scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Manual matching of perceived surface orientation is affected by arm posture: evidence of calibration between proprioception and visual experience in near space

01 Jan 2012-Experimental Brain Research (Springer-Verlag)-Vol. 216, Iss: 2, pp 299-309
TL;DR: Two claims are supported: (1) manual orientation matching to visual surfaces is based on manual proprioception and (2) calibration between visual and proprioceptive experiences guarantees relatively accurate manual matching for surfaces within reach, despite systematic visual biases in perceived surface orientation.
Abstract: Proprioception of hand orientation (orientation production using the hand) is compared with manual matching of visual orientation (visual surface matching using the hand) in two experiments. In experiment 1, using self-selected arm postures, the proportions of wrist and elbow flexion spontaneously used to orient the pitch of the hand (20 and 80%, respectively) are relatively similar across both manual matching tasks and manual orientation production tasks for most participants. Proprioceptive error closely matched perceptual biases previously reported for visual orientation perception, suggesting calibration of proprioception to visual biases. A minority of participants, who attempted to use primarily wrist flexion while holding the forearm horizontal, performed poorly at the manual matching task, consistent with proprioceptive error caused by biomechanical constraints of their self-selected posture. In experiment 2, postural choices were constrained to primarily wrist or elbow flexion without imposing biomechanical constraints (using a raised forearm). Identical relative offsets were found between the two constraint groups in manual matching and manual orientation production. The results support two claims: (1) manual orientation matching to visual surfaces is based on manual proprioception and (2) calibration between visual and proprioceptive experiences guarantees relatively accurate manual matching for surfaces within reach, despite systematic visual biases in perceived surface orientation.

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Three different assessments of experimental demand indicate that even when the physical environment is naturalistic, and the goal of the main experimental manipulation was primarily concealed, artificial aspects of the social environment may still be primarily responsible for altered judgments of hill orientation.
Abstract: Experiments take place in a physical environment but also a social environment. Generalizability from experimental manipulations to more typical contexts may be limited by violations of ecological validity with respect to either the physical or the social environment. A replication and extension of a recent study (a blood glucose manipulation) was conducted to investigate the effects of experimental demand (a social artifact) on participant behaviors judging the geographical slant of a large-scale outdoor hill. Three different assessments of experimental demand indicate that even when the physical environment is naturalistic, and the goal of the main experimental manipulation was primarily concealed, artificial aspects of the social environment (such as an explicit requirement to wear a heavy backpack while estimating the slant of a hill) may still be primarily responsible for altered judgments of hill orientation.

114 citations


Cites background or methods from "Manual matching of perceived surfac..."

  • ...We used a custom inclinometer (Li & Durgin, 2011a) to measure hand orientation relative to a horizontal baseline, using the central axis of the hand to represent the response (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010)....

    [...]

  • ...Moreover, free hand measures also have been found to be tightly correlated with verbal measures (Li & Durgin, 2011a)....

    [...]

  • ...The gesture was conducted with the hand occluded behind a screen and was measured with a custom inclinometer (Li & Durgin, 2011a)....

    [...]

  • ...Li and Durgin (2011a) showed that free hand manual matching techniques were correlated with verbal reports (see also Li & Durgin, 2011b)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is concluded that walking measures are calibrated for perceived egocentric distance, but that pantomime walking measures may suffer range compression.
Abstract: Two experiments are reported concerning the perception of ground extent in order to discover whether prior reports of anisotropy between frontal extents and extents in depth were consistent across different measures (visual matching and pantomime walking) and test environments (outdoor environments and virtual environments). In Experiment 1 it was found that depth extents of up to 7 m are indeed perceptually compressed relative to frontal extents in an outdoor environment, and that perceptual matching provided more precise estimates than did pantomime walking. In Experiment 2, similar anisotropies were found using similar tasks in a similar (but virtual) environment. In both experiments pantomime walking measures seemed to additionally compress the range of responses. Experiment 3 supported the hypothesis that range compression in walking measures of perceived distance might be due to proactive interference (memory contamination). It is concluded that walking measures are calibrated for perceived egocentric distance, but that pantomime walking measures may suffer range compression. Depth extents along the ground are perceptually compressed relative to frontal ground extents in a manner consistent with the angular scale expansion hypothesis.

40 citations


Cites background or methods from "Manual matching of perceived surfac..."

  • ...…that perceived gaze angles are exaggerated and that perceived egocentric distances are foreshortened, as we have mentioned above, there is no actual contradiction because action can be calibrated to visual experience (Harris, 1963; Held & Freedman, 1965; Li & Durgin, 2012b; Rieser et al., 1995)....

    [...]

