scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Is friendship akin to kinship

TL;DR: A domain-specific over a domain-general approach to understanding intimate relationships is supported and a number of interesting questions about the modular structure of cognitive and affective processes involved in these relationships are raised.
About: This article is published in Evolution and Human Behavior.The article was published on 2007-09-01 and is currently open access. It has received 125 citations till now. The article focuses on the topics: Kin recognition & Kinship.
Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: According to a life history framework, variability across an organism's lifespan necessitates trade-offs between behaviors that promote survival and those that promote reproduction as discussed by the authors, and this trade-off can be found in the literature.
Abstract: According to a life history framework, variability across an organism's lifespan necessitates trade-offs between behaviors that promote survival and those that promote reproduction. Adopting this p...

23 citations


Cites background from "Is friendship akin to kinship"

  • ...This replicates past research showing that friendship (non-sexual affiliation) is more important to women (Ackerman, Kenrick, and Schaller, 2007; Tamres, Janicki, and Helgeson, 2002; Taylor et al, 2000)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Results suggest that friendship jealousy is uniquely evoked by third-party threats to friendships (but not the prospective loss of the friendship alone), and may include features designed for beneficial-and arguably prosocial-ends: to help maintain friendships.
Abstract: Friendships can foster happiness, health, and reproductive fitness. However, friendships end-even when we might not want them to. A primary reason for this is interference from third parties. Yet, little work has explored how people meet the challenge of maintaining friendships in the face of real or perceived threats from third parties, as when our friends inevitably make new friends or form new romantic relationships. In contrast to earlier conceptualizations from developmental research, which viewed friendship jealousy as solely maladaptive, we propose that friendship jealousy is one overlooked tool of friendship maintenance. We derive and test-via a series of 11 studies (N = 2,918) using hypothetical scenarios, recalled real-world events, and manipulation of online emotional experiences-whether friendship jealousy possesses the features of a tool well-designed to help us retain friends in the face of third-party threats. Consistent with our proposition, findings suggest that friendship jealousy is (a) uniquely evoked by third-party threats to friendships (but not the prospective loss of the friendship alone), (b) sensitive to the value of the threatened friendship, (c) strongly calibrated to cues that one is being replaced, even over more intuitive cues (e.g., the amount of time a friend and interloper spend together), and (d) ultimately motivates behavior aimed at countering third-party threats to friendship ("friend guarding"). Even as friendship jealousy may be negative to experience, it may include features designed for beneficial-and arguably prosocial-ends: to help maintain friendships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

23 citations


Cites background from "Is friendship akin to kinship"

  • ...…theory and evidence suggest that sustained friendships may have long provided benefits—both across human cultures and also among nonhuman primates (Ackerman et al., 2007; Aktipis et al., 2018; Barclay, 2013; Benenson, 2014; Boesch, 2009; Campbell, 2002; David-Barrett et al., 2015; De Waal &…...

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Two studies suggest that mu-opioids play a significant role in experiences of social bonding within ritual contexts, with naltrexone leading to significantly lower social bonding compared with placebo.
Abstract: Religious rituals are universal human practices that play a seminal role in community bonding. In two experiments, we tested the role of mu-opioids as the active factor fostering social bonding. We used a mu-opioid blocker (naltrexone) in two double-blind studies of rituals from different religious traditions. We found the same effect across both studies, with naltrexone leading to significantly lower social bonding compared with placebo. These studies suggest that mu-opioids play a significant role in experiences of social bonding within ritual contexts.

22 citations


Cites background from "Is friendship akin to kinship"

