scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Infant and parent factors associated with early maternal sensitivity: A caregiver-attachment systems approach

TL;DR: For mothers of avoidant children, vagal withdrawal was associated with sensitivity to child distress, and no association was found betweenvagal withdrawal and sensitivity for mothers of securely attached children, suggesting that mothers ofAvoidant children may be uniquely challenged by the affective demands of their infants.
Abstract: We examined variations in maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child age as a function of child negativity and maternal physiology. We expected maternal vagal withdrawal in response to infant negative affect to facilitate the maintenance of sensitivity, but only for mothers of securely attached children. One hundred and forty-eight infant-mother dyads were observed in multiple contexts at 6 months of child age, and associations among maternal and child variables were examined with respect to 12-month attachment quality. Mothers of later securely attached children were more sensitive than mothers of avoidant children. However, sensitivity decreased for all mothers at high levels of infant negative affect. Furthermore, for mothers of avoidant children, vagal withdrawal was associated with sensitivity to child distress. No association was found between vagal withdrawal and sensitivity for mothers of securely attached children. This suggests that mothers of avoidant children may be uniquely challenged by the affective demands of their infants.

Summary (5 min read)

Introduction

  • The authors expected maternal vagal withdrawal in response to infant negative affect to facilitate the maintenance of sensitivity, but only for mothers of securely attached children.
  • Furthermore, for mothers of avoidant children, vagal withdrawal was associated with sensitivity to child distress.
  • © 2007 Published by Elsevier Inc. Keywords: Infant; Parent; Caregiver Article:.
  • Additionally, the notion that there may be caregiver regulatory qualities that, via Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Pederson, Gleason, Moran, & Bento, 1998; Pedersen & Moran, 1995).

1. CHILD FACTORS, MATERNAL SENSITIVITY, AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY

  • Bowlby’s (1969) attachment theory highlighted the importance of shifts between positive and negative affect as the primary mechanism of communication for infants.
  • The coordination of caregiving behaviors with the infant attachment system is a benchmark of sensitive parenting, which is exemplified by child-centered responses to the physical and emotional needs of the infant (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1969).
  • Given this, their second goal was to determine whether physiological measures of maternal self-regulation were associated with sensitive caregiving in response to changes in the infant’s affective state.
  • Porges (1985, 1991, 1996) has (i.e., vagal withdrawal) is believed to reflect the parasympathetic influence of vagal regulation, whereby the withdrawal of parasympathetic control of the heart allows for sympathetic activity and thus increases in heart rate and cardiac output.
  • As such, effective vagal withdrawal has been examined as an index of self-regulation and may underlie the behavioral attention strategies necessary for regulation of arousal (Huffman et al., 1998; Posner & Rothbart, 2000).

3. HYPOTHESES

  • The goals of the current research were (1) to determine whether associations between child negative affect and maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child age were constant across later attachment classifications, and (2) to determine whether mother’s physiological regulation, as indexed by vagal withdrawal, mediates or moderates associations between infant negativity and maternal sensitivity.
  • For their second hypothesis, the authors predicted that for mothers of secure children, high maternal vagal withdrawal would be associated with the maintenance of sensitivity when mothers were faced with high levels of infant negative affect.
  • The current analyses are limited to dyads with organized attachment relationships at 12 months of age.
  • There is no conceptual basis for including them in the present analyses.
  • Specifically, several studies have shown that these mothers are as sensitive as mothers with children in other attachment classifications, secure or otherwise (van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1999).

4.1. Participants

  • The participants in the current study were 173 families recruited by the Durham Child Health income groups.
  • The family’s race was determined by the race of the mother (or primary caregiver), while income status was determined by whether families were above or below 200 percent of the federally established poverty threshold.
  • Birth order or family structures were not used as inclusion criteria.
  • Of those, 7 families were excluded due to audio-visual difficulties that made their videotaped assessments impossible to code.
  • In addition to the focal child, 69 families in this sample had additional children in the household.

4.2.1. 6-Month free play observations

  • Mothers and children were observed in the home for the free play session, during which time mothers were asked to interact with their children as they normally would during a typical day.
  • A standard set of toys was provided for the mother and child to use, and the pair was asked to sit on a blanket that was laid out across the floor.
  • This session was videotaped for later coding, with researchers monitoring the camera discreetly to minimize interference with the ongoing interaction.

4.2.2. 6-Month book reading observations

  • Within 2 weeks of the home visit, mothers and children participated in a 6-month laboratory visit.
  • During this time mothers were asked to try to involve their child with a picture book provided by the researchers.
  • The book involved pictures without words to control for any effect of illiteracy among the parents.
  • Mothers were responsible for creating a storyline that followed the illustrations in the book.
  • Like the free play session in the home visit, this procedure took place on the floor with the child positioned between the mother and a single camera.

