scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Proceedings ArticleDOI

Overlapping speech, utterance duration and affective content in HHI and HCI — An comparison

TL;DR: The davero corpus a large naturalistic spoken corpus of real call center telephone conversations is investigated and the findings allow the prediction of forthcoming threat of overlapping speech, and hence preventive measures, especially in professional environments like call-centers with human or automatic agents.
Abstract: In human conversation, turn-taking is a critical issue. Especially if only the speech channel is available (e.g. telephone), correct timing as well as affective and verbal signals are required. In cases of failure, overlapping speech may occur which is in the focus of this paper. We investigate the davero corpus a large naturalistic spoken corpus of real callcenter telephone conversations and compare our findings to results on the well-known SmartKom corpus consisting of human-computer interaction. We first show that overlapping speech occurs in different types of situational settings — extending the well-known categories cooperative and competitive overlaps —, all of which are frequent enough to be analyzed. Furthermore, we present connections between the occurrence of overlapping speech and the length of the previous utterance, and show that overlapping speech occurs at dialog instances where certain affective states are changing. Our results allow the prediction of forthcoming threat of overlapping speech, and hence preventive measures, especially in professional environments like call-centers with human or automatic agents.

Summary (2 min read)

Introduction

  • The impact of food safety standards on bilateral trade is commonly evaluated using the gravity econometric model.
  • Burger et al. (2009) further extend the PPML estimation of Santos Silva and Tenreyro (2006) by considering the negative binomial, zero-inflated Poisson, and zero-inflated negative binomial models.
  • The Poisson regressions can solve the zero-omitted problem faced by the conventional log-normal OLS specification of the gravity equation and are robust to heteroskedasticity.
  • In this paper the authors use zero-accounting gravity models to evaluate the impact of food safety standards on developed country seafood imports.
  • Since the early 2000s, chemical standards including veterinary drug and other chemical residues have become the most serious challenges in the international seafood trade (Ababouch et al., 2005).

Conventional OLS and Zero-Accounting Models of the Gravity Equation

  • Anderson and van Wincoop’s gravity model: Tinbergen (1962) was the first to apply the Newtonian law of universal gravitation in physics to generate the gravity econometric model for studying bilateral trade flows.
  • The relevance of including GDPs in the gravity equation has been questioned because it is not relevant to the micro-founded gravity 1 Eq. (3) can be written in the level form as: K Gravity Model Selection in Seafood Trade 6 model (Disdier & Marette, 2010; Feenstra, 2004).
  • The Heckman estimation approach faces two essential problems.
  • Under such a situation, extensions of the PPML and NB models, Zero Inflated Poisson (ZIP) and Zero Inflated Negative Binomial (ZINB) models can be used to overcome the encountered problems.

Empirical Model Specification and Data Sources

  • In order to test the hypothesis that chemical standards act as barriers to international seafood trade, the authors first estimate the OLS gravity model suggested by Anderson and van Wincoop (2003) and the Heckman model in the log linear form of the dependent variable, bilateral trade.
  • The authors then estimate the gravity model in the level form using the Poisson family regressions: the PPML, NB, ZIP, and ZINB models.
  • Gravity Model Selection in Seafood Trade 15.

Estimated Results and Discussions

  • Table 1 shows the empirical results of the OLS and Heckman maximum likelihood models estimated in the log linear specification form.
  • The conditional marginal effect, and not the coefficient of the Heckman model, is comparable with the coefficient of the OLS model (Hoffmann & Kassouf, 2005).
  • With regards to the intensive margin of trade, conditioned on positive trade being observed, one unit reduction in chloramphenicol analytical limit (1 ppb) reduces bilateral seafood import 0.86% predicted by the OLS model and 0.84%predicted by the Heckman model.
  • The bilateral distance variable has a negative relationship with the probability of positive trade being observed.
  • Results of the Poisson family regressions are reported in Table 3.

