scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Manual matching of perceived surface orientation is affected by arm posture: evidence of calibration between proprioception and visual experience in near space

01 Jan 2012-Experimental Brain Research (Springer-Verlag)-Vol. 216, Iss: 2, pp 299-309
TL;DR: Two claims are supported: (1) manual orientation matching to visual surfaces is based on manual proprioception and (2) calibration between visual and proprioceptive experiences guarantees relatively accurate manual matching for surfaces within reach, despite systematic visual biases in perceived surface orientation.
Abstract: Proprioception of hand orientation (orientation production using the hand) is compared with manual matching of visual orientation (visual surface matching using the hand) in two experiments. In experiment 1, using self-selected arm postures, the proportions of wrist and elbow flexion spontaneously used to orient the pitch of the hand (20 and 80%, respectively) are relatively similar across both manual matching tasks and manual orientation production tasks for most participants. Proprioceptive error closely matched perceptual biases previously reported for visual orientation perception, suggesting calibration of proprioception to visual biases. A minority of participants, who attempted to use primarily wrist flexion while holding the forearm horizontal, performed poorly at the manual matching task, consistent with proprioceptive error caused by biomechanical constraints of their self-selected posture. In experiment 2, postural choices were constrained to primarily wrist or elbow flexion without imposing biomechanical constraints (using a raised forearm). Identical relative offsets were found between the two constraint groups in manual matching and manual orientation production. The results support two claims: (1) manual orientation matching to visual surfaces is based on manual proprioception and (2) calibration between visual and proprioceptive experiences guarantees relatively accurate manual matching for surfaces within reach, despite systematic visual biases in perceived surface orientation.

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Three different assessments of experimental demand indicate that even when the physical environment is naturalistic, and the goal of the main experimental manipulation was primarily concealed, artificial aspects of the social environment may still be primarily responsible for altered judgments of hill orientation.
Abstract: Experiments take place in a physical environment but also a social environment. Generalizability from experimental manipulations to more typical contexts may be limited by violations of ecological validity with respect to either the physical or the social environment. A replication and extension of a recent study (a blood glucose manipulation) was conducted to investigate the effects of experimental demand (a social artifact) on participant behaviors judging the geographical slant of a large-scale outdoor hill. Three different assessments of experimental demand indicate that even when the physical environment is naturalistic, and the goal of the main experimental manipulation was primarily concealed, artificial aspects of the social environment (such as an explicit requirement to wear a heavy backpack while estimating the slant of a hill) may still be primarily responsible for altered judgments of hill orientation.

114 citations


Cites background or methods from "Manual matching of perceived surfac..."

  • ...We used a custom inclinometer (Li & Durgin, 2011a) to measure hand orientation relative to a horizontal baseline, using the central axis of the hand to represent the response (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010)....

    [...]

  • ...Moreover, free hand measures also have been found to be tightly correlated with verbal measures (Li & Durgin, 2011a)....

    [...]

  • ...The gesture was conducted with the hand occluded behind a screen and was measured with a custom inclinometer (Li & Durgin, 2011a)....

    [...]

  • ...Li and Durgin (2011a) showed that free hand manual matching techniques were correlated with verbal reports (see also Li & Durgin, 2011b)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is concluded that walking measures are calibrated for perceived egocentric distance, but that pantomime walking measures may suffer range compression.
Abstract: Two experiments are reported concerning the perception of ground extent in order to discover whether prior reports of anisotropy between frontal extents and extents in depth were consistent across different measures (visual matching and pantomime walking) and test environments (outdoor environments and virtual environments). In Experiment 1 it was found that depth extents of up to 7 m are indeed perceptually compressed relative to frontal extents in an outdoor environment, and that perceptual matching provided more precise estimates than did pantomime walking. In Experiment 2, similar anisotropies were found using similar tasks in a similar (but virtual) environment. In both experiments pantomime walking measures seemed to additionally compress the range of responses. Experiment 3 supported the hypothesis that range compression in walking measures of perceived distance might be due to proactive interference (memory contamination). It is concluded that walking measures are calibrated for perceived egocentric distance, but that pantomime walking measures may suffer range compression. Depth extents along the ground are perceptually compressed relative to frontal ground extents in a manner consistent with the angular scale expansion hypothesis.

40 citations


Cites background or methods from "Manual matching of perceived surfac..."

  • ...…that perceived gaze angles are exaggerated and that perceived egocentric distances are foreshortened, as we have mentioned above, there is no actual contradiction because action can be calibrated to visual experience (Harris, 1963; Held & Freedman, 1965; Li & Durgin, 2012b; Rieser et al., 1995)....

    [...]

  • ...Note, in contrast, that if an L-shape configuration (1.5 m x 1.5 m) were placed on the ground 9 m away, and the direct comparison of the frontal and depth legs were requested, participants might tend to switch to a judgment based on estimating the optical slant (Li & Durgin, 2012a)....

