scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Book

Investing in Our Future: The Evidence Base on Preschool Education

About: The article was published on 2013-10-01 and is currently open access. It has received 512 citations till now.

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Investing in Our Future:
The Evidence Base on
Preschool Education
Hirokazu Yoshikawa, Christina Weiland, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Margaret R. Burchinal, Linda M. Espinosa,
William T. Gormley, Jens Ludwig, Katherine A. Magnuson, Deborah Phillips, Martha J. Zaslow
O C T O B E R 2 0 1 3

Investing in Our Future:
The Evidence Base on
Preschool Education
Hirokazu Yoshikawa, Christina Weiland, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Margaret R. Burchinal, Linda M. Espinosa,
William T. Gormley, Jens Ludwig, Katherine A. Magnuson, Deborah Phillips, Martha J. Zaslow
*
* After the first two primary authors, the authors are listed alphabetically. The authors thank Deborah Phillips and the Foundation for Child Development for
funding this work. The authors would also like to thank those who provided helpful reviews: J. Lawrence Aber, Mary Catherine Arbour, Karen Bierman, Maia
Connors, Greg Duncan, Philip Fisher, Ruth Friedman, Eugene Garcia, Ron Haskins, Jacqueline Jones, Laura Justice, Nonie Lesaux, Joan Lombardi, Pamela
Morris, Adele Robinson, Jack Shonkoff, Catherine Tamis-LeMonda, Elizabeth Votruba-Drzal, and Jane Waldfogel.
Hirokazu Yoshikawa, New York University
Christina Weiland, University of Michigan
Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Columbia University
Margaret R. Burchinal, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, University of North Carolina
Linda M. Espinosa, University of Missouri, Columbia
William T. Gormley, Georgetown University
Jens Ludwig, University of Chicago
Katherine A. Magnuson, University of Wisconsin, Madison
Deborah Phillips, Georgetown University
Martha J. Zaslow, Society for Research in Child Development and Child Trends
You can find this report at
http://fcd-us.org/resources/evidence-base-preschool
http://www.srcd.org/policy-media/policy-updates/meetings-briefings/investing-our-future-
evidence-base-preschool

Investing in Our Future: The Evidence Base on Preschool Education 1
Executive Summary
Large-scale public preschool programs can have substantial impacts on children’s
early learning. Scientific evidence on the impacts of early childhood education has
progressed well beyond exclusive reliance on the Perry Preschool and Abecedarian
programs. A recent analysis integrating evaluations of 84 preschool programs concluded
that, on average, children gain about a third of a year of additional learning across language,
reading, and math skills. At-scale preschool systems in Tulsa and Boston have produced
larger gains of between a half and a full year of additional learning in reading and math.
Benefits to children’s socio-emotional development and health have been documented in
programs that focus intensively on these areas.
Quality preschool education is a profitable investment. Rigorous efforts to estimate
whether the economic benefits of early childhood education outweigh the costs of providing
these educational opportunities indicate that they are a wise financial investment. Available
benefit-cost estimates based on older, intensive interventions, such as the Perry Preschool
Program, as well as contemporary, large-scale public preschool programs, such as the
Chicago Child-Parent Centers and Tulsa’s preschool program, range from three to seven
dollars saved for every dollar spent.
The most important aspects of quality in preschool education are stimulating and
supportive interactions between teachers and children and effective use of curricula.
Children benefit most when teachers engage in stimulating interactions that support
learning and are emotionally supportive. Interactions that help children acquire new
knowledge and skills provide input to children, elicit verbal responses and reactions from
them, and foster engagement in and enjoyment of learning. Recent evaluations tell us that
effective use of curricula focused on such specific aspects of learning as language and
literacy, math, or socio-emotional development provide a substantial boost to childrens
learning. Guidelines about the number of children in a classroom, the ratio of teachers and
children, and staff qualifications help to increase the likelihood of—but do not assure—
supportive and stimulating interactions. Importantly, in existing large-scale studies, only a
minority of preschool programs are observed to provide excellent quality and levels of
instructional support are especially low.
Supporting teachers in their implementation of instructional approaches through
coaching or mentoring can yield important benefits for children. Coaching or
mentoring that provides support to the teacher on how to implement content-rich and
engaging curricula shows substantial promise in helping to assure that such instruction
is being provided. Such coaching or mentoring involves modeling positive instructional
approaches and providing feedback on the teacher’s implementation in a way that sets goals
but is also supportive. This can occur either directly in the classroom or though web-based
exchange of video clips.