  • ...Note, in contrast, that if an L-shape configuration (1.5 m x 1.5 m) were placed on the ground 9 m away, and the direct comparison of the frontal and depth legs were requested, participants might tend to switch to a judgment based on estimating the optical slant (Li & Durgin, 2012a)....

    [...]

  • ...The angular expansion hypothesis has been successfully applied to findings regarding the perception of egocentric extents, vertical extents and slant whereas other theories tend to have a more limited scope (see Li & Durgin, 2012a)....

    [...]

  • ...One, associated with shape tasks such as the aspect ratio task developed by Loomis et al. (1992), is quite a large anisotropy that seems to be related to the misperception of local optical slant (Li & Durgin, 2010, 2012a; Loomis & Philbeck, 1999; Loomis et al., 2002)....

    [...]

  • ...…hypothesis The angular expansion hypothesis is based on evidence that two angular variables, optical slant and the gaze declination, are perceptually exaggerated in the range most relevant in action space (Durgin and Li, 2011; Durgin et al., 2010; Li and Durgin, 2009, 2010, 2012a; Li et al., 2011)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: While the intrinsic bias hypothesis is proposed only for explaining distance biases, the angular expansion hypothesis provides accounts for a broader range of spatial biases.
Abstract: Two theories of distance perception—ie, the angular expansion hypothesis (Durgin and Li, 2011 Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 73 1856-1870) and the intrinsic bias hypothesis (Ooi et al, 2006 Perception 35 605-624)—are compared. Both theories attribute exocentric distance foreshortening to an exaggeration in perceived slant, but their fundamental geometrical assumptions are very different. The intrinsic bias hypothesis assumes a constant bias in perceived geographical slant of the ground plane and predicts both perceived egocentric and exocentric distances are increasingly compressed. In contrast, the angular expansion hypothesis assumes exaggerations in perceived gaze angle and perceived optical slant. Because the bias functions of the two angular variables are different, it allows the angular expansion hypothesis to distinguish two types of distance foreshortening—the linear compression in perceived egocentric distance and the nonlinear compres- sion in perceived exocentric distance. While the intrinsic bias is proposed only for explaining distance biases, the angular expansion hypothesis provides accounts for a broader range of spatial biases.

35 citations

01 Jan 2017
TL;DR: This chapter reviews current knowledge of the phenomenology of slant misperception in relation to both functionalist and mechanistic accounts of this perceptual bias with respect to not only slant, but also other angular variables relevant to the biological measurement of surface layout.
Abstract: Hills look much steeper than they are. This chapter reviews current knowledge of the phenomenology of slant misperception in relation to both functionalist and mechanistic accounts of this perceptual bias. Recent discoveries suggest that this misperception of the geometry of our environment may be related to useful biological information coding strategies with respect to not only slant, but also other angular variables relevant to the biological measurement of surface layout. Even in the absence of hills, people misperceive the direction of their gaze systematically in ways that seem to contribute to the vertical expansion of the perceived environment.

20 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The collaborative findings of two laboratories clearly support the biased-midline hypothesis, which suggests that the behavioral dissociation is expected if the visual system uses a single frame of reference whose origin (the apparent midline) is biased toward the offset frame.

78 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Another possibility is that proprioception (i.e., proprioceptive perception of the orientation of the hand itself) is also misperceived, but in a manner that is calibrated and consistent with the distorted visual representation (see Dassonville et al 2004)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: One of the properties of a visual surface, along with hardness, distance, and color-with-illumination, is that of slant, which must be understood to include not-slanted as well as slanted; in other words the variable consists of opposite qualities having zero slant as a norm.
Abstract: One of the properties of a visual surface along with hardness, distance, and color-with-illumination, is that of slant. This term must be understood to include not-slanted as well as slanted; in other words the variable consists of opposite qualities having zero slant as a norm. There is evidence that optical slant, so-called, is determined by stimulation. When vision is monocular and the head is motionless, this quality seems to depend on the gradient of the density of the \"texture\" of the retinal image (1). The experiment which appeared to demonstrate this psychophysical correspondence, however, is defective in that the procedure failed to isolate the quality of optical slant from a congruent quality of geographical slant which accompanied it. This failure should be amended if possible. Moreover the two kinds of slant need to be denned and their relevance to spaceperception discussed. Consider first the impression of slant embodied in the face of an object—a bounded surface, or a segment of an array of surfaces. It can be studied in the following situation. The 0 sits in an ordinary room with his gaze horizontally straight ahead and fixates the center of a surface such as a sheet of textured cardboard. This surface is then rotated by E around a horizontal axis, either forward or back1 The experimental study here reported was carried out by Janet Crum Cornsweet under the general supervision of the senior author. The experiment was planned with the assistance of Howard Gruber. The research is part of a project carried out under Contract AF 41(128)-42 between Cornell University and the USAF School of Aviation Medicine. ward. The quality of slant will increase as rotation increases, either ceilingwise or floorwise, until just after reaching the greatest possible slant the surface suddenly becomes an edge (3). Putting aside the question of change in shape, this situation provides variation in slant without variation in distance or any of the other qualities of a surface. It also shows that the quality of slant has an upper absolute threshold at the point where the surface becomes parallel to the line of sight. It should be noted, however, that in this experiment the inclination of the surface to the line of sight is so arranged as to have the same value as its inclination to the physical horizontal. Consider next the impressions of slant embodied in a continuous plane surface filling most of the visual field. Take as an example the visual experience of a man standing on a level desert plain and looking about. This example is particularly significant since it is a kind of minimum perception for any sort of spatial behavior. What he sees is a level ground extending to the horizon with himself standing on it. No impression of slant seems to be evident. But this perception of the earth is almost certainly a product of the integration of successive eye-fixations (2, ch. 8). Ordinarily the man is unaware of his saccadic eyemovements, but if he attempts to in-