    [...]

  • ...The angular expansion hypothesis has been successfully applied to findings regarding the perception of egocentric extents, vertical extents and slant whereas other theories tend to have a more limited scope (see Li & Durgin, 2012a)....

    [...]

  • ...One, associated with shape tasks such as the aspect ratio task developed by Loomis et al. (1992), is quite a large anisotropy that seems to be related to the misperception of local optical slant (Li & Durgin, 2010, 2012a; Loomis & Philbeck, 1999; Loomis et al., 2002)....

    [...]

  • ...…hypothesis The angular expansion hypothesis is based on evidence that two angular variables, optical slant and the gaze declination, are perceptually exaggerated in the range most relevant in action space (Durgin and Li, 2011; Durgin et al., 2010; Li and Durgin, 2009, 2010, 2012a; Li et al., 2011)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: While the intrinsic bias hypothesis is proposed only for explaining distance biases, the angular expansion hypothesis provides accounts for a broader range of spatial biases.
Abstract: Two theories of distance perception—ie, the angular expansion hypothesis (Durgin and Li, 2011 Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 73 1856-1870) and the intrinsic bias hypothesis (Ooi et al, 2006 Perception 35 605-624)—are compared. Both theories attribute exocentric distance foreshortening to an exaggeration in perceived slant, but their fundamental geometrical assumptions are very different. The intrinsic bias hypothesis assumes a constant bias in perceived geographical slant of the ground plane and predicts both perceived egocentric and exocentric distances are increasingly compressed. In contrast, the angular expansion hypothesis assumes exaggerations in perceived gaze angle and perceived optical slant. Because the bias functions of the two angular variables are different, it allows the angular expansion hypothesis to distinguish two types of distance foreshortening—the linear compression in perceived egocentric distance and the nonlinear compres- sion in perceived exocentric distance. While the intrinsic bias is proposed only for explaining distance biases, the angular expansion hypothesis provides accounts for a broader range of spatial biases.

35 citations

01 Jan 2017
TL;DR: This chapter reviews current knowledge of the phenomenology of slant misperception in relation to both functionalist and mechanistic accounts of this perceptual bias with respect to not only slant, but also other angular variables relevant to the biological measurement of surface layout.
Abstract: Hills look much steeper than they are. This chapter reviews current knowledge of the phenomenology of slant misperception in relation to both functionalist and mechanistic accounts of this perceptual bias. Recent discoveries suggest that this misperception of the geometry of our environment may be related to useful biological information coding strategies with respect to not only slant, but also other angular variables relevant to the biological measurement of surface layout. Even in the absence of hills, people misperceive the direction of their gaze systematically in ways that seem to contribute to the vertical expansion of the perceived environment.

20 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Two different theoretical frameworks for predicting what observers actually see in response to visual stimuli are compared: Bayesian decision theory and empirical ranking theory.

45 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...N IH -PA Author M anuscript N IH -PA Author M anuscript N IH -PA Author M anuscript (Howe and Purves 2005a; Howe et al. 2006)....

    [...]

  • ...Why should perception be systematically biased? One possibility is that this is because perceptual representations are constructs derived empirically during successful behaviors (Howe and Purves 2005a; Howe et al. 2006)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is demonstrated that older observers (at least through the age of 83 years) can effectively extract information about slant in depth from optical patterns containing texture, motion parallax, or binocular disparity.
Abstract: The ability of younger and older observers to perceive surface slant was investigated in four experiments. The surfaces possessed slants of 20 degrees, 35 degrees, 50 degrees, and 65 degrees, relative to the frontoparallel plane. The observers judged the slants using either a palm board (Experiments 1, 3, and 4) or magnitude estimation (Experiment 2). In Experiments 1-3, physically slanted surfaces were used (the surfaces possessed marble, granite, pebble, and circle textures), whereas computer-generated 3-D surfaces (defined by motion parallax and binocular disparity) were utilized in Experiment 4. The results showed that the younger and older observers' performance was essentially identical with regard to accuracy. The younger and older age groups, however, differed in terms of precision in Experiments 1 and 2: The judgments of the older observers were more variable across repeated trials. When taken as a whole, the results demonstrate that older observers (at least through the age of 83 years) can effectively extract information about slant in depth from optical patterns containing texture, motion parallax, or binocular disparity.

37 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...In research on space perception, action based measures are often used in studying perceived distance and slant (e.g. Bingham and Pagano 1998; Gibson and Cornsweet 1952; Loomis et al. 1992; Norman et al. 2009; Rieser et al. 1990)....

    [...]

  • ...the surface orientation they perceive (Gibson and Cornsweet 1952; Norman et al. 2009; ProYtt et al. 1995)....

    [...]