2 Investing in Our Future: The Evidence Base on Preschool Education
Quality preschool education can benefit middle-class children as well as disadvantaged
children; typically developing children as well as children with special needs; and dual
language learners as well as native speakers. Although early research focused only on
programs for low-income children, more recent research focusing on universal preschool
programs provides the opportunity to ask if preschool can benefit children from middle-
income as well as low-income families. The evidence is clear that middle-class children can
benefit substantially, and that benefits outweigh costs for children from middle-income as
well as those from low-income families. However, children from low-income backgrounds
benefit more. Children with special needs who attended Tulsa’s preschool program showed
comparable improvements in reading and pre-writing skills as typically developing children.
Further, at the end of first grade, children with special needs who had attended Head Start
as 3-year-olds showed stronger gains in math and social-emotional development than
children with special needs who had not attended Head Start. Studies of both Head Start
and public preschool programs suggest that dual language learners benefit as much as, and
in some cases more than, their native speaker counterparts.
A second year of preschool shows additional benefits. The available studies, which focus
on disadvantaged children, show further benefits from a second year of preschool. However,
the gains are not always as large as from the first year of preschool. This may be because
children who attend two years of preschool are not experiencing a sequential building of
instruction from the first to the second year.
Long-term benefits occur despite convergence of test scores. As children from
low-income families in preschool evaluation studies are followed into elementary school,
differences between those who received preschool and those who did not on tests of
academic achievement are reduced. However, evidence from long-term evaluations of
both small-scale, intensive interventions and Head Start suggest that there are long-term
effects on important societal outcomes such as high-school graduation, years of education
completed, earnings, and reduced crime and teen pregnancy, even after test-score effects
decline to zero. Research is now underway focusing on why these long-term effects occur
even when test scores converge.
There are important benefits of comprehensive services when these added services
are carefully chosen and targeted. When early education provides comprehensive
services, it is important that these extensions of the program target services and practices
that show benefits to children and families. Early education programs that have focused in
a targeted way on health outcomes (e.g., connecting children to a regular medical home;
integrating comprehensive screening; requiring immunizations) have shown such benefits as
an increase in receipt of primary medical care and dental care. In addition, a parenting focus
can augment the effects of preschool on children’s skill development, but only if it provides
parents with modeling of positive interactions or opportunities for practice with feedback.
Simply providing information through classes or workshops is not associated with further
improvements in children’s skills.

Investing in Our Future: The Evidence Base on Preschool Education 3
Investing in Our Future:
The Evidence Base on
Preschool Education
Hirokazu Yoshikawa, Christina Weiland, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Margaret R. Burchinal, Linda M. Espinosa,
William T. Gormley, Jens Ludwig, Katherine A. Magnuson, Deborah Phillips, Martha J. Zaslow
**
The expansion of publicly-funded preschool education is currently the focus of a prominent
debate. At present, 42% of 4-year-olds attend publicly funded preschool (28% attend
public prekindergarten programs, 11% Head Start, and 3% special education preschool
programs).
1
A vigorous debate about the merits of preschool education is underway,
although at times it has not included the most recent available evidence. The goal of this
brief is to provide a non-partisan, thorough, and up-to-date review of the current science
and evidence base on early childhood education (ECE). Our interdisciplinary group of early
childhood experts reviewed rigorous evidence on why early skills matter, the short- and
long-term effects of preschool programs on children’s school readiness and life outcomes,
the importance of program quality, which children benefit from preschool (including
evidence on children from different family income backgrounds), and the costs versus
benefits of preschool education. We focus on preschool (early childhood education) for
four-year-olds, with some review of the evidence for three-year-olds when relevant. We
do not discuss evidence regarding programs for 0 – 3 year olds.
Early skills matter,
and preschool can help children build these skills.
The foundations of brain architecture, and subsequent lifelong developmental potential, are
laid down in a child’s early years through a process that is exquisitely sensitive to external
influence. Early experiences in the home, in other care settings, and in communities interact
with genes to shape the developing nature and quality of the brain’s architecture. The
growth and then environmentally based pruning of neuronal systems in the first years
support a range of early skills, including cognitive (early language, literacy, math), social
(theory of mind, empathy, prosocial), persistence, attention, and self-regulation and
**After the first two primary authors, the authors are listed alphabetically. The authors thank Deborah Phillips and the Foundation for Child Development for
funding this work. The authors would also like to thank those who provided helpful reviews: J. Lawrence Aber, Mary Catherine Arbour, Karen Bierman, Maia
Connors, Greg Duncan, Philip Fisher, Ruth Friedman, Eugene Garcia, Ron Haskins, Jacqueline Jones, Laura Justice, Nonie Lesaux, Joan Lombardi, Pamela
Morris, Adele Robinson, Jack Shonkoff, Catherine Tamis-LeMonda, Elizabeth Votruba-Drzal, and Jane Waldfogel.