74 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...In research on space perception, action based measures are often used in studying perceived distance and slant (e.g. Bingham and Pagano 1998; Gibson and Cornsweet 1952; Loomis et al. 1992; Norman et al. 2009; Rieser et al. 1990)....

    [...]

  • ...the surface orientation they perceive (Gibson and Cornsweet 1952; Norman et al. 2009; ProYtt et al. 1995)....

    [...]

  • ...Participants rotate the palm board by hand to match the surface orientation they perceive (Gibson and Cornsweet, 1952; Norman et al. 2009; Proffitt et al. 1995)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown that outward drift in Experiment 1 was visually driven, and visually guided reaches were accurate when participants used binocular vision but when they used monocular vision, reaches were distorted.
Abstract: Psychophysical studies reveal distortions in perception of distance and shape. Are reaches calibrated to eliminate distortions? Participants reached to the front, side, or back of a target sphere. In Experiment 1, feedforward reaches yielded distortion and outward drift. In Experiment 2, haptic feedback corrected distortions and instability. In Experiment 3, feedforward reaches with only haptic experience of targets replicated the shape distortions but drifted inward. This showed that outward drift in Experiment 1 was visually driven. In Experiment 4, visually guided reaches were accurate when participants used binocular vision but when they used monocular vision, reaches were distorted. Haptic feedback corrected inaccuracy and instability of distance but did not correct monocular shape distortions. Dynamic binocular vision is representative and accurate and merits further study.

73 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown that the probability distributions of the possible real-world sources of projected lines and angles derived from a range-image database of natural scenes accurately predict each of these perceptual peculiarities.
Abstract: Visual stimuli that entail the intersection of two or more straight lines elicit a variety of well known perceptual anomalies. Preeminent among these anomalies are the systematic overestimation of acute angles, the underestimation of obtuse angles, and the misperceptions of line orientation exemplified in the classical tilt, Zollner, and Hering illusions. Here we show that the probability distributions of the possible real-world sources of projected lines and angles derived from a range-image database of natural scenes accurately predict each of these perceptual peculiarities. These findings imply that the perception of angles and oriented lines is determined by the statistical relationship between geometrical stimuli and their physical sources in typical visual environments.

72 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...N IH -PA Author M anuscript N IH -PA Author M anuscript N IH -PA Author M anuscript (Howe and Purves 2005a; Howe et al. 2006)....

    [...]

  • ...As a result, a Bayesian perceptual system can give rise to perceptual biases and optical illusions (e.g. Howe and Purves 2005b)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Results revealed that the haptic oblique effect occurs when the sensory motor traces associated with exploratory movement are represented more abstractly at a cognitive level.
Abstract: This review examines the isotropy of the perception of spatial orientations in the haptic system. It shows the existence of an oblique effect (i.e., a better perception of vertical and horizontal orientations than oblique orientations) in a spatial plane intrinsic to the haptic system, determined by the gravitational cues and the cognitive resources and defined in a subjective frame of reference. Similar results are observed from infancy to adulthood. In 3D space, the haptic processing of orientations is also anisotropic and seems to use both egocentric and allocentric cues. Taken together, these results revealed that the haptic oblique effect occurs when the sensory motor traces associated with exploratory movement are represented more abstractly at a cognitive level.

72 citations


"Manual matching of perceived surfac..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Hand proprioception and arm posture Systematic perceptual biases in somatosensory system have been reported in many studies (e.g. Darling, 1991; Flanders and Soechting 1995; Fuentes and Bastian 2010; Kappers 1999; see Gentaz et al. 2008 for a recent review on biases in haptic perception)....

    [...]