4.2.3. 6-Month challenge tasks observations

  • The challenge tasks administered in the laboratory visit were the Still Face Paradigm (Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1979) and the arms restraint procedure (Goldsmith & Rothbarth, 1994).
  • For these purposes the mother was asked to secure her child in a car seat situated on top of a large, sturdy table.
  • The mother was again asked to turn her head away for 15 s, after which she was instructed to interact with her child for 2 min.
  • After this recovery period, the mother was instructed to again turn her head away from her child, but this time she was also asked to gently hold her child’s arms down and to do so with just enough pressure to prevent arm movements.
  • Scores for maternal and child behaviors from the two recovery periods were used, and a summary score was derived by collapsing across the two 2-min interactive episodes that followed the challenge procedures.

4.2.4. 12-Month Strange Situation Paradigm

  • At 12 months of child age another laboratory visit was scheduled, during which mothers and children participated in the Ainsworth Strange Situation Paradigm (SSP).
  • This procedure followed the protocol developed by Ainsworth et al. (1978) for observing and classifying infants into discrete categories of attachment quality.

4.3.1. Infant negative affect

  • During each 6-month observation (free play, book reading, and challenge recovery), child negative affect was coded in 5-s intervals using a 3-point scale adapted from previous studies (Haley & Stansbury, 2003).
  • Interrater reliability was calculated based on randomly double coding 20 percent of the sample across coders.
  • The average intraclass correlation among coders was .89 across all contexts of observation.
  • An overall infant negative affect score was calculated for each observational context as the percentage of 5-s intervals during which the child was rated as 2 or higher in negative affect.

4.3.2. Maternal cardiac data

  • Cardiac data were collected from mothers for each observational context.
  • A data file containing the heart interbeat intervals (IBIs) for the entire period of collection was transferred to a computer for editing artifacts that result from excess movement.
  • During the home assessment, heart rate data were collected for a 2-min baseline measure as well as during the free play session.
  • Missing data due to high levels of artifact editing or equipment malfunction accounted for approximately 25 percent of cases from each observational context (data analytic procedures described below allow for the inclusion of missing data in the current analyses).
  • The difference in vagal tone from baseline provides a measure of vagal withdrawal during each task.

4.3.3. Maternal sensitivity

  • Parenting during each interactive context was coded using 5-point rating scales adapted from Egeland and Hiester (1995) and the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1997).
  • A global sensitivity scale rated mother’s responses to the child’s gestures, facial expressions, and signals as she responded to the child’s emotional and physical needs.
  • The average intraclass correlation across coders was .81.
  • An overall sensitivity composite was constructed by summing each mother’s scores on these three codes.

4.3.4. Attachment security

  • Patterns of child behaviors observed during the Strange Situation Paradigm are used to classify children into the following three broad categories: secure, insecure-avoidant, and insecureresistant based on the procedures outlined by Ainsworth et al. (1978).
  • Avoidant children are distinguished by their conspicuous avoidance and reticence to engage their mother upon reunion, regardless of their level of distress upon separation.
  • Two coders trained and certified by the Sroufe attachment group coded videotapes for attachment quality.
  • Cohen’s kappa for these coders was k = .85 for 30 percent of the full sample.

5. RESULTS

  • The first section provides descriptive statistics and correlations among model parameters used in the current analyses.
  • The second section examines differences in model parameters across contexts of observation and across attachment classifications.
  • The third section uses hierarchical linear modeling to examine the association between infant negative affect and maternal sensitivity for different attachment dyads across multiple observations of parent-child interaction.
  • Additional analysis expands this model to test for the mediating or moderating effects of maternal vagal withdrawal on the relationship between infant negativity and maternal sensitivity.

5.1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among covariates

  • Means and standard deviations for model parameters in each observational context are presented in Table 1 (variables were standardized within each context of observation).
  • Correlations were examined between model parameters and demographic factors.
  • Within each observational context there were limited correlations among covariates.
  • In the book reading context maternal sensitivity was negatively correlated with infant negative affect.
  • Mother’s vagal withdrawal during book reading was positively correlated with vagal withdrawal during challenge.

5.2.1. Observational contexts

  • Differences in model parameters across observational contexts were examined using one-way ANOVAs.
  • Maternal sensitivity differed significantly across contexts [F(2, 423) = 21.27,p <.001] with post hoc analyses revealing maternal sensitivity to be higher in the contexts of book reading and challenge as compared to free play.
  • Infant negative affect differed across contexts [F(2, 259)=48.46,p<.001] with post hoc analyses revealing negativity to be significantly higher during challenge than either free play or book reading.
  • Vagal withdrawal was also significantly different across contexts [F(2, 428) =15.27, p <.001] with post hoc analyses revealing maternal withdrawal to be lower during book reading than free play or challenge.