Conclusions

  • The main objective of this investigation was to test if food safety standards act as barriers to international seafood trade.
  • The Gravity Equation in International Trade: Some Microeconomic Foundations and Emperical Evidence.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Overlapping Speech, Utterance Duration and
Affective Content in HHI and HCI an Comparison
Ingo Siegert, Ronald B
¨
ock, Andreas Wendemuth
Cognitive Systems Group
Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany
{firstname.lastname}@ovgu.de
Bogdan Vlasenko
Idiap Research Institute
Martigny, Switzerland
bogdan.vlasenko@idiap.ch
Kerstin Ohnemus
davero Dialog Gruppe
91058 Erlangen, Germany
kerstin.ohnemus@davero.de
Abstract—In human conversation, turn-taking is a critical
issue. Especially if only the speech channel is available (e.g.
telephone), correct timing as well as affective and verbal signals
are required. In cases of failure, overlapping speech may occur
which is in the focus of this paper. We investigate the davero
corpus a large naturalistic spoken corpus of real callcenter
telephone conversations and compare our findings to results on
the well-known SmartKom corpus consisting of human-computer
interaction. We first show that overlapping speech occurs in
different types of situational settings extending the well-known
categories cooperative and competitive overlaps –, all of which
are frequent enough to be analyzed. Furthermore, we present
connections between the occurrence of overlapping speech and
the length of the previous utterance, and show that overlapping
speech occurs at dialog instances where certain affective states are
changing. Our results allow the prediction of forthcoming threat
of overlapping speech, and hence preventive measures, especially
in professional environments like call-centers with human or
automatic agents.
I. INTRODUCTION
Human communication consists of several information
layers, of which the factual layer is the most important. But
beyond the pure textual information other relevant information
such as affective state, self-revelation, and appeal are transmit-
ted [1]. These different pieces of information are provided to
support human conversations and to increase the likelihood of
a fluent conversation.
One important requirement for a fluent and successful
conversation is an efficient turn-taking, which has to be orga-
nized by specific “underlying mechanisms”, such as intonation,
semantic cues, facial expressions, eye contact, breathing, and
gestures [2], [3], [4]. In the organization of turn-taking and
to evaluate the conversation, overlapping speech has a major
role. Based on the turn-taking model by Sacks et al. [3],
conversational partners aim to minimize overlaps and gaps in
their conversations. From to this model, overlaps occurs at
places of possible turn ends, either as “terminal overlaps” or
“simultaneous start”. Thus, overlapping speech is explained
as a result of turn-taking principles. This explanation is ex-
tended for different situational settings e.g. by [5], [6], where
short feedback signals confirming the statement of the current
speaker, are seen as “response token” overlaps. Furthermore,
several studies also analyses competitive overlaps, in which
the conversational partners compete for the turn [7], [8].
Many recent studies analyzed the phonetic structure of
overlapping speech and found that fundamental frequency,
intensity, speech rate and rhythm are important features charac-
terizing the overlaps as either being cooperative or competitive
[9], [8], [10]. Most of these studies concentrate on local
analyses investigating the acoustic characteristics next to or
directly at the overlap. Only a few studies incorporate for
example information on the duration of turns [11]. But the
relation of the length of utterances with the situational type of
overlap is not analyzed.
Former studies on overlapping speech concentrate to seek
an explanation of how overlapping speech works. They do
not analyze which consequences lead to an overlap or which
consequences the overlap has for the progress of the interac-
tion. The analyses especially disregard the length of utterances
where an overlap occurs (consequence lead to an overlap) and
which influence the affective state could have. Especially, in
[12] it is emphasized that affective states influence the turn-
taking behavior. Thus, problems in turn-taking can also be
traced back to changes in the affective state.
Furthermore, these studies are conducted only on human-
human interaction (HHI). Investigations on human-computer
interaction (HCI) do not consider overlapping speech as an
informative signal so far. But, to reach the target of a more
naturalistic interaction, future systems have to be adaptable to
the users’ individual skills, preferences, and current emotional
state [13], [14]. Lot of progress have been made in the area
of affect detection from speech, facial expression and gesture
[15]. For a fully naturalistic HCI, it is necessary to capture as
many human abilities as possible. Thus also linguistic features
gain considerable importance [16]. In an earlier study, we
could show that discourse particles, exchanged among the
interaction partners and used to signalize the progress of the
dialogue, are also used in naturalistic HCI, although the system
was not able to properly react to them [17]. The usage of these
cues is influenced by the user’s age and gender [18].
In this paper we will extend our analysis of linguistic
cues to overlapping speech and analyze the meaningfulness
of overlapping speech regarding the utterance length and the
user’s affective state change. We conducted a contrasting
study using human-human interaction (HHI) as well as HCI.
This will be the first step towards an automatic evaluation
of overlapping speech and could help future “Cognitive In-
focommunication” systems to understand the human better
[14]. Technical systems that use this extended recognition
of linguistic cues adapt to their users and thus become his
attendant and ultimately his companion [13], [19].

