers of
a single hand then an intermediate test
block presented on the left is perceived wider than
an equal block on the right (Walker
and Shea 1974). The perceived width of the test block is contingent
on the location of
the block. If a rectangular inspection block is oriented with its
long side horizontally
and with its short side vertically, and if a subject alternately
grasps this block horizon-
tally and vertically, then the horizontal side of
a subsequently presented square test
block is perceived shorter than the vertical side (Walker 1977).
Here the perceived size
of a test block is contingent on the orientation of the block.
Thus, many haptic stimuli give rise to
an aftereffect. Because the haptic sense is
particularly important for the recognition and manipulation of
objects, it would be
useful if the haptic sense provided us with veridical information
about the shape and
curvature of environmental objects. Because aftereffects could affect
veridicality,
it
would be of interest to know whether aftereffects
occur when subjects touch curved
three-dimensional objects. Such an aftereffect of curved surfaces
has not been reported
in the literature.

Haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces
111
It is known that in general the categorisation of curvature of
one-dimensional strips
is not veridical. Hunter (1954) and Davidson (1972) let subjects judge
whether strips
were convex, concave, or straight. All
subjects judged the straight strips to be curved
and the strips which were judged to be straight typically corresponded to a
curved
strip. So, straight was not judged to be straight. It is conceivable that in
the case of the
judgment of curved surfaces, the surface which is judged to be flat (phenomenal
flatness)
also corresponds to a curved surface. The interesting question is, however,
whether the
phenomenal flatness, veridical or not, is constant over time. The experiments
reported
here are designed to investigate this. In the first experiment we
investigated whether the
categorisation of curvature is influenced by a previously touched curved
surface. Two
spherical surfaces (a conditioning surface and a test surface) were presented
sequentially.
We measured which curvature of the test surface was judged to
be flat for several
differently curved conditioning surfaces. We found an aftereffect; the
phenomenal
flatness depended on the curvature of the conditioning surface. In
experiment 2 we
investigated how much time was needed to build up the aftereffect.
The magnitude of
the aftereffect was measured for several different periods of time
during which the
conditioning surface was touched (conditioning period). In experiment 3 we
investigated
whether the aftereffect was maintained or whether it disappeared
after the touching
had ceased. The magnitude of the aftereffect was measured for
several time intervals
between the conditioning period and the touching of the test surface.
2 Experiment 1
In the first experiment we tested whether judgments of convexity or
concavity of a
spherical surface are influenced by the curvature of a previously touched
surface. The
results of this experiment can give us an idea of how haptic curvature
information is
processed. Does the haptic system possess an absolute internal reference for curvature
or does recalibration occur? The results are
also important for other haptic experiments:
does it make sense to let subjects judge the absolute curvature when aspects
of haptic
curvature perception are being investigated?
2.1 Method
2.1.1 Experimental setup. The stimuli had a spherical upper
surface, either convex or
concave, and a flat bottom which rested on a
table (see figure 1). The diameter of the
stimuli was 20 cm and the total height ranged between 2.5 and 6.5 cm.
This height
was not directly related to the curvature,
because we randomised the base height.
20 cm
Figure 1. Example of a convex spherical stimulus. All stimuli had a flat bottom
and a diameter
of 20 cm. The height difference H between the total height of the stimulus
and the base
height B was 2 cm for the largest curvature (4 m-I) and 0.125 cm for the
smallest curvature
(0.25 m-'). The total height ranged between 2.5 and 6.5 cm.
H
B