5.2.2. Attachment classifications

  • Differences in model parameters across attachment classifications were similarly examined using one-way ANOVAs.
  • Maternal sensitivity was observed to be significantly different across attachment classifications [F(2, 367) = 6.19, p<.01] with post hoc analyses revealing sensitivity to be higher among mothers of securely attached children as compared to mothers of children with avoidant attachments.
  • Infant negative affect was also observed to be different across attachment classifications [F(2, 367) = 3.52, p <.05] with post hoc analyses revealing children.

5.3. Maternal sensitivity, infant negative affect and mother’s vagal withdrawal across

  • Attachment classifications Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) was used to examine differences in maternal sensitivity across contexts and across attachment classifications.
  • HLM is ideally suited to accommodate the features of the current data set, including both the nested nature of the observations within the parent-child dyads and the need for statistical power to test complex interactions within a limited sample size (Littell, Milliken, Stroup, & Wolfinger, 1996).
  • Models were constructed to examine the associations between infant negativity and maternal sensitivity across attachment classifications.
  • No such interactions were observed, suggesting that these effects were constant across ethnicity and income.
  • These analyses revealed that vagal withdrawal does moderate the association of infant negative affect with maternal sensitivity at 6 months, but only for mothers of children who were avoidant at 12 months [F(1, 25) = 5.05, p <.05; Fig. 1].

6. DISCUSSION

  • As part of the broader caregiving system, sensitive parenting must be considered as not just a trait characteristic of the mother, but rather a fluid array of behaviors that responds to the changing physical and emotional needs of her infant.
  • So the authors expected that mothers of later securely attached children would maintain high levels of sensitivity despite varying levels of infant negativity, while mothers of later insecurely attached children would display decreases in sensitivity in the face of high infant negative affect.
  • Because the relevance of vagal withdrawal is limited to mothers of avoidant children, this suggests that these mothers are uniquely challenged by elevated infant negativity.
  • Furthermore, it is important to remember that the context for the organization of parenting behavior is not limited to exogenous factors, but also includes the endogenous emotional and cognitive background of the parent.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Infant and parent factors associated with early maternal sensitivity: A caregiver-
attachment systems approach
By W. Roger Mills-Koonce, Jean-Louis Gari´epy, Cathi Propper, Kelly Suttona, Susan Calkins,
Ginger Moore, Martha Cox
Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gariépy, J.-L., Propper, C. B., Sutton, K., Calkins, S., Moore, G., & Cox,
M. J. (2007). Infant and parent factors associated with early maternal sensitivity: A
caregiver-attachment systems approach. Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 114-126.
Made available courtesy of Elsevier:
http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/620197/description#description
***Note: Figures may be missing from this format of the document
***Note: Footnotes and endnotes indicated with parentheses
Abstract:
We examined variations in maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child age as a function of child
negativity and maternal physiology. We expected maternal vagal withdrawal in response to
infant negative affect to facilitate the maintenance of sensitivity, but only for mothers of securely
attached children. One hundred and forty-eight infant-mother dyads were observed in multiple
contexts at 6 months of child age, and associations among maternal and child variables were
examined with respect to 12-month attachment quality. Mothers of later securely attached
children were more sensitive than mothers of avoidant children. However, sensitivity decreased
for all mothers at high levels of infant negative affect. Furthermore, for mothers of avoidant
children, vagal withdrawal was associated with sensitivity to child distress. No association was
found between vagal withdrawal and sensitivity for mothers of securely attached children. This
suggests that mothers of avoidant children may be uniquely challenged by the affective demands
of their infants. © 2007 Published by Elsevier Inc. Keywords: Infant; Parent; Caregiver
Article:
The conception of parental sensitivity as a stable personality attribute that directly benefits
attachment security has been challenged by meta-analyses (De Wolff & van IJzendoorn,1997)
identifying child factors as important contributors to attachment formation (Goldsmith &
Alansky, 1987) and showing that the relation between maternal sensitivity and attachment
security is not as strong as initially proposed by Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978).
Two reports by Crockenberg (1981) and van den Boom (1994) that highly irritable infants tend
to receive less sensitive care and are at greater risk for developing insecure relationships to their
caregiver were especially compelling in this regard. Their findings suggest that sensitive
parenting might be challenged and reduced by infant negativity, but enhanced when the child
positively stimulates the mother and responds to her bids for interaction (see also Atkinson et al.,
1999; Cox, Owen, Henderson, & Margand, 1992; Kochanska, 2001; Thompson, 1997; van den
Boom, 1997).
In this light, both van den Boom (1997) and Thompson (1997) have pressed for amore
dyadic/contingent concept of caregiver sensitivity that takes into account the infant’s changing
affective state. Additionally, the notion that there may be caregiver regulatory qualities that, via