Based on these considerations, we investigate the following
three research questions in this paper.
Q1 Is overlapping speech occurring frequently enough in
our material to be analyzed in a dyadic conversation?
Q2 Is there any connection between overlapping speech
and the length of the previous utterance?
Q3 Is overlapping speech occurring at points where the
affective state is changing?
The remainder of the paper is structured as follows: In
Section II the utilized datasets are shortly described and
specific differences are emphasized. Afterwards, in Section III,
we describe the preparation of the date in terms of types of
overlap and affective annotation. In Section IV the results are
presented and discussed. Finally, in Section V, a conclusion of
our investigations and an outlook for further research is given.
II. UTILIZED DATASETS
A. Davero Corpus of Telephone-based Conversations
The dataset is described in detail in [20]. It is created
within a research project aiming to develop a technical system
that supports callcenter employees to respond appropriately to
the current affective state of the caller and was recorded in a
callcenter collecting real and authentic phone calls in German.
The calls embrace various topics, like information talks, data
change notifications, and complaints.
In order to allow a complete analysis of the conversation
both, agent and caller, were recorded acoustically. To gain real-
istic and high-quality recordings as well as to avoid disturbing
background noise, a separate recording place had been set up.
In total, 49 days 7 hours have been recorded. Since the
recorded phone conversations are real customer interactions
they had to be anonymized first, blanking out all personal
information. Furthermore, the start and end-times of each
dialog and overlapping speech segments were marked and each
utterance was assigned to its corresponding speaker (agent or
caller). To date, this dataset contains 1,600 dialogs with 27,000
individual utterances. The dialogs have an average length of
about 5 minutes with a standard deviation of ± 2 minutes.
B. SmartKom multi-modal Corpus
The SmartKom multi-modal corpus contains naturalistic af-
fects within a HCI [21]. The system responses were generated
by a Wizard-of-Oz (WOZ) setup. For our evaluations we use
German dialogs concerning a technical scenario, recorded in
a public environment. The database contains multiple audio
channels and two video channels (face and body in profile
posture). The primary aim of this corpus was the empirical
study of HCI in a number of different tasks. It is structured
into several sessions. Each session contains one conversation
and is approximately 4.5 minutes long.
This corpus has several annotation levels, of which for our
investigation the turn segmentation and an affective annotation
based on the acoustic channel is used [22]. The considered
set of the SmartKom corpus contains 438 emotionally labeled
dialogs with 12,076 utterances in total and 6,079 user utter-
ances. The utterances are labeled in seven broader affective
states: neutral, joy, anger, helplessness, pondering, surprise and
unidentifiable episodes. Unfortunately, the turn segmentation is
not time-aligned with the affective annotation.
III. PREPARATION OF DATASETS
A. Analysis of Overlapping Speech
We analyze overlapping speech as an additional pattern
of an interaction, as we assume that a valuable contribution
to the assessment of interactions is provided. Overlapping
speech refers to the case, when both speakers are talking
simultaneously.
1) Davero Corpus: By listening to examples of the Davero
corpus, four different situations (S) can be identified where
overlapping speech occurs:
S1 Short feedback, no interruption of the speaker
S2 Premature turn-taking at the end of the speaker’s turn
S3 Simultaneous starting after longer silence
S4 Barge-in, aiming to take the turn over
These situations are based on the descriptions of [11], distin-
guishing response tokens (S1), terminal overlaps (S2), simulta-
neous starts (S3) and competitive overlaps (S4). A prototypical
illustration is given in Figure 1.
In the first situation (S1), the listener just wants to give a
feedback. Lacking of other feedback methods (head nodding,
eye gaze), the listener has to give the feedback acoustically.
Thus no real turn-taking occurs. The second situation (S2) can
be seen as a functional turn-taking. The listener knows that
the speaker’s turn is due to end, but because of the missing
visual feedback the listener starts his turn a bit too early. In
this case just the alignment of the turn-taking is incomplete.
S3 is similar, both speakers start talking coincidentally after
a longer silence due to missing cues. S4 shows an disturbed
turn-taking. It describes the case where one speaker barges-in
while the other is still speaking to deliberately steal the turn
from that other speaker.
S1
S2
S3
Time
S4
Figure 1. Prototypes of the four different situations of two speakers (denoted
as and ) for overlapping speech in HHI.
To evaluate the overlapping speech according to these
descriptions, we employed two labelers with psychological
background for the assessment. They could choose between
all four situations or describe a situation not covered by
the definitions. Of the currently available 27,000 utterances
in 1,600 dialogs 5,100 utterances (18.9%, 830 dialogs) are
marked to contain overlapping speech.
The final assessment is as follows: S1 has a share of
61.6%, S2 a share of 11.2%, S3 a share of 10.7%, and S4
a share of 16.6%. Furthermore, no additional situation was
selected by the annotators. As inter-rater reliability of the
crosstalk labelling we calculated a Krippendorffs alpha of
0.63, a substantial reliability according to [23].