Citations
More filters
Journal Article
TL;DR: The National Academy of Sciences founded The National Academies Press (NAP) with the goal of publishing reports of all four national academies as mentioned in this paper, which publishes more than 200 books from the fields of science, engineering and medicine and offers more than 4000 titles in PDF on its website.
Abstract: The National Academy of Sciences founded The National Academies Press (NAP) with the goal of publishing reports of all four national academies. Annually, NAP publishes more than 200 books from the fields of science, engineering and medicine and offers more than 4000 titles in PDF on its website (http://www.nap.edu/) free of charge.

1,241 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The authors compared public school kindergarten classrooms between 1998 and 2010 using two large, nationally representative data sets and showed substantial changes in each of the five dimensions considered: kindergarten teachers' beliefs about school readiness, time spent on academic and non-academic content, classroom organization, pedagogical approach, and use of standardized assessments.
Abstract: Recent accounts suggest that accountability pressures have trickled down into the early elementary grades and that kindergarten today is characterized by a heightened focus on academic skills and a reduction in opportunities for play. This paper compares public school kindergarten classrooms between 1998 and 2010 using two large, nationally representative data sets. We show substantial changes in each of the five dimensions considered: kindergarten teachers’ beliefs about school readiness, time spent on academic and nonacademic content, classroom organization, pedagogical approach, and use of standardized assessments. Kindergarten teachers in the later period held far higher academic expectations for children both prior to kindergarten entry and during the kindergarten year. They devoted more time to advanced literacy and math content, teacher-directed instruction, and assessment and substantially less time to art, music, science, and child-selected activities.

417 citations


Cites background from "Investing in Our Future: The Eviden..."

  • ...…early childhood learning experiences can meaningfully influence their short- and longer-term life outcomes (Barnett, 1995; Chetty et al., 2011; Yoshikawa et al., 2013), it is less clear precisely what aspects of the early learning environment (e.g., curricular focus, pedagogical approach)…...

    [...]

  • ...Although there is growing consensus that children’s early childhood learning experiences can meaningfully influence their short- and longer-term life outcomes (Barnett, 1995; Chetty et al., 2011; Yoshikawa et al., 2013), it is less clear precisely what aspects of the early learning environment (e....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
Abstract: High-quality early care and education (ECE) programs are thought to increase opportunities for all children to succeed in school, but recent findings call into question whether these programs affect children as anticipated. In this article, I examine research relating the quality of ECE to children's outcomes, finding somewhat inconsistent and modest associations with widely used measures of process and structural quality, and more consistent and stronger associations with other dimensions of ECE such as curricula and type of ECE program. I discuss why the associations between ECE quality and outcomes are so modest, including limited children's outcomes, psychometric issues with quality measures, and a need to revise and expand measures of ECE quality. The evidence indicates that we need to focus on the content of instruction and teaching practices, as well as the extent to which teachers actively scaffold learning opportunities. We also need to continue to focus on the quality of interactions between teachers and children, and on children's access to age-appropriate activities.

203 citations


Cites background from "Investing in Our Future: The Eviden..."

  • ...50) for dual-language learners and children from low-income families (1)....

    [...]

  • ...Researchers have used this model extensively to examine the association between ECE quality and children’s outcomes, and their findings have contributed to funding public ECE programs and policies (1, 9)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors assessed impacts on classroom quality and on 5 child language and behavioral outcomes of a 2-year teacher professional-development program for publicly funded prekindergarten and kindergarten in Chile.
Abstract: We assessed impacts on classroom quality and on 5 child language and behavioral outcomes of a 2-year teacher professional-development program for publicly funded prekindergarten and kindergarten in Chile. This cluster-randomized trial included 64 schools (child N = 1,876). The program incorporated workshops and in-classroom coaching. We found moderate to large positive impacts on observed emotional and instructional support as well as classroom organization in prekindergarten classrooms after 1 year of the program. After 2 years of the program, moderate positive impacts were observed on emotional support and classroom organization. No significant program impacts on child outcomes were detected at posttest (1 marginal effect, an increase in a composite of self-regulation and low problem behaviors, was observed). Professional development for preschool teachers in Chile can improve classroom quality. More intensive curricular approaches are needed for these improvements to translate into effects on children.