their effects on sensitivity, promote or compromise security of attachment (van IJzendoorn,
1995) has also gained visibility in attachment research (Adam, Gunnar, & Tanaka, 2004;
Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Pederson, Gleason, Moran, & Bento, 1998; Pedersen &
Moran, 1995). In this study, we examine how maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child age varies
as a function of child distress and the mother’s ability to physiologically self-regulate in response
to this distress.
1. CHILD FACTORS, MATERNAL SENSITIVITY, AND ATTACHMENT SECURITY
Bowlby’s (1969) attachment theory highlighted the importance of shifts between positive and
negative affect as the primary mechanism of communication for infants. Accordingly, while
positive affect promotes affiliation and the acquisition of competence through play and
exploration, shifts to negative affect serve as potent signals of distress and the need for comfort
from the caregiver. The caregiving system must adequately detect and attend to these shifts in
order to support the development of a secure attachment relationship. The coordination of
caregiving behaviors with the infant attachment system is a benchmark of sensitive parenting,
which is exemplified by child-centered responses to the physical and emotional needs of the
infant (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1969). Repeated experience with this type of caregiving
allows the child to develop a sense of efficacy and agency and to use the full repertoire of
emotional communication in a well-regulated manner (Tronick,1989). The fact that the
caregiving system must respond appropriately to a variety of emotional needs from the child
highlights the relevance of examining parenting across a spectrum of interactive contexts.
Although experiencing sensitive caregiving is important for children in all contexts, experiencing
effective soothing and comfort when distressed may be particularly important for the
development of self-regulation. In contrast, the inability to rely on the caregiver for comfort
when distressed may lead the child to develop coping strategies, such as avoidance, that in the
long term are maladaptive. Several researchers, such as Thompson (1997), have proposed that
―sensitivity expressed when the child is fearful, anxious or distressed might be more prognostic
of a secure attachment than sensitivity displayed during nonstressful episodes . . . ‖ (p. 596).
Although several studies have examined the relationships between infant affect, maternal
sensitivity, and attachment security (Braungart & Stifter, 1991; Frodi & Thompson, 1985;
Mangelsdorf, Gunnar, Kestenbaum, Lang, & Andreas, 1990; Thompson & Lamb, 1984), to date
only a few longitudinal studies explicitly considered early child influences on maternal
sensitivity and later attachment classification (Braungart-Reiker, Garwood, Powers, & Wang,
2001; Posada et al., 1999). Although these studies identify independent effects of early maternal
sensitivity and infant affective displays on later attachment classification, they also highlight the
difficulty of establishing clear causal links between maternal and infant behaviors. In spite of
these limitations, there is much to be gained by examining how these behaviors are coordinated,
especially with respect to the dyad’s emerging attachment relationship.
For the purpose of the present research, we measured maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child
age under three different conditions of increasing stress for the dyad. We expected that these
conditions would differ in terms of the probability of observing child negative affect. These
conditions included a free play session, a book reading session, and the recovery period that
followed two challenge procedures known to elicit infant distress. Based on previous findings
that mothers of insecurely attached children (especially of avoidant children; Cassidy, 1999)
have more difficulties with their own emotion regulation and parental function under conditions