2) SmartKom Corpus: The SmartKom Corpus does not
have explicitly marked overlapping speech segments. By using
the segmentation annotation we could identify two types (T)
of overlapping speech (Figure 2):
T1 User interrupts the system.
T2 System interrupts the user.
T1
T2
Figure 2. Prototypes of the two different type of overlapping speech between
system () and user () in HCI.
In HCI the system is not seen as an equivalent dialog
partner [24], [25]. Therefore, the variety of types is not as big
as in HHI. From the 12,640 dialog acts within the SmartKom
corpus, we have 817 overlapping speech samples for T1 and
672 samples for T2.
B. Evaluation of the Affective States
We analyzed the affective states in both corpora based on
the Geneva Emotion Wheel by K. Scherer [26]. This is an
empirically tested instrument for the assessment of affective
states including 16 “emotional families”, which are arranged
on a circle along the axes dominance and valence.
1) Davero Corpus: To conduct the affective assessment,
we first employed a few annotators to manually segment the
recordings into single dialogs including the speaker turns. We
asked four annotators, all of them with psychological back-
ground, to assess the affective content of the single utterances.
We conducted several training rounds to make them familiar
with the used annotation scheme and the affective assessment
of acoustic data [27]. To support the annotation process, the
program ikannotate was used [28], [29]. This tool supports the
annotators by employing a three-step annotation process:
1. The annotator decides if the dominance is high or low.
2. The annotator decides for positive or negative valence.
3. The resulting quadrant of the wheel is displayed with
the containing emotion families. The annotator selects
one family among them to indicate the perceived
emotion.
For the present investigation, we only consider the labels from
step 1 and step 2, as we are only interested in a general
affective change. For the inter-rater reliability of the affective
annotation we calculated a Krippendorffs alpha of 0.20 for
dominance and of 0.35 for valence. Although these numbers
seem to be quite low compared to other reliability values
known from content analysis, they are in line with results from
other research groups on affective analyses [27].Considering
the annotation results we observe a nearly balanced distribution
among the utterances. High dominance has a share of 59.0%
and low dominance of 41.0%, positive valence has a share of
53.5% and negative valence of 46.5%.
2) SmartKom Corpus: The SmartKom Corpus already has
an affective annotation [30]. Unfortunately, this annotation is
not on the same time-scale as the dialog act segments. Thus,
we have to perform an alignment of both annotation levels,
by using the individual timing information of both annotation
levels. Unfortunately, the corpus authors only measured the
annotation correctness by comparing the results of different
annotation rounds rather than calculating an inter-rater agree-
ment measure like Krippendorffs alpha or Fleiss’ kappa. Their
calculated correctness is 45.52% [31]
Furthermore, in contrast to the Davero corpus, the affective
annotation in SmartKom is based on emotional categories.
Thus, we first have to deploy a mapping of the categories
used in SmartKom to our utilized dimensional categories of
dominance and valence. To conduct this mapping, we rely
on the Geneva Emotion Wheel [26], as it is also used for
the annotation of the Davero corpus. In analogy to similar
mappings [32], the assignment of the emotional categories to
the valence-dominance space is given in Table I. In contrast to
the Davero corpus, neutral is used as a category in SmartKom.
Table I. Mapping of SmartKom’s emotional categories to dominance and
valence.
category dominance valence
neutral 0 0
joy +1 +1
anger +1 -1
helplessness -1 -1
pondering -1 +1
surprise -1 +1
unidentifiable 0 0
In total we have 14,298 affective segments. Most of these
segments (59.7%) are neutral. The distribution on the dom-
inance dimension is 30.7% low and 69.3% high dominance.
Positive valence has a share of 39.0% positive and negative
valence a share of 61%. Thus, the emotional content within this
corpus is shifted towards negative valence and high dominance.
IV. RESULTS
A. Q1: Occurrence of Overlapping Speech
To answer the first question, we calculated the ratio of
overlapping speech segments and number of utterances. For the
Davero corpus we have 27,000 utterances and 5,100 of them
contain overlapping speech. Thus, we have a share of 18.9%
for overlapping speech segments. If we consider the dialog
level, we have 1,600 dialogs in total of which 830 dialogs
contain overlapping speech segments. This results in a share
of 51.9%.
The German part of the SmartKom Corpus has a total num-
ber of 12,076 utterances with 1,489 occurrences of overlapping
speech. This results in a share of 12.3% overlapping speech
utterances for this HCI. As we only take into account the
overlapping speech, we have 6,347 user utterances and 817
utterances contain overlapping speech.
Thus, we can conclude that overlapping speech is occur-
ring frequently and the first question: “Is overlapping speech
occurring frequently enough to be analyzed in a dyadic con-
versation?” is approved.
B. Q2: Overlapping Speech and Utterance Lengths
This investigation is triggered by the assumption that
overlapping speech is occurring because the actual speaker is

