scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Partisan bias in opinion formation on episodes of political controversy : evidence from Great Britain.

TL;DR: This article found that partisan predispositions and tolerance of political misconduct are both important in shaping voter opinions and that partisanship has the strongest influence among the more knowledgeable and interested voters on political controversies.
Abstract: Voters form judgements about political controversies through a process of motivated reasoning driven by two goals: the desire to reach an objectively accurate conclusion (accuracy) and the desire to reach a conclusion congruent with pre-existing views (direction). The impact of directional goals may depend on political sophistication. We test our hypotheses using data from a 2011 British survey that measured voters' opinions on three specific real-life political controversies. We use voters' underlying tolerance of political misconduct as an indicator of accuracy goals and party identification as a measure of directional goals. We find that partisan predispositions and tolerance of political misconduct are both important in shaping voter opinions and that partisanship has the strongest influence among the more knowledgeable and interested voters. These findings further our understanding of how voters react to political controversies and how they process new political information.

Summary (2 min read)

Introduction

  • Controversies about the behaviour of a politician are common in any democracy.
  • But while media focus on political controversy undoubtedly puts instances of political misconduct on the public agenda, the consequences of such an episode depend on the opinions that voters form about just how wrong and punishable a politician’s behaviour was.
  • The two goals highlighted by political psychologists are accuracy and directional goals (Lodge and Taber 2000).

The role of political sophistication

  • Because voters are heterogeneous, the influence of partisan affect on opinions about scandals may not be the same for all individuals.
  • On the one hand, there are arguments that the least sophisticated would be the most biased.
  • Such voters might therefore also be more inclined towards using heuristics in determining their political opinions.
  • Partisan bias affects decisions more among people with higher levels of political knowledge because they are also more likely to recognize the partisan aspects of an issue in the first place and are able to consciously judge it in relation to their pre-existing partisan loyalties (Zaller, 1992: 121).
  • The theory and evidence on the moderating influence of political sophistication is therefore mixed and inconclusive.

Data

  • Sanders et al. (2007) provide evidence for the reliability of such internet-based surveys.
  • This shows that opinions on episodes of political controversy are not knee-jerk responses: voters do distinguish between the politicians involved.
  • In their sample, 79 per cent said Laws should have resigned, 62 per cent said the same for Chris Huhne, and only 34 per cent believed that Ken Clarke should step down.
  • The authors then create a composite indicator based on all answers to these questions; the Cronbach’s alpha for the scale is a satisfactory 0.73.
  • The authors therefore decided to measure political knowledge by providing respondents with a list of six politicians and asked which of these were currently in the UK Cabinet; only three of the politicians actually were.

Results

  • The authors begin their discussion of the results by examining voter opinions on the politician’s actions.
  • The authors then consider views on whether the politician should resign before examining the moderating influence of political sophistication.

Voter opinions on a politician’s actions

  • The authors first results concern the influence of partisanship and general attitudes on right and wrong on how wrong voters think a politician’s behaviour was.
  • First, Models 2, 4 and 6 show that their control variables do not always have the same effects.
  • The effect of the ‘no identification’ indicator is estimated at between 0.47 and 0.68, with all p-values below 0.1.
  • The authors also present the expected change in the wrongness scale for various shifts in party identification, either from identifying with the politician’s party or from identifying with no party.
  • In sum, the effect of partisanship on specific opinions about episodes of political controversy is strong and consistent, as is the effect of general intolerance towards political misconduct.

Resignation

  • When it comes to political controversies, the debate is just as much about the appropriate consequences of the particular wrongdoing as about the gravity of the case itself.
  • To answer this question, the authors consider the estimated impact of partisanship and general intolerance of political misconduct on right and wrong on whether a respondent thinks a politician should resign.
  • Again, the authors show the results from two sets of models: first with only the key independent variables and then controlling for the same variables in Models 2, 4 and 6.
  • All predicted probabilities are calculated using the SPost package in Stata (Long and Freese, 2005).
  • Accuracy and directional goals both clearly influence calls for resignation, and do so strongly.

Political sophistication

  • Finally, the authors hypothesised that political sophistication may moderate the impact of partisan identification on opinions on episodes of political controversy.
  • The authors present results graphically following the recommendations of Brambor et al. (2006).
  • Figure 4 shows how the effect of party identification depends on a voter’s level of political sophistication.
  • The results are substantively similar for the Huhne and Clarke controversies.
  • When comparing Conservative to Liberal Democrat identifiers in the cases of Huhne and Laws, there is a stronger effect of partisanship as sophistication increases only for the Huhne episode.

Conclusion

  • This research has suggested that accuracy and directional goals are both important to understanding how voters respond to reports of misbehaviour by politicians.
  • The authors overall findings can be interpreted in two ways.
  • In addition, existing scholarship argues that the influence and importance of party identification in Britain is similar to that in other European countries and lower than in the United States (Shiveley 1979; Westholm and Niemi 1992; Dalton 2008; Milazzo et al. 2012).
  • This also appears to have an important influence on how partisanship influences opinion formation.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Durham Research Online
Deposited in DRO:
30 November 2015
Version of attached le:
Accepted Version
Peer-review status of attached le:
Peer-reviewed
Citation for published item:
Wagner, Markus and Tarlov, Jessie and Vivyan, Nick (2014) 'Partisan bias in opinion formation on episodes of
political controversy : evidence from Great Britain.', Political studies., 62 (1). pp. 136-158.
Further information on publisher's website:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2012.01002.x
Publisher's copyright statement:
This is the accepted version of the following article: Wagner, M., Tarlov, J. and Vivyan, N. (2014), Partisan Bias in
Opinion Formation on Episodes of Political Controversy: Evidence from Great Britain. Political Studies, 62(1):
136-158, which has been published in nal form at http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2012.01002.x. This article
may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance With Wiley Terms and Conditions for self-archiving.
Additional information:
Use policy
The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for
personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that:
a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source
a link is made to the metadata record in DRO
the full-text is not changed in any way
The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.
Please consult the full DRO policy for further details.
Durham University Library, Stockton Road, Durham DH1 3LY, United Kingdom
Tel : +44 (0)191 334 3042 | Fax : +44 (0)191 334 2971
https://dro.dur.ac.uk