of infant distress than mothers of secure children, we hypothesized that mothers of children later
categorized as secure would be better able to maintain sensitivity under conditions of infant
distress than mothers from insecure dyads.
2. MOTHER’S SELF-REGULATION IN RESPONSE TO INFANT DISTRESS
Bowlby (1969) posited that negative affect is the most potent tool of communication available to
the infant because of its distinctness and saliency for the mother in almost any situation.
Although being aroused by a crying infant is certainly an adaptive feature of the caregiving
system, infant distress can also be quite dysregulating for mothers, especially when persistent
and intense. Consider the current definitions of parental sensitivity, including attentiveness and
awareness of child signaling, emotional availability, consistency, appropriate emotional tone, and
lack of irritation and anger (Ainsworth et al., 1978; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network,
1999; Posada et al., 1999; van den Boom, 1997). Each of these descriptive aspects requires some
degree of behavioral self-regulatory capabilities on the part of the mother. Given this, our second
goal was to determine whether physiological measures of maternal self-regulation were
associated with sensitive caregiving in response to changes in the infant’s affective state.
In measuring maternal emotion regulation it was important to consider that differential maternal
responding to infant distress signals may be associated with and supported by differences in
maternal reactivity and self-regulation, both of which have known biological bases in the
autonomic and central nervous systems (Rothbath & Derryberry,1981). Thus, the quality of a
mother’s response to her child’s needs may depend in part on the degree to which her
physiological stress regulation supports active attention and engagement with the needs of the
child. There is ample evidence from the animal literature that such physiological support for
organized parenting behavior is necessary (e.g., Boccia & Pederson, 2001; Porges, 1998;
Winslow et al., 2000; Wotjak et al., 1996). Of special relevance to this topic is the demonstration
by Porges (1998) that mammalian pair-bonding is supported by the vagal system, which
physiologically mediates social engagement, facial expressivity, and vocalization. Thus, as part
of the broader caregiving system, maternal vagal regulation in response to infant distress signals
may provide a psychophysiological support system for the organization of effective parenting
behaviors.
One commonly used index of vagal regulation and autonomic functioning is heart-rate
variability, a measure of parasympathetic activity. Heart-rate variability, or vagal tone, has been
linked to several measures of cognitive and emotional processes involving attention and stimulus
appraisal (Lacey & Lacey, 1958; Porges, Arnold, & Forbes, 1973; Richards, 1987; Wenger,
1941). Similarly, a decrease of vagal tone during periods of environmental challenge has been
linked to self-regulation and the ability to engage the source of stimulation or take distance from
it (Calkins, 1997; Kagan, 1994; Porges, 1996, 1998; Stifter & Fox, 1990; Stifter & Jain, 1996).
Interestingly, polyvagal theory (Porges, 1995) suggests that somatomotor and visceromotor
nerve fibers that innervate organs such as the larynx, pharynx, bronchi, and heart act
synergistically to promote social interaction. Porges explains that under most conditions, heart
rate is kept low and heart-rate variability high via the nucleus ambiguous serving as a vagal
―brake‖ in order to curb the use of psychophysiological resources. However, under challenging
conditions, this ―brake‖ is released and the resulting decrease in heart-rate variability and
increase in cardiac output facilitates active engagement and monitoring with respect to a set goal.
Although there are multiple methods for measuring this change, Porges (1985, 1991, 1996) has

developed a commonly used method that indexes heart-rate variability at the frequency of
breathing, known as respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA). A decrease in RSA from baseline levels
(i.e., vagal withdrawal) is believed to reflect the parasympathetic influence of vagal regulation,
whereby the withdrawal of parasympathetic control of the heart allows for sympathetic activity
and thus increases in heart rate and cardiac output. As such, effective vagal withdrawal has been
examined as an index of self-regulation and may underlie the behavioral attention strategies
necessary for regulation of arousal (Huffman et al., 1998; Posner & Rothbart, 2000). Although
most studies of vagal withdrawal and behavior focus on children, there is both theoretical and
empirical evidence to suggest that similar patterns of vagal-behavior associations exist among
adults (Beauchaine, 2001). As vagal withdrawal facilitates the flexible shift of resources from a
stable state to one that can meet environmental demands, we expected elevated withdrawal to be
associated with the maintenance of sensitive care even under conditions of high child distress.
Furthermore, we hypothesized this relationship to be particularly characteristic of mothers whose
infants would later show secure attachments.
3. HYPOTHESES
The goals of the current research were (1) to determine whether associations between child
negative affect and maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child age were constant across later
attachment classifications, and (2) to determine whether mother’s physiological regulation, as
indexed by vagal withdrawal, mediates or moderates associations between infant negativity and
maternal sensitivity. Specifically, the first hypothesis predicted that infant negative affect would
be more strongly associated with maternal sensitivity among mothers of insecurely attached
children. In contrast, we expected little to no measurable effects of infant negativity on maternal
sensitivity among securely attached dyads. For our second hypothesis, we predicted that for
mothers of secure children, high maternal vagal withdrawal would be associated with the
maintenance of sensitivity when mothers were faced with high levels of infant negative affect. In
contrast, low levels of vagal withdrawal in response to elevated infant negativity were expected
to be associated with less sensitive care among mothers of insecurely attached children.
The current analyses are limited to dyads with organized attachment relationships at 12 months
of age. Although mothers of children with a disorganized primary classification are an interesting
subgroup, there is no conceptual basis for including them in the present analyses. Specifically,
several studies have shown that these mothers are as sensitive as mothers with children in other
attachment classifications, secure or otherwise (van IJzendoorn, Schuengel,
& Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1999). Rather, disorganized attachments have been linked to
maternal behaviors such as disruptive affective communication (Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, &
Parsons, 1999), overt familial risks such as maltreatment (Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, &
Braunwald, 1989), or other broad environmental factors such as the death of a caregiver (van
IJzendoorn,1995). In fact, within disorganized samples the forced ABC classification of children
is often differentiated by maternal sensitivity, with forced secure children receiving sensitive
care to a similar degree as primarily secure children (Mills-Koonce, Gariepy, Sutton, & Cox,
under review). Given these findings, the current research focuses on mothers of children with
secure, insecure-avoidant, and insecure-resistant attachment classifications.
4. METHOD
4.1. Participants
The participants in the current study were 173 families recruited by the Durham Child Health