talking too long and the listener wants to get the turn. To
investigate this assumption, we calculated the mean length of
the utterance where the overlapping speech occurs (utt
ov erlap
)
in relation to all other utterances (utt
remain
) of this speaker
within a dialog. Afterwards, we averaged over all dialogs and
calculated the difference between both averaged mean lengths:
∆len = len
utt
overlap
len
utt
remain
(1)
This calculation is performed for each of the previous iden-
tified different situations separately and averaged afterwards
(∆len). Additionally, we used the non-parametric Mann-
Whitney-U-Test, to test the significance of the difference
within the utterance lengths. The star denotes the significance
level: ** p < 0.001.
1) Davero Corpus: From Figure 3 it can be seen, that in
two situations, the length of the utterance with overlapping
speech is different from other utterances of the same speaker.
For S1, the len
utt
overlap
is significantly longer than for other
utterances. In this situation one speaker gives statements that
are just confirmed by the listener without interrupting the
speaker. Thus, the speaker can continue his turn. The presence
of this type of overlapping speech does not indicate a change
in the progress of the dialog. The same statement can be made
for S2, where the len
utt
overlap
is significantly shorter than for
other utterances. The overlapping speech in both situations is
just occurring because only the acoustic channel can be used
to negotiate the turn-taking. Thus, the length of an utterance
together with the information of an occurring speech overlap
cannot be used as an indicator for dysfunctional conversation.
S1 S2 S3 S4
2
0
2
4
** **
Overlapping Speech Situation
∆len [s]
Figure 3. Difference of the utterance length for the four defined overlapping
speech situations and the average utterance length in the Davero corpus, stars
indicate a significant difference.
2) SmartKom Corpus: From Figure 4 it can be seen that
for type T1 the system utterance with the overlapping speech
segment (len
utt
overlap
) is significantly longer than the other
system utterances. Thus, it can be assumed that for a pos-
itive interaction outcome and fluent conversation the system
prompts shouldn’t be too long.
For the second type, where the system interrupts the users,
the users’ utterance length containing overlapping speech is not
significantly different from other user’s utterances. Therefore,
we assume that these interruptions of the system are caused
by operator errors of the WOZ-system.
Regarding our second research question, we can state that
there is a significant correlation between overlapping speech
and the length of the previous utterance in both HHI and HCI.
C. Q3: Overlapping Speech and Affective Changes
To investigate the affective change at the point where
overlapping speech occurs, we take into account the observed
affective states in two preceding utterances and compare it to
T1 T2
0
1
2
3
**
Type of Overlapping Speech
∆len [s]
Figure 4. Difference of the utterance length for the two types of overlapping
and the average utterance length in SmartKom. The stars indicate a significant
difference.
the observed affective states in the two succeeding utterances.
We distinguish between high (+1) and low (-1) dominance and
positive (+1) and negative (-1) valence. Utterances that do not
have an affective label or are labeled as neutral are assigned a
0. Thus, we can calculate the difference between the affective
states of the preceding utterances and the succeeding utterances
for an overlapping speech segment:
∆Affect = Affect
before overlap
Affect
after overlap
(2)
Afterwards, we average over all segments (∆Affect). The sig-
nificance of the affective change is tested by using the Mann-
Whitney-U-Test. The stars denotes the significance level: *
p < 0.01 and ** p < 0.001.
S1 S2 S3 S4
0
0.5
1
**
Overlapping Speech Situation
∆Dom
Figure 5. Dominance change at the point where overlapping speech occurs,
stars indicate a significant affective state change
1) Davero Corpus: Analyzing the change of affective states
in connection with overlapping speech only in S3 (simulta-
neous starting after longer silence) a significant change in
the affective state can be observed, see Figure 5. A possible
interpretation for this observation is that the dominance level
is dropping. Having a deeper analysis of the data, we can state
that the dominance level of the interrupter is raising, while the
dominance of the speaker whose turn is interrupted is slightly
decreasing. In this case, the overlapping speech event could be
a good marker for identifying changes in dominance. For all
other situations of overlapping speech, the dominance of the
two speakers is not influenced by overlapping speech.
S1 S2 S3 S4
0.2
0
0.2
Overlapping Speech Situation
∆Val
Figure 6. Valence change at the point where overlapping speech occurs in
the Davero corpus.
For the change of the speaker’s valence, we can state
that there is no significant connection with the occurrence of
overlapping speech, cf. Figure 6. This could be expected as
overlapping speech is related to the turn-taking behavior of