202 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The findings support both construct and predictive validity of the teaching through interactions conceptual framework as assessed by the Classroom Assessment Scoring System in Chile.
Abstract: Quality of teacher–child interactions is central to prekindergarten children's learning. In the United States, the quality of teacher–child interactions is commonly assessed using the teaching through interactions conceptual framework and an associ/ated observational tool, the Classroom Assessment Scoring System (CLASS). This study examined: (a) whether there was evidence supporting three distinctive domains of teacher–child interactions in Chile (construct validity) and (b) whether these domains predicted end-of-prekindergarten language, academic, and executive function skills in Chile (predictive validity). The sample consisted of 91 Chilean prekindergarten classrooms (1,868 four-year-old children). The findings support both construct and predictive validity of the teaching through interactions conceptual framework as assessed by the CLASS in Chile. Implications for cross-country comparison of quality of teacher–child interactions in prekindergarten classrooms are discussed.

199 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A meta-analysis of the results shows that early math skills have the greatest predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills, while measures of socioemotional behaviors were generally insignificant predictors of later academic performance.
Abstract: Using 6 longitudinal data sets, the authors estimate links between three key elements of school readiness--school-entry academic, attention, and socioemotional skills--and later school reading and math achievement In an effort to isolate the effects of these school-entry skills, the authors ensured that most of their regression models control for cognitive, attention, and socioemotional skills measured prior to school entry, as well as a host of family background measures Across all 6 studies, the strongest predictors of later achievement are school-entry math, reading, and attention skills A meta-analysis of the results shows that early math skills have the greatest predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills By contrast, measures of socioemotional behaviors, including internalizing and externalizing problems and social skills, were generally insignificant predictors of later academic performance, even among children with relatively high levels of problem behavior Patterns of association were similar for boys and girls and for children from high and low socioeconomic backgrounds

4,384 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The collection of implementation data is an essential feature of program evaluations, and more information is needed on which and how various factors influence implementation in different community settings.
Abstract: The first purpose of this review was to assess the impact of implementation on program outcomes, and the second purpose was to identify factors affecting the implementation process. Results from over quantitative 500 studies offered strong empirical support to the conclusion that the level of implementation affects the outcomes obtained in promotion and prevention programs. Findings from 81 additional reports indicate there are at least 23 contextual factors that influence implementation. The implementation process is affected by variables related to communities, providers and innovations, and aspects of the prevention delivery system (i.e., organizational functioning) and the prevention support system (i.e., training and technical assistance). The collection of implementation data is an essential feature of program evaluations, and more information is needed on which and how various factors influence implementation in different community settings.

4,175 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
30 Jun 2006-Science
TL;DR: Evidence on the effects of early environments on child, adolescent, and adult achievement and how early inputs strongly affect the productivity of later inputs is summarized.
Abstract: This paper summarizes evidence on the effects of early environments on child, adolescent, and adult achievement. Life cycle skill formation is a dynamic process in which early inputs strongly affect the productivity of later inputs.

2,882 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Results indicated that the various aspects of child self-regulation accounted for unique variance in the academic outcomes independent of general intelligence and that the inhibitory control aspect of executive function was a prominent correlate of both early math and reading ability.
Abstract: This study examined the role of self-regulation in emerging academic ability in one hundred and forty-one 3- to 5-year-old children from low-income homes. Measures of effortful control, false belief understanding, and the inhibitory control and attention-shifting aspects of executive function in preschool were related to measures of math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Results indicated that the various aspects of child self-regulation accounted for unique variance in the academic outcomes independent of general intelligence and that the inhibitory control aspect of executive function was a prominent correlate of both early math and reading ability. Findings suggest that curricula designed to improve self-regulation skills as well as enhance early academic abilities may be most effective in helping children succeed in school.

2,760 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
03 Jun 2009-JAMA
TL;DR: A scientific consensus is emerging that the origins of adult disease are often found among developmental and biological disruptions occurring during the early years of life as mentioned in this paper, and that these early experiences can affect adult health in 2 ways: cumulative damage over time or by the biological embedding of adversities during sensitive developmental periods.
Abstract: A scientific consensus is emerging that the origins of adult disease are often found among developmental and biological disruptions occurring during the early years of life. These early experiences can affect adult health in 2 ways—either by cumulative damage over time or by the biological embedding of adversities during sensitive developmental periods. In both cases, there can be a lag of many years, even decades, before early adverse experiences are expressed in the form of disease. From both basic research and policy perspectives, confronting the origins of disparities in physical and mental health early in life may produce greater effects than attempting to modify health-related behaviors or improve access to health care in adulthood.

2,065 citations