and Development Study. For this study, recruitment procedures specified an approximately equal
number of European- and African- American families sampled from both lower- and higher-
income groups. The family’s race was determined by the race of the mother (or primary
caregiver), while income status was determined by whether families were above or below 200
percent of the federally established poverty threshold. Birth order or family structures were not
used as inclusion criteria. Families were drawn from a largely urban community via fliers and
postings at birth and parenting classes, as well as through phone contact via birth records. There
was a 10 percent rate of attrition from the first to the second time point, resulting in a total
sample of 155 families that completed all assessment protocols. Of those, 7 families were
excluded due to audio-visual difficulties that made their videotaped assessments impossible to
code. The final sample of 148 families used for the current analyses was 56 percent African-
American and 44 percent European-American. Approximately 53 percent was low income
(below 200 percent of the poverty level). A total of 13 percent of mothers had no high school
degree, 43 percent had either a high school diploma or a G.E.D., 11 percent had some college or
vocational school, and 33 percent had a 4-year bachelors degree or higher. The ages of mothers
in this sample ranged from 18 to 40 (M= 28.3, SD = 5.6). In addition to the focal child, 69
families in this sample had additional children in the household. When present, the number of
additional children in the household ranged from 1 to 10 (M=1.6, SD = 2.7). All siblings were
older than the focal child, ranging from 1.5 to 13 years of age (M= 5.0, SD = 2.7). The sample
was almost evenly split based on the sex of the child, with 51 percent male. The distribution of
12-month attachment classifications in the present sample consisted of 96 children classified as
secure, 37 children classified as insecure-avoidant, and 15 classified as insecure-resistant.
4.2. Assessment Procedures
4.2.1. 6-Month free play observations
Mothers and children were observed in the home for the free play session, during which time
mothers were asked to interact with their children as they normally would during a typical day. A
standard set of toys was provided for the mother and child to use, and the pair was asked to sit on
a blanket that was laid out across the floor. This session was videotaped for later coding, with
researchers monitoring the camera discreetly to minimize interference with the ongoing
interaction.
4.2.2. 6-Month book reading observations
Within 2 weeks of the home visit, mothers and children participated in a 6-month laboratory
visit. During this time mothers were asked to try to involve their child with a picture book
provided by the researchers. The book involved pictures without words to control for any effect
of illiteracy among the parents. Mothers were responsible for creating a storyline that followed
the illustrations in the book. Like the free play session in the home visit, this procedure took
place on the floor with the child positioned between the mother and a single camera. Mothers
were allowed up to 10 min for this activity.
4.2.3. 6-Month challenge tasks observations
The challenge tasks administered in the laboratory visit were the Still Face Paradigm (Tronick,
Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1979) and the arms restraint procedure (Goldsmith &
Rothbarth, 1994). For these purposes the mother was asked to secure her child in a car seat
situated on top of a large, sturdy table. Prior to the still face episode, the mother was instructed to

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Evidence for bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and children's characteristics of frustration, fear, self-regulation, and impulsivity was reviewed, and an overall model of children’s individual differences in response to parenting is proposed.
Abstract: Accounting for both bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and child temperament can fine-tune theoretical models of the role of parenting and temperament in children’s development of adjustment problems. Evidence for bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and children’s characteristics of frustration, fear, self-regulation, and impulsivity was reviewed, and an overall model of children’s individual differences in response to parenting is proposed. In general, children high in frustration, impulsivity and low in effortful control are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of negative parenting, while in turn, many negative parenting behaviors predict increases in these characteristics. Frustration, fearfulness, and effortful control also appear to elicit parenting behaviors that can predict increases in these characteristics. Irritability renders children more susceptible to negative parenting behaviors. Fearfulness operates in a very complex manner, sometimes increasing children’s responses to parenting behaviors and sometimes mitigating them and apparently operating differently across gender. Important directions for future research include the use of study designs and analytic approaches that account for the direction of effects and for developmental changes in parenting and temperament over time.