the speakers and the dominance of a speaker is seen as the
underlying mechanism to regulate the turn-taking [12].
T1 T2
1
0.5
0
** **
Type of Overlapping Speech
∆Dom
Figure 7. Dominance change at the point where overlapping speech occurs in
the SmartKom Corpus. The stars indicate a significant affective state change.
2) SmartKom Corpus: Regarding Figure 7, it can be seen
that the dominance for both types is significantly higher after
the overlapping speech segment than before. This it quite
obvious for the case where the user actively interrupts the
system, but when the system interrupts the user this seems
quite unintuitive and can only be explained in connection
with the valence change. Regarding the valence change (cf.
Figure 8), we can state that after the overlapping speech
the user is significantly more moved to negative values. This
finding in connection with a higher dominance shows that it
can be assumed that the user is more angry after overlapping
speech, either because he wants to speak and interrupts the
system, or he is annoyed because the system interrupts him.
T1 T2
0
0.5
1
** *
Type of Overlapping Speech
∆Val
Figure 8. Valence change at the point where overlapping speech occurs in
the SmartKom Corpus. The stars indicate a significant affective state change.
Regarding our third research question: “Is overlapping
speech occurring at points where the affective state is chang-
ing?”, we can conclude that in HHI only changes in the
dominance are related to overlapping speech, whereas in HCI
significant changes in both affective dimensions, dominance
and valence, can be observed.
V. CONCLUSION
In this paper, we present a first study investigating over-
lapping speech effects in both HHI and HCI. The analyses are
conducted on a dataset of realistic HHI containing telephone
based conversations and the well-known SmartKom Corpus
of naturalistic HCI. We could show that in both datasets
overlapping speech occurs frequently enough to be analyzed
with a share of 18.9% for HHI and 12.3% for HCI. For the
investigated HHI, this share is in-line with the results of other
research groups [11], [33]. The amount of overlap in HCI is
a bit lower but still sufficient. For this no numbers of other
researchers are to our best knowledge reported in the literature.
Based on the description of situational settings, we first an-
alyzed the correlation between the length of overlap-preceding
utterances and the occurrence of overlap. As a result of our
first analysis, we could expose significant relations to the
length of the spoken utterances and changes in the affective
state of the conversational partners. In HHI we could find a
significant correlation between overlapping speech as feedback
and premature turn-taking. Also in HCI a significant corre-
lation is found between overlapping speech and the length
of system utterances. The user’s utterance-lengths did not
show significant correlations for the occurrence of overlaps,
we assume that these overlaps are just caused by operator
malfuntions, as the SmartKom data are recorded in a WOZ-
scenario. And now pre-defined design rules are given for the
wizards how to use overlap [21].
Secondly, we analyzed the correlation of affective changes
at in the surrounding of the overlap in both types of inter-
actions. For this investigation, we showed that overlapping
speech goes along with changes in the affective states of
dominance and valence in certain situations. In HHI only
the situation where both speakers start simultaneously after a
longer pause effects a significant change of dominance. For the
valence dimension no significant correlation could be found. In
the investigated HCI both affective dimensions, dominance and
valence, show a significant correlation to overlapping speech
in both situation types.
From these results, we are able to derive some rules for
the organization of interactions: In telephone based HHI the
utterances should not be too long and the listener should be
encouraged to give feedback. This avoids competitive barge-
in overlaps. For HCI, the system should not talk to long as
for all overlapping speech segments, an affective change to
higher dominance and negative valence of the speaker can be
observed. But this kind of affective change should be avoided.
To evaluate these statements for their generality, a broader
investigation including additional corpora has to be conducted.
A possible application of our investigations in HHI and
HCI is the identification of parts where the affective state
changes based on the knowledge of overlapping speech and the
dialog course: As e.g. situation S3, where both speakers start
simultaneously, can be easily identified by duration analysis,
this knowledge can be used to find affective material for further
emotional analyses.
In our further research activities, we will develop a robust
automatic identification of the different types of overlap.
Together with the recognition of the user’s affective state,
we are a step further to future Cognitive Infocommunication
systems acting as a companion towards human users [13], [14].
VI. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The work presented in this paper was done within the
Transregional Collaborative Research Centre SFB/TRR 62
‘Companion-Technology for Cognitive Technical Systems’
funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG).
REFERENCES
[1] F. Schulz von Thun, Miteinander reden 1 - St
¨
orungen und Kl
¨
arungen.
Reinbek, Germany: Rowohlt, 1981.
[2] R. Ishii, K. Otsuka, S. Kumano, and J. Yamato, Analysis of respiration
for prediction of ”who will be next speaker and when?” in multi-party
meetings, in Proc. of the 16th International Conference on Multimodal
Interaction, ser. ICMI ’14, Istanbul, Turkey, 2014, pp. 18–25.
[3] H. Sacks, E. A. Schegloff, and G. Jefferson, A simplest systematics
for the organization of turn taking for conversation, Language, vol. 50,
pp. 696–735, 1974.

Citations
More filters
Proceedings ArticleDOI
20 Aug 2017
TL;DR: This paper presents an approach for automatic classification of competitive and cooperative overlaps using the emotional content of the speakers’ utterances before and after the overlap, and concludes that emotional features are clearly superior to acoustic features for this task.
Abstract: One interesting phenomenon of natural conversation is overlapping speech. Besides causing difficulties in automatic speech processing, such overlaps carry information on the state of the overlapper: competitive overlaps (i.e. “interruptions”) can signal disagreement or the feeling of being overlooked, and cooperative overlaps (i.e. supportive interjections) can signal agreement and interest. These hints can be used to improve humanmachine interaction. In this paper we present an approach for automatic classification of competitive and cooperative overlaps using the emotional content of the speakers’ utterances before and after the overlap. For these experiments, we use real-world data from human-human interactions in call centres. We also compare our approach to standard acoustic classification on the same data and come to the conclusion, that emotional features are clearly superior to acoustic features for this task, resulting in an unweighted average f-measure of 71.9%. But we also find that acoustic features should not be entirely neglected: using a late fusion procedure, we can further improve the unweighted average f-measure by 2.6%.