  • ...Participants rotate the palm board by hand to match the surface orientation they perceive (Gibson and Cornsweet, 1952; Norman et al. 2009; Proffitt et al. 1995)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The final posture of the arm when grasping a sphere to place it elsewhere is determined to a larger extend by the initial position of the object than by effects of starting and placing position.
Abstract: Despite the infinitely many ways to grasp a spherical object, regularities have been observed in the posture of the arm and the grasp orientation. In the present study, we set out to determine the factors that predict the grasp orientation and the final joint angles of reach-to-grasp movements. Subjects made reach-to-grasp movements toward a sphere to pick it up and place it at an indicated location. We varied the position of the sphere and the starting and placing positions. Multiple regression analysis showed that the sphere's azimuth from the subject was the best predictor of grasp orientation, although there were also smaller but reliable contributions of distance, starting position, and perhaps even placing position. The sphere's initial distance from the subject was the best predictor of the final elbow angle and shoulder elevation. A combination of the sphere's azimuth and distance from the subject was required to predict shoulder angle, trunk-head rotation, and lateral head position. The starting position best predicted the final wrist angle and sagittal head position. We conclude that the final posture of the arm when grasping a sphere to place it elsewhere is determined to a larger extend by the initial position of the object than by effects of starting and placing position.

36 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Interestingly, the trend to use more elbow than wrist was also observed in spontaneous arm postures during grasping actions (Schot et al. 2010)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An original design for a portable lightweight digital device for measuring hand orientation is described, which is microcontroller-based and uses a micro inclinometer chip as its inclination sensor and the parts are fairly inexpensive.
Abstract: Palm boards are often used as a nonverbal measure in human slant perception studies. It was recently found that palm boards are biased and relatively insensitive measures, and that an unrestricted hand gesture provides a more sensitive response (Durgin, Hajnal, Li, Tonge, & Stigliani, Acta Psychologica, 134, 182-197, 2010a). In this article, we describe an original design for a portable lightweight digital device for measuring hand orientation. This device is microcontroller-based and uses a micro inclinometer chip as its inclination sensor. The parts are fairly inexpensive. This device, used to measure hand orientation, provides a sensitive nonverbal method for studying slant perception, which can be used in both indoor and outdoor environments. We present data comparing the use of a free hand to palm-board and verbal measures for surfaces within reach and explain how to interpret free-hand measures for outdoor hills.

31 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background or methods or result in this paper

  • ...…IH -PA Author M anuscript N IH -PA Author M anuscript The excellent free-hand matching for slants in reach is somewhat at odds with results from other perceptual measures that suggest systematic biases in near space orientation perception (Durgin, Li and Hajnal 2010; Li and Durgin 2010, 2011)....

    [...]

  • ...Recently, a free hand was used as an alternative manual matching measure of perceived slant (Durgin, Hajnal, Li, Tonge and Stigliani 2010; Durgin, Li and Hajnal 2010; Hajnal et al. 2011; Li and Durgin 2011)....

    [...]

  • ...In previous studies using free-hand measures (i.e., Durgin et al. 2010a, b; Li and Durgin 2011), the experimenter typically demonstrated a hand gesture to...

    [...]

  • ...However, in contrast to this expectation, free hand performances can be predicted by verbal measures of visual slant in both cases (Li and Durgin 2011)....

    [...]

  • ...In previous studies using free hand measures (i.e. Durgin, Hajnal, Li, Tonge and Stigliani 2010; Durgin, Li and Hajnal 2010; Li and Durgin 2011), the experimenter typically demonstrated a hand gesture to the participants while explaining the task....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Orientation judgment channel capacity was studied under various conditions, and the number of discriminable orientations is far above the magic number “7“ limit, reaching over 20 in optimal circumstances.
Abstract: A systematic error is reported in orientation estimation, in that on average, estimates are closer to the vertical axis than are the stimuli by up to 6°. This systematic error results from a specific mechanism that may be related to depth perception, and that is avoided in certain circumstances or when other mechanisms take over. For example, the estimates of one observer who was a welltrained professional draughtsman did not show this systematic error. Furthermore, for all observers tested, estimation of clock time is not subject to the regular orientation estimation error. Rather, observers tend to estimate timesas slightly further from the quarter hour than they really are. Orientation judgment channel capacity was also studied under various conditions. The number of discriminable orientations is far above the magic number “7“ limit, reaching over 20 in optimal circumstances. The distribution of discriminable orientations is nonlinear, in that these are more closely packed about the horizontal and vertical axis than at the oblique.

31 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...But there is evidence that perception of the comparison orientation is itself biased; for example, the perception of 2D orientation is subject to systematic perceptual biases whether measured as absolute orientation (Dick and Hochstein 1989; Durgin and Li 2011a) or as an angular...

    [...]

  • ...For example, the perception of 2D orientation is subject to systematic perceptual biases whether measured as absolute orientation (Dick and Hochstein 1989; Durgin and Li 2011a) or as an angular deviation from a horizontal reference (Fischer, 1968; Jastrow, 1892; Wundt, 1862)....

    [...]