449 citations


Cites background from "Infant and parent factors associate..."

  • ...Mixed findings have been obtained when difficult temperament is considered a predictor of responsive or sensitive parenting, with some studies showing an evocative role for temperament (Feldman et al. 1997; Mills-Koonce et al. 2007) but others not (Clark et al. 2000)....

    [...]

01 Jan 1997
TL;DR: Both genetic and environmental factors were found to influence the association between children’s school readiness and later school achievement, and should be seen as a further incentive for continued implementation and evaluation of preventive intervention programs aimed at improving the level of school readiness in children from at-risk families.
Abstract: Early school success seems to depend largely on children entering school ready to learn, and many policy initiatives have highlighted the importance of preparing children for school entry. A new study finds that children’s environment plays a major role in their readiness for school, suggesting that intervention could help boost readiness in at-risk youngsters. The study, conducted by researchers at Laval University, the University of Montreal, and the University of Quebec at Montreal, appears in the November/December 2007 issue of the journal Child Development. It is one of the first studies to consider both environmental and genetic influences on children’s readiness for school. The researchers examined 420 pairs of 5-year-old twins, assessing the children on four measures of school readiness that included identifying colors and shapes; answering questions about spatial position (such as above, below, left, right), relative size (such as smaller, bigger), and order (such as first, middle, last); identifying numbers and counting; and identifying letters and writing. Two years later, the children’s teachers were asked to rate the school achievement of 237 pairs of the twins. Environmental factors shared by twins in the same family–such as family resources and income, parents’ behavior with respect to learning, and the twins’ child care experiences–were responsible for much of the individual difference in the children’s school readiness skills, according to the study. The influence of the environmental factors was seen over and above the influence of genetic factors. These shared factors influenced school readiness in both general and specific ways, that is, they were found to be significant for each component of school readiness, as well for the core abilities underlying overall school readiness. Genetic factors played a significant role in the children’s core abilities underlying the four components of school readiness, but the environment shared by twins of the same family remained the most important factor overall. Both genetic and environmental factors were found to influence the association between children’s school readiness and later school achievement. “Our results have important implications for preventive interventions,” said Michel Boivin, Canada Research Chair in Child Social Development and professor of psychology at Laval University in Quebec City and one of the study’s authors. “They should be seen as a further incentive for continued implementation and evaluation of preventive intervention programs aimed at improving the level of school readiness in children from at-risk families.”

447 citations

01 Jan 2011
TL;DR: In this paper, an overall model of children's individual differences in response to parenting is proposed, showing that children high in frustration, impulsivity and low in effortful control are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of negative parent- ing, while in turn many negative parenting behaviors predict increases in these characteristics.
Abstract: Accounting for both bidirectional and interac- tive effects between parenting and child temperament can fine-tune theoretical models of the role of parenting and temperament in children's development of adjustment problems. Evidence for bidirectional and interactive effects between parenting and children's characteristics of frus- tration, fear, self-regulation, and impulsivity was reviewed, and an overall model of children's individual differences in response to parenting is proposed. In general, children high in frustration, impulsivity and low in effortful control are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of negative parent- ing, while in turn, many negative parenting behaviors predict increases in these characteristics. Frustration, fear- fulness, and effortful control also appear to elicit parenting behaviors that can predict increases in these characteristics. Irritability renders children more susceptible to negative parenting behaviors. Fearfulness operates in a very com- plex manner, sometimes increasing children's responses to parenting behaviors and sometimes mitigating them and apparently operating differently across gender. Important directions for future research include the use of study designs and analytic approaches that account for the direction of effects and for developmental changes in parenting and temperament over time.

402 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is proposed that vulnerability to psychopathology in general is related to impairments in epistemic trust, leading to disruptions in the process of salutogenesis, the positive effects associated with the capacity to benefit from the social environment.
Abstract: A new developmental model of borderline personality disorder (BPD) and its treatment is advanced based on evolutionary considerations concerning the role of attachment, mentalizing, and epistemic trust in the development of psychopathology. We propose that vulnerability to psychopathology in general is related to impairments in epistemic trust, leading to disruptions in the process of salutogenesis, the positive effects associated with the capacity to benefit from the social environment. BPD is perhaps the disorder par excellence that illustrates this view. We argue that this conceptualization makes sense of the presence of both marked rigidity and instability in BPD, and has far-reaching implications for intervention.

283 citations


Cites background from "Infant and parent factors associate..."