3 citations


Cites background from "Overlapping speech, utterance durat..."

  • ...The most interesting aspect about it is the occurrence of overlaps in spoken human-machine interaction, since overlaps are not an exclusive human-human interaction phenomenon, but occur in human-machine interaction, too [10]....

    [...]

01 Jan 2019
TL;DR: The proposed paper study this phenomenon on the small corpus of Slovak interviews, where the attention is focused on overlapping segments and discussed possible applications, where anticipatory behavior on the machine side can bring benefits.
Abstract: Behinds the capability of a person to be an interlocutor of a conversation there lies many human capabilities, many of which are carried out unconsciously and very naturally in childhood. Human-like turn-taking in the human-machine interactions (HMI) can be seen as a critical issue to achieve natural conversational interaction. The production of the listener response starts before the speaker turn is finished, which means, that listener is often able to anticipate the remaining content of unfolded speaker turn. The ability to anticipate can be identified as a very important human capability, which supports rapid turn-taking. This anticipation process, which occurs during listening relates to sentence comprehension and turn-taking. The proposed paper study this phenomenon on the small corpus of Slovak interviews, where the attention is focused on overlapping segments and discussed possible applications, where anticipatory behavior on the machine side can bring benefits.

2 citations

Book ChapterDOI
29 Jul 2016
TL;DR: A computational model of real-time face tracking has been presented and the tracking problem into three parts: the region extractor is based on the integration of skin-color, motion, and silhouette features, while the face detector uses a simple, rule-based face detection algorithm, and SVM.
Abstract: Rapid and tough tracking of multiple faces have many crucial applications in various fields like video surveillance and video conferencing. Hence, it gained lot of momentum from computer vision researchers. Initialization, tracking and display are the three important steps in real-time tracking of multiple faces in high-resolution videos. Out of which, tracking is quite intensive in computation. Object detection and tracking are important in many computer vision applications including activity recognition, automotive safety, and surveillance. In this paper, a computational model of real-time face tracking has been presented. The tracking problem into three parts: the region extractor is based on the integration of skin-color, motion, and silhouette features, while the face detector uses a simple, rule-based face detection algorithm, and SVM. Real-time detection and tracking implementation has been carried out and results presented.

2 citations

Proceedings ArticleDOI
18 Sep 2022
TL;DR: In this article , a new approach to overlapped speech detection (OSD) is introduced, which is designed for real-time processing of streamed data and utilizes x-vectors as its input features.
Abstract: A new approach to overlapped speech detection (OSD) is introduced in this work. It is designed for real-time processing of streamed data and utilizes x-vectors as its input features. It thus allows us to reduce computational demands within the en-tire streaming data processing chain, where the same x-vectors can also be used for the related task of speaker diarization. Within our method, the x-vectors are extracted using a feed-forward sequential memory network (FSMN) and then fed into a simple neural classifier (speech or cross-talk), whose output is smoothed by a decoder based on weighted finite-state transducers (WFSTs). The evaluation is done on a Czech/Slovak broadcast dataset (we make this data public) and on the AMI meeting corpus. Our online method yields a solid performance while operating with a 2-second latency.

2 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Esposito, Cordasco, Vogel and Baranyi as mentioned in this paper , have published an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY), provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited.
Abstract: © 2023 Esposito, Cordasco, Vogel and Baranyi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. Editorial: Cognitive infocommunications

1 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A general statistical methodology for the analysis of multivariate categorical data arising from observer reliability studies is presented and tests for interobserver bias are presented in terms of first-order marginal homogeneity and measures of interob server agreement are developed as generalized kappa-type statistics.
Abstract: This paper presents a general statistical methodology for the analysis of multivariate categorical data arising from observer reliability studies. The procedure essentially involves the construction of functions of the observed proportions which are directed at the extent to which the observers agree among themselves and the construction of test statistics for hypotheses involving these functions. Tests for interobserver bias are presented in terms of first-order marginal homogeneity and measures of interobserver agreement are developed as generalized kappa-type statistics. These procedures are illustrated with a clinical diagnosis example from the epidemiological literature.