  • ...Five items were verbal: ‘At this moment, how connected do you feel to the people in the group?’ [30]; ‘How much do you like the people in the group overall?’ [31]; ‘At this moment, how emotionally close do you feel to the other members of this group as a whole?’ [32]; ‘Thinking about everyone in this session now, do you feel you have a lot in common with others?’ [33]; ‘Thinking about everyone in this session now, how much do you trust the others in this group?’; with one pictorial item (the Inclusion of Others in Self scale, IOS; [34])....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown that perceived closeness has a strong independent effect on helping, which cannot be accounted for by other factors, and illustrates the potential value of reintegrating proximate psychological measures into anthropological studies of human cooperation.
Abstract: Anthropologists have long been interested in the reasons humans choose to help some individuals and not others Early research considered psychological mediators, such as feelings of cohesion or closeness, but more recent work, largely in the tradition of human behavioral ecology, shifted attention away from psychological measures to clearer observables, such as past behavior, genetic relatedness, affinal ties, and geographic proximity In this paper, we assess the value of reintegrating psychological measures—perceived social closeness—into the anthropological study of altruism Specifically, analyzing social network data from four communities in rural Bangladesh (N = 516), we show that perceived closeness has a strong independent effect on helping, which cannot be accounted for by other factors These results illustrate the potential value of reintegrating proximate psychological measures into anthropological studies of human cooperation

21 citations


Cites background from "Is friendship akin to kinship"

  • ...…is an internal state that integrates a number of cues of propinquity—frequency of interaction, amount of interaction, and degree of similarity—that would have been reliable indicators of genetic relatedness in past environments (Ackerman et al. 2007; Korchmaros and Kenny 2006; Neyer et al. 2015)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This work had people rank their support network members in order of who would be most likely to help and found that relatives were more likely to be ranked in primary positions and that the degree of relatedness correlated with rank.
Abstract: Hamilton’s rule predicts that individuals should be more likely to altruistically help closer kin and this theory is well supported from zoological studies of nonhumans. In contrast, there is a paucity of relevant human data. This is largely due to the difficulties of either experimentally testing relatives or of collecting data on genuinely costly cooperation. We test Hamilton’s rule in humans by seeing if the availability of help in times of crises is predicted by the degree of genetic relatedness. In social network research, the pool of people that one can go to for support during times of crisis is termed the support network. By definition, the members of a support network provide various benefits in times of need, and larger support networks have been shown to be important for general health. As this level of support bears costs for the providers and has clear benefits for the receivers, it therefore allows us to test Hamilton’s rule. We use an Internet sample to analyze the composition of 540 people’s support networks. We had people rank their support network members in order of who would be most likely to help and found that relatives were more likely to be ranked in primary positions and that the degree of relatedness correlated with rank.

20 citations

References
More filters
Book
01 Jan 1935
TL;DR: In this paper, Neuberg and Heine discuss the notion of belonging, acceptance, belonging, and belonging in the social world, and discuss the relationship between friendship, membership, status, power, and subordination.
Abstract: VOLUME 2. Part III: The Social World. 21. EVOLUTIONARY SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY (Steven L. Neuberg, Douglas T. Kenrick, and Mark Schaller). 22. MORALITY (Jonathan Haidt and Selin Kesebir). 23. AGGRESSION (Brad J. Bushman and L. Rowell Huesmann). 24. AFFILIATION, ACCEPTANCE, AND BELONGING: THE PURSUIT OF INTERPERSONAL CONNECTION (Mark R. Leary). 25. CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS (Margaret S. Clark and Edward P. Lemay, Jr.). 26. INTERPERSONAL STRATIFICATION: STATUS, POWER, AND SUBORDINATION (Susan T. Fiske). 27. SOCIAL CONFLICT: THE EMERGENCE AND CONSEQUENCES OF STRUGGLE AND NEGOTIATION (Carsten K. W. De Dreu). 28. INTERGROUP RELATIONS 1(Vincent Yzerbyt and Stephanie Demoulin). 29. INTERGROUP BIAS (John F. Dovidio and Samuel L. Gaertner). 30. SOCIAL JUSTICE: HISTORY, THEORY, AND RESEARCH (John T. Jost and Aaron C. Kay). 31. INFLUENCE AND LEADERSHIP (Michael A. Hogg). 32. GROUP BEHAVIOR AND PERFORMANCE (J. Richard Hackman and Nancy Katz). 33. ORGANIZATIONAL PREFERENCES AND THEIR CONSEQUENCES (Deborah H. Gruenfeld and Larissa Z. Tiedens). 34. THE PSYCHOLOGICAL UNDERPINNINGS OF POLITICAL BEHAVIOR (Jon A. Krosnick, Penny S. Visser, and Joshua Harder). 35. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LAW (Margaret Bull Kovera and Eugene Borgida). 36. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LANGUAGE: WORDS, UTTERANCES, AND CONVERSATIONS (Thomas Holtgraves). 37. CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY (Steven J. Heine). AUTHOR INDEX. SUBJECT INDEX.

