scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Mathematics: The Loss of Certainty.

01 Nov 1982-American Mathematical Monthly-Vol. 89, Iss: 9, pp 715
About: This article is published in American Mathematical Monthly.The article was published on 1982-11-01. It has received 453 citations till now. The article focuses on the topics: Certainty.
Citations
More filters
Posted Content
TL;DR: Conceptual integration—“blending”—is a general cognitive operation on a par with analogy, recursion, mental modeling, conceptual categorization, and framing that yields products that frequently become entrenched in conceptual structure and grammar.
Abstract: This is an expanded version of Gilles Fauconnier and Mark Turner. 1998. "Conceptual Integration Networks." Cognitive Science 22:2 (April-June 1998), 133-187. Conceptual integration - "blending" - is a general cognitive operation on a par with analogy, recursion, mental modeling, conceptual categorization, and framing. It serves a variety of cognitive purposes. It is dynamic, supple, and active in the moment of thinking. It yields products that frequently become entrenched in conceptual structure and grammar, and it often performs new work on its previously entrenched products as inputs. Blending is easy to detect in spectacular cases but it is for the most part a routine, workaday process that escapes detection except on technical analysis. It is not reserved for special purposes, and is not costly.In blending, structure from input mental spaces is projected to a separate, blended mental space. The projection is selective. Through completion and elaboration, the blend develops structure not provided by the inputs. Inferences, arguments, and ideas developed in the blend can have effect in cognition, leading us to modify the initial inputs and to change our view of the corresponding situations.Blending operates according to a set of uniform structural and dynamic principles. It additionally observes a set of optimality principles.

1,151 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
B. F. Skinner1

1,092 citations

Book
Anna Sfard1
21 Jan 2008
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors present a discourse on thinking about (mathematical) thinking and its relation to discourse in the context of mathematical discourse, and discuss how we mathematize and what we do about it.
Abstract: Introduction Part I. Discourse on Thinking: 1. Puzzling about (mathematical) thinking 2. Objectification 3. Commognition: thinking as communicating 4. Thinking in language Part II. Mathematics as Discourse: 5. Mathematics as a form of communication 6. Objects of mathematical discourse: what mathematizing is all about 7. Routines: how we mathematize 8. Explorations, deeds, and rituals: what we mathematize for 9. Looking back and ahead: solving old quandaries and facing new ones.

885 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The 100th anniversary of the British Ecological Society in 2013 is an opportune moment to reflect on the current status of ecology as a science and look forward to high-light priorities for future work.
Abstract: Summary 1. Fundamental ecological research is both intrinsically interesting and provides the basic knowledge required to answer applied questions of importance to the management of the natural world. The 100th anniversary of the British Ecological Society in 2013 is an opportune moment to reflect on the current status of ecology as a science and look forward to high-light priorities for future work.

652 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
30 May 2008-Science
TL;DR: The Mundurucu, an Amazonian indigene group with a reduced numerical lexicon and little or no formal education, mapped symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers onto a logarithmic scale, whereas Western adults used linear mapping with small or symbolic numbers and logarithsmic mapping when numbers were presented nonsymbolically under conditions that discouraged counting.
Abstract: The mapping of numbers onto space is fundamental to measurement and to mathematics. Is this mapping a cultural invention or a universal intuition shared by all humans regardless of culture and education? We probed number-space mappings in the Mundurucu, an Amazonian indigene group with a reduced numerical lexicon and little or no formal education. At all ages, the Mundurucu mapped symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers onto a logarithmic scale, whereas Western adults used linear mapping with small or symbolic numbers and logarithmic mapping when numbers were presented nonsymbolically under conditions that discouraged counting. This indicates that the mapping of numbers onto space is a universal intuition and that this initial intuition of number is logarithmic. The concept of a linear number line appears to be a cultural invention that fails to develop in the absence of formal education.

532 citations