  • ...…(e.g., Beebe et al., 2010) and more global (e.g., DeWolf & van IJzendoorn, 1997; Isabella, Belsky, & von Eye, 1989; Kiser, Bates, Maslin, & Bayles, 1986; Mills-Koonce et al., 2007) manifestations of sensitive caregiving can be seen as in essence acts of recognition of the child‘s agentive self....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Mothers' and infants' physiological responses may be a function of mutual responsiveness and parents' physiological regulation may support infants' regulation.
Abstract: Parents’ physiological regulation may support infants’ regulation. Mothers (N= 152) and 6-month-old male and female infants were observed in normal and disrupted social interaction. Affect was coded at 1-s intervals and vagal tone measured as respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA). Maternal sensitivity was assessed in free play. Mothers and infants showed opposite patterns of RSA change. During disrupted interaction, mothers’ RSA increased and infants’ decreased, suggesting self-regulation of distress. During reunion, although the typical pattern was for infants to return to baseline levels, infants of sensitive mothers and sensitive mothers both showed a significant decrease in RSA from baseline. Mothers’ and infants’ physiological responses may be a function of mutual responsiveness.

226 citations


Cites background from "Infant and parent factors associate..."

  • ...…inhibition or withdrawal of RSA during a challenging situation (typically measured as a decrease in RSA from baseline to a challenge task) is thought to reflect more effective regulation, although it may also simply indicate individual differences in what is experienced as aversive or challenging....

    [...]

  • ...…physiological stress reactivity, were rated as more negative with their infants only if they failed to show RSA withdrawal in a challenge task (Mills-Koonce et al., 2007), providing additional support that parents who can regulate their own physiological reactivity effectively may be better…...

    [...]

References
More filters
Book
01 Jan 1975
TL;DR: In this article, the Mathematical Basis for Multiple Regression/Correlation and Identification of the Inverse Matrix Elements is presented. But it does not address the problem of missing data.
Abstract: Contents: Preface. Introduction. Bivariate Correlation and Regression. Multiple Regression/Correlation With Two or More Independent Variables. Data Visualization, Exploration, and Assumption Checking: Diagnosing and Solving Regression Problems I. Data-Analytic Strategies Using Multiple Regression/Correlation. Quantitative Scales, Curvilinear Relationships, and Transformations. Interactions Among Continuous Variables. Categorical or Nominal Independent Variables. Interactions With Categorical Variables. Outliers and Multicollinearity: Diagnosing and Solving Regression Problems II. Missing Data. Multiple Regression/Correlation and Causal Models. Alternative Regression Models: Logistic, Poisson Regression, and the Generalized Linear Model. Random Coefficient Regression and Multilevel Models. Longitudinal Regression Methods. Multiple Dependent Variables: Set Correlation. Appendices: The Mathematical Basis for Multiple Regression/Correlation and Identification of the Inverse Matrix Elements. Determination of the Inverse Matrix and Applications Thereof.

29,764 citations

Book
01 Jan 1991
TL;DR: In this article, the effects of predictor scaling on the coefficients of regression equations are investigated. But, they focus mainly on the effect of predictors scaling on coefficients of regressions.
Abstract: Introduction Interactions between Continuous Predictors in Multiple Regression The Effects of Predictor Scaling on Coefficients of Regression Equations Testing and Probing Three-Way Interactions Structuring Regression Equations to Reflect Higher Order Relationships Model and Effect Testing with Higher Order Terms Interactions between Categorical and Continuous Variables Reliability and Statistical Power Conclusion Some Contrasts Between ANOVA and MR in Practice

27,897 citations

Book
01 Jan 1969

18,243 citations


"Infant and parent factors associate..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Bowlby (1969) suggested that the parent’s caregiving system must be in alignment with the needs of the child’s attachment system in order to facilitate the formation of a secure attachment relationship....

    [...]

  • ...Bowlby (1969) posited that negative affect is the most potent tool of communication available to the infant because of its distinctness and saliency for the mother in almost any situation....

    [...]

  • ...The coordination of caregiving behaviors with the infant attachment system is a benchmark of sensitive parenting, which is exemplified by child-centered responses to the physical and emotional needs of the infant (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1969)....

    [...]

Book
16 Jul 1996

9,086 citations

Journal ArticleDOI

7,023 citations


"Infant and parent factors associate..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...Procedures outlined by Aiken and West (1991) and Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003) were used to probe this interaction....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (1)
Q1. What are the contributions in this paper?

The authors examined variations in maternal sensitivity at 6 months of child age as a function of child negativity and maternal physiology. Furthermore, for mothers of avoidant children, vagal withdrawal was associated with sensitivity to child distress. This suggests that mothers of avoidant children may be uniquely challenged by the affective demands of their infants.