64,109 citations


Additional excerpts

  • ...63, a substantial reliability according to [23]....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Dec 1974-Language
TL;DR: Turn-taking is used for the ordering of moves in games, for allocating political office, for regulating traffic at intersections, for the servicing of customers at business establishments, and for talking in interviews, meetings, debates, ceremonies, conversations.
Abstract: Publisher Summary Turn taking is used for the ordering of moves in games, for allocating political office, for regulating traffic at intersections, for the servicing of customers at business establishments, and for talking in interviews, meetings, debates, ceremonies, conversations. This chapter discusses the turn-taking system for conversation. On the basis of research using audio recordings of naturally occurring conversations, the chapter highlights the organization of turn taking for conversation and extracts some of the interest that organization has. The turn-taking system for conversation can be described in terms of two components and a set of rules. These two components are turn-constructional component and turn-constructional component. Turn-allocational techniques are distributed into two groups: (1) those in which next turn is allocated by current speaker selecting a next speaker and (2) those in which next turn is allocated by self-selection. The turn-taking rule-set provides for the localization of gap and overlap possibilities at transition-relevance places and their immediate environment, cleansing the rest of a turn's space of systematic bases for their possibility.

10,944 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the authors define emotion as a phenomenon to be studied, without consensual conceptualization and operationalization of exactly what phenomenon is to be investigated. But progress in theory and research is difficult to a...
Abstract: Defining “emotion” is a notorious problem. Without consensual conceptualization and operationalization of exactly what phenomenon is to be studied, progress in theory and research is difficult to a...

3,247 citations


"Overlapping speech, utterance durat..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...To conduct this mapping, we rely on the Geneva Emotion Wheel [26], as it is also used for the annotation of the Davero corpus....

    [...]

  • ...We analyzed the affective states in both corpora based on the Geneva Emotion Wheel by K. Scherer [26]....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors discuss human emotion perception from a psychological perspective, examine available approaches to solving the problem of machine understanding of human affective behavior, and discuss important issues like the collection and availability of training and test data.
Abstract: Automated analysis of human affective behavior has attracted increasing attention from researchers in psychology, computer science, linguistics, neuroscience, and related disciplines. However, the existing methods typically handle only deliberately displayed and exaggerated expressions of prototypical emotions despite the fact that deliberate behaviour differs in visual appearance, audio profile, and timing from spontaneously occurring behaviour. To address this problem, efforts to develop algorithms that can process naturally occurring human affective behaviour have recently emerged. Moreover, an increasing number of efforts are reported toward multimodal fusion for human affect analysis including audiovisual fusion, linguistic and paralinguistic fusion, and multi-cue visual fusion based on facial expressions, head movements, and body gestures. This paper introduces and surveys these recent advances. We first discuss human emotion perception from a psychological perspective. Next we examine available approaches to solving the problem of machine understanding of human affective behavior, and discuss important issues like the collection and availability of training and test data. We finally outline some of the scientific and engineering challenges to advancing human affect sensing technology.

2,503 citations


"Overlapping speech, utterance durat..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Lot of progress have been made in the area of affect detection from speech, facial expression and gesture [15]....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (11)
Q1. What are the contributions mentioned in the paper "Overlapping speech, utterance duration and affective content in hhi and hci – an comparison" ?

In cases of failure, overlapping speech may occur which is in the focus of this paper. The authors investigate the davero corpus a large naturalistic spoken corpus of real callcenter telephone conversations and compare their findings to results on the well-known SmartKom corpus consisting of human-computer interaction. The authors first show that overlapping speech occurs in different types of situational settings – extending the well-known categories cooperative and competitive overlaps –, all of which are frequent enough to be analyzed. Furthermore, the authors present connections between the occurrence of overlapping speech and the length of the previous utterance, and show that overlapping speech occurs at dialog instances where certain affective states are changing. 

In their further research activities, the authors will develop a robust automatic identification of the different types of overlap. Together with the recognition of the user ’ s affective state, the authors are a step further to future Cognitive Infocommunication systems acting as a companion towards human users [ 13 ], [ 14 ]. 

the authors used the non-parametric MannWhitney-U-Test, to test the significance of the difference within the utterance lengths. 

The considered set of the SmartKom corpus contains 438 emotionally labeled dialogs with 12,076 utterances in total and 6,079 user utterances. 

Of the currently available 27,000 utterances in 1,600 dialogs 5,100 utterances (18.9%, 830 dialogs) are marked to contain overlapping speech. 

For this investigation, the authors showed that overlapping speech goes along with changes in the affective states of dominance and valence in certain situations. 

This corpus has several annotation levels, of which for their investigation the turn segmentation and an affective annotation based on the acoustic channel is used [22]. 

A possible application of their investigations in HHI and HCI is the identification of parts where the affective state changes based on the knowledge of overlapping speech and the dialog course: 

the corpus authors only measured the annotation correctness by comparing the results of different annotation rounds rather than calculating an inter-rater agreement measure like Krippendorff’s alpha or Fleiss’ kappa. 

As the authors only take into account the overlapping speech, the authors have 6,347 user utterances and 817 utterances contain overlapping speech. 

To conduct the affective assessment, the authors first employed a few annotators to manually segment the recordings into single dialogs including the speaker turns.