13,453 citations

Book ChapterDOI
12 Jul 2017
TL;DR: The p,cnetics of sex nas now becn clarif ied, and Fishcr ( 1958 ) hrs produccd , n,od"l to cxplarn sex ratios at coDception, a nrodel recently extendcd to include special mccha_ nisms that operate under inbreeding (Hunrilron I96?).
Abstract: There is a tendency among biologists studying social behavior to regard the adult sex ratio as an independent variable to which the species reacts with appropriate adaptations D Lack often interprets social behavior as an adaptation in part to an unbalanced (or balanced) sex ratio, and J Verner has summarized other instances of this tendency The only mechanism that will generate differential mortality independent of sexual differences clearly related to parental investment and sexual selection is the chromosomal mechanism, applied especially to humans and other mammals: the unguarded X chromosome of the male is presumed to predispose him to higher mortality Each offspring can be viewed as an investment independent of other offspring, increasing investment in one offspring tending to decrease investment in others Species can be classified according to the relative parental investment of the sexes in their young In the vast majority of species, the male's only contribution to the survival of his offspring is his sex cells

10,571 citations


"Is friendship akin to kinship" refers background in this paper

  • ...A second line of reasoning applies more clearly to cross-sex friendships and draws on the logic of differential parental investment which suggests that, compared to men, women are likely to be more cautious and risk-averse in their approach to mating (Trivers, 1972)....

    [...]

  • ...In line with the immense literature on differential parental investment (Trivers, 1972), it also makes sense that these modules would operate somewhat differently for males and females....

    [...]

Book
01 Jan 1985

7,197 citations

Book
01 Jan 1959
TL;DR: In this article, the authors focus on patterns of interdependence and assume that these patterns play an important causal role in the processes, roles, and norms of relationships in interpersonal relations.
Abstract: This landmark theory of interpersonal relations and group functioning argues that the starting point for understanding social behavior is the analysis of dyadic interdependence. Such an analysis portrays the ways in which the separate and joint actions of two persons affect the quality of their lives and the survival of their relationship. The authors focus on patterns of interdependence, and on the assumption that these patterns play an important causal role in the processes, roles, and norms of relationships. This powerful theory has many applications in all the social sciences, including the study of social and moral norms; close-pair relationships; conflicts of interest and cognitive disputes; social orientations; the social evolution of economic prosperity and leadership in groups; and personal relationships.

5,869 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In the second series of experiments, it was found that the maximum joint profit independent of group membership did not affect significantly the manner in which the subjects divided real pecuniary rewards; however, maximum profit for own group did affect the distribution of rewards; and the clearest effect on the subject's attempt to achieve a maximum difference between the ingroup and the outgroup even at the price of sacrificing other "objective" advantages.
Abstract: The aim of the studies was to assess the effefcs of social categorization on intergroup behaviour when, in the intergroup situation, neither calculations of individual interest nor previously existing attitudes of hostility could have been said to have determined discriminative behaviour against an outgroup. These conditions were satisfied in the experimental design. In the first series of experiments, it was found that the subjects favoured their own group in the distribution of real rewards and penalities in a situation in which nothing but the variable of fairly irrelevant classification distinguished between the ingroup and the outgroup. In the second series of experiments it was found that: 1) maximum joint profit independent of group membership did not affect significantly the manner in which the subjects divided real pecuniary rewards; 2) maximum profit for own group did affect the distribution of rewards; 3) the clearest effect on the distribution of rewards was due to the subjects' attempt to achieve a maximum difference between the ingroup and the outgroup even at the price of sacrificing other ‘objective’ advantages. The design and the results of the study are theoretically discussed within the framework of social norms and expectations and particularly in relation to a ‘generic’ norm of outgroup behaviour prevalent in some societies.

4,523 citations