112
IMLC Vogels, A M L Kappers, J J Koenderink
The curvature ranged from 4 m-1 to 4 m-1 and two successive curvatures differed
by a factor -12- (the whole set of curvatures being 4, 2.8, 2, 1.4, 1, 0.7, 0.5,
0.35, 0.25, 0, 0.25, 0.35, 0.5, 0.7, 1, 1.4, 2, 2.8, and 4 m-1). Curvature is convention-
ally expressed as reciprocal radius, so a sphere with radius 2
m has a curvature of
0.5 m-1. A plane has zero curvature and the curvature of the tip of
a needle would
tend to infinity. A positively curved surface is called convex,
a negatively curved surface
is called concave.
2.1.2 Subjects. Three naïve paid subjects, about 20
years old, participated in this
experiment; one was a strongly left-handed female (GD) and two
were strongly right-
handed males (MH and EK). The degree of left/right-handedness is defined by Coren
(1993). None of the subjects reported any haptic deficiencies.
2.1.3 Procedure. Subjects were seated behind a curtain in front of
a table. They put
their right hand under the curtain so that they could touch the stimuli without seeing
them. In each trial subjects put their hand on a surface (conditioning surface) for
a fixed
conditioning period of 5 s. After that period they transferred their hand
to a second
surface (test surface). Subjects had to decide whether this second surface
was convex
or concave. They were not allowed to move their hand over the surfaces. All stimuli
were in the same position with respect to the thorax when they were touched. Seven
conditioning surfaces were used, with curvatures of 4, 2, 1, 0,
1, 2, and 4 m-1.
In order to measure the phenomenal flatness we presented each conditioning surface
together with nine differently curved test surfaces. Each combination of conditioning
surface and test surface was presented 15 times. The total number of presentations
was thus 945 (7 conditioning surfaces x 9 test surfaces x 15 presentations). All presen-
tations were randomly distributed over seven sessions. Each session took about 1 h,
so for each subject the experiment involved 7 h in total.
We adapted the range of test surfaces to the curvature of the surface judged
to
be flat. This was done by means of a pilot experiment in which
we estimated which
surface was judged to be flat for each conditioning surface and each subject. The nine
test surfaces were chosen symmetrically around this phenomenal flat surface. In this
way
the range of test surfaces was optimised for each conditioning surface.
2.2 Results
In figure 2 we present the results in the case of a flat conditioning surface (0 m-1) for
subject GD. The percentage of convex judgments is plotted against the
curvature of
the test surface. The psychometric function is fitted with
an error function, according
to the Levenberg Marquardt method (Press et al 1988). This method minimises x2.
Figure 2 is a typical example of how well the function fits the data (x2
= 3642).
The fit can be characterised by two parameters: the shift and the threshold. The shift
corresponds to the value of the 50% point. In our analysis the shift of the psycho-
metric curve is called 'phenomenal flatness' because the curved surface for which
convex and concave judgments are equally frequent is assumed to be perceived as flat.
The threshold, in this case the threshold of curvature detection, is inversely
propor-
tional to the steepness of the curve and corresponds to the difference between the
values of the 50% and 85% points. Because the psychometric
curve is symmetrical,
the threshold also corresponds to the difference between the values of the 50%
and
15% points.
It can be seen that in figure 2 the value of the 50% point is
not zero. In this
example, the surface which is perceived as flat corresponds to
a geometrically concave
surface. For all three subjects phenomenal flatness corresponded
to a surface with
negative curvature.
1

Haptic aftereffect of curved surfaces
113
100:
807
60.
threlold
40.:
0
20
0
)1
shift
-1 0
-0.5
0.0
0.5
1 0
Curvature/m-1
Figure 2. The percentage of convex judgments for a flat conditioning
surface as a function of
the curvature of the test surface, for subject GD. A psychometric
function is fitted to the data
points (x2 = 3642). The shift, which corresponds to the value of the 50%
point, is 0.18 m-1.
This indicates that a geometrically flat surface is not judged to be flat. The
threshold for curva-
ture detection, which corresponds to the difference between
the values of the 50% and 85% (or
15%) points, is 0.32 m-1.
Figure 3 shows the results in the case of all conditioning surfaces for subject GD.
For clarity the fitted curves are plotted without the data points. Most
values of X2 are
somewhat smaller than the value of figure 2; x2 varies between 105 and 16 323
for
subject GD, 260 and 7650 for subject MH, and 740 and 4480 for subject
EK.
The shapes of the psychometric curves look rather similar. Although it seems as
if the
steepness of the curves decreases with increasing curvature,
this is not significant.
For the other two subjects the steepness did not systematically increase or
decrease.
The threshold is
thus independent of the curvature of the conditioning surface.
The average thresholds are 0.25 ± 0.04 in-', 0.24 ± 0.03 m-1, and 0.21 ± 0.04 m
for
subjects GD, MH, and EK respectively.
The position of the curves, ie the value of the phenomenal flatness, is not constant.
The curves are horizontally shifted with respect to each other, the order being the same
100
80 -
20 -
0
1 5
1.0
0.5
0.0
0.5 1.0
1 5
Curvature/m-1
Figure 3. Psychometric curves for seven differently curved conditioning surfaces (-4, 2, 1, 0,
1, 2, and 4 m-1) for subject GD. The values of x2 are, from left to right, 105, 593, 322, 3642,
1469, 3567, and 16 323. The thresholds of the seven curves are almost similar; the average
is 0.25 ± 0.04 m-1. The values of the phenomenal flatness are clearly different; the stronger the
conditioning curvature the larger the shift of the phenomenal flatness. This indicates that
phenomenal flatness depends on previous exposure to curved surfaces.
ti
.
-
-
0
g 60
kl 40
2 1 0 1
2 4

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper surveys more than twenty types of tactile illusions and discusses several of their aspects, including the ease with which they can be demonstrated and whether they have clear visual analogs.

146 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper surveys the research literature on robust tactile and haptic illusions by briefly considering a number of important general themes that have emerged in the materials surveyed.
Abstract: This paper surveys the research literature on robust tactile and haptic illusions. The illusions are organized into two categories. The first category relates to objects and their properties, and is further differentiated in terms of haptic processing of material versus geometric object properties. The second category relates to haptic space, and is further differentiated in terms of the observer's own body versus external space. The illusions are initially described and where possible addressed in terms of their functional properties and/or underlying neural processes. The significance of these illusions for the design of tactile and haptic displays is also discussed. We conclude by briefly considering a number of important general themes that have emerged in the materials surveyed.

142 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown that the odor representation evolves after the first breath and that there is a centrally maintained odor afterimage, similar to other sensory systems, which may help identify novel odorants in complex environments.
Abstract: Rodents can discriminate odors in one breath, and mammalian olfaction research has thus focused on the first breath. However, sensory representations dynamically change during and after stimuli. To investigate these dynamics, we recorded spike trains from the olfactory bulb of awake, head-fixed mice and found that some mitral cells’ odor representations changed following the first breath and others continued after odor cessation. Population analysis revealed that these postodor responses contained odor- and concentration-specific information—an odor afterimage. Using calcium imaging, we found that most olfactory glomerular activity was restricted to the odor presentation, implying that the afterimage is not primarily peripheral. The odor afterimage was not dependent on odorant physicochemical properties. To artificially induce aftereffects, we photostimulated mitral cells using channelrhodopsin and recorded centrally maintained persistent activity. The strength and persistence of the afterimage was dependent on the duration of both artificial and natural stimulation. In summary, we show that the odor representation evolves after the first breath and that there is a centrally maintained odor afterimage, similar to other sensory systems. These dynamics may help identify novel odorants in complex environments.

68 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results of 3 experiments in which people viewed simple animations of objects colliding and made judgments of force and resistance supported several predictions made by this account.
Abstract: Impressions of force are commonplace in the visual perception of objects interacting. It is proposed that these impressions have their source in haptically mediated experiences of exertion of force in actions on objects. Visual impressions of force in interactions between objects occur by a kind of generalization of the proprioceptive impression of force to interactions between objects on the basis of matching to stored representations of actions on objects carried out by the perceiver. Such experiences give rise to a distinctive perceptual interpretation of interactions between objects as involving force exerted by one object acting against resistance offered by the other object. Active, moving objects are seen as exerting force; inactive objects are seen as offering varying degrees of resistance and not as exerting force unless there is reason to think that they acted back on the active object. The results of 3 experiments in which people viewed simple animations of objects colliding and made judgments of force and resistance supported several predictions made by this account.

65 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The observed response across a variety of stiffness values lead to a constant average interaction force after adaptation, which suggests that the nervous system may select from the two responses through a mechanism that attempts to establish a constant interaction force.
Abstract: The ability to discriminate an object's shape and mechanical properties from touch is one of the most fundamental somatosensory functions. When exploring physical properties of an object, such as stiffness and curvature, humans probe the object's surface and obtain information from the many sensory receptors in their upper limbs. This sensory information is critical for the guidance of actions. We studied how humans acquire an internal representation of the shape and mechanical properties of surfaces and how this information affects the execution of trajectories over the surface. Experiments involved subjects executing trajectories while holding a planar manipulandum that renders planar virtual objects with variable shape and mechanical properties. Subjects were instructed to make reaching movements with the hand between points on the boundary of a curved virtual disk of varying stiffness and curvature. The results suggest two classifications of adaptive responses: force perturbations and object boundaries. In the first case, a rectilinear hand movement is enforced by opposing the interaction forces. In the second case, the trajectory conforms to the object boundary so as to reduce interaction forces. While this dichotomy is evident for very rigid and very soft objects, the likelihood of an object boundary classification depended, in a smooth and monotonic way, on the average force experienced during the initial movements. Furthermore, the observed response across a variety of stiffness values lead to a constant average interaction force after adaptation. This suggests that the nervous system may select from the two responses through a mechanism that attempts to establish a constant interaction force.

60 citations


Cites background from "Haptic Aftereffect of Curved Surfac..."

  • ...Further study of actively touched curved surfaces has shown that adaptation and after effects are present after haptic exploration (Vogels et al. 1996)....

    [...]

References
More filters
01 Jan 1994
TL;DR: The Diskette v 2.06, 3.5''[1.44M] for IBM PC, PS/2 and compatibles [DOS] Reference Record created on 2004-09-07, modified on 2016-08-08.
Abstract: Note: Includes bibliographical references, 3 appendixes and 2 indexes.- Diskette v 2.06, 3.5''[1.44M] for IBM PC, PS/2 and compatibles [DOS] Reference Record created on 2004-09-07, modified on 2016-08-08

19,881 citations

Book
05 Sep 2011

211 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the functional similarities between the senses rather than the differences between different modalities have been studied in a laboratory manual of experiments on sensation in which the emphasis was laid upon phenomena common to the various modalities and qualities.
Abstract: Psychologists have in the past directed a great deal of effort toward classifying the sensory categories of our world— distinguishing, delimiting, and introspectively analyzing its qualities; but they have spent very little time in noting or studying the functional similarities which cut across these categories. That red, sour and warm are distinct experiences, based on different processes, has been emphasized to the neglect of the fact that they all manifest adaptation and other phenomena in common. Holt and Yerkes (12) prepared in 1903 a laboratory manual of experiments on sensation in which the emphasis was laid upon phenomena common to the various modalities and qualities—latency, after-image, adaptation, contrast, fusion, inhibition, and others—and in general upon the similarities between the senses rather than the differences. But this lead has not been followed in experiments to any significant extent.

150 citations