1
Partisan bias in opinion formation on episodes of political controversy: evidence from
Great Britain
Abstract
Voters form judgments about political controversies through a process of motivated reasoning
driven by two goals: the desire to reach an objectively accurate conclusion (accuracy) and the
desire to reach a conclusion congruent with pre-existing views (direction). The impact of
directional goals may depend on political sophistication. We test our hypotheses using data
from a 2011 British survey that measured voters’ opinions on three specific real-life political
controversies. We use voters’ underlying tolerance of political misconduct as an indicator of
accuracy goals and party identification as a measure of directional goals. We find that partisan
predispositions and tolerance of political misconduct are both important in shaping voter
opinions and that partisanship has the strongest influence among the more knowledgeable and
interested voters. These findings further our understanding of how voters react to political
controversies and how they process new political information.

2
Introduction
Controversies about the behaviour of a politician are common in any democracy. Such
controversies are often characterised by intense media attention and an incremental
uncovering of the facts of the case. Whether politicians can survive a political controversy
depends on a variety of factors. For example, the so-called ‘Alistair Campbell rule’, named
after British Prime Minister Tony Blair’s former director of communications, states that ‘no
Minister can survive beyond a two-week feeding frenzy in the press’.
1
But while media focus
on political controversy undoubtedly puts instances of political misconduct on the public
agenda, the consequences of such an episode depend on the opinions that voters form about
just how wrong and punishable a politician’s behaviour was. For one, nowadays almost
instant polls ask voters for their opinion regarding the events, and the results of these polls can
influence both media coverage and politicians strategies. Moreover, political controversy can
also have electoral consequences, for example when voters punish misbehaving politicians at
subsequent elections (Ahuja, et al. 1994, Banducci and Karp, 1994, Clarke, et al. 1999,
Dimock and Jacobsen 1995, Farrell et al. 1998, Pattie and Johnston forthcoming, Peters and
Welch 1980, Vivyan et al. forthcoming, Welch and Hibbing 1997).
Voters form their opinions on the moral acceptability and appropriate consequences of
a politician’s actions in the same way they form opinions about other political events: through
a process of motivated reasoning (Kunda 1990; Lodge and Taber 2000). According to this
view, voters form opinions by incorporating and evaluating new incoming information, but
they do so in ways that are guided by specific goals. The two goals highlighted by political
psychologists are accuracy and directional goals (Lodge and Taber 2000). Accuracy goals
exist due to the need to reach a correct, truthful conclusion. Directional goals exist due to the
need to reach a specific conclusion, so one that is in accordance with prior opinions and
1
This is the version of the Alistair Campbell rule cited by Polly Toynbee in the Guardian, 16
December 2004 (http://www.guardian.co.uk/politics/2004/dec/16/schools.davidblunkett).

3
predispositions. These two goals are always present, though one of the two is of often stronger
than the other (Lodge and Taber 2000).
In this paper, we assess the influence of these two motivations in opinion formation on
political controversy. We do so by considering the effect of two voter attitudes: (1) a voter’s
underlying tolerance of political misconduct and (2) his or her partisanship. If a voter’s
tolerance of political misconduct in general predicts reactions to political controversy, then
voter opinions are shaped by accuracy goals: their view on the specific episode is consistent
with the toughness of their underlying normative stance towards political misbehaviour.
In contrast, if partisanship predicts reactions to the episode, then a voter’s opinion is
shaped by directional goals. It is well-known that partisanship, also known as party
identification or party affect, has an important impact on how voters react to and interpret
political events (e.g. Campbell et al. 1960; Zaller 1992; Bartels 2002; Johnston 2006). This
has been elaborated on in more recent research on motivated reasoning and selective
processing (Lodge and Taber 2000), also in relation to political scandals (Fischle 2000). In the
case of political controversy, we would expect partisanship to exert a directional effect on
voter reasoning: if the politician is from the party the voter is attached to, then the voter’s
judgment may be more lenient, whereas if the politician is from a rival party, then the voter’s
judgment may be tougher (Chang and Kerr 2009).
Assessing whether voter opinions are shaped more by general normative standards or
by pre-existing sympathies is important for two reasons. First, from a normative point of view
we want to know what the sources of public disapproval of politician’s behaviour are. Ideally,
voters would base their reactions not on partisan or other types of affect, but on careful
consideration of the facts about the politician’s conduct and by benchmarking this conduct
against their personal moral or ethical expectations of politicians. Second, knowing the extent
to which the two goals shape opinions may help us understand the course of political
controversy. If public reactions depend on the extent to which generally held ethical norms

4
were violated, the politicians should be more likely to face calls for resignation depending on
how normatively wrong their actions were. However, if public reactions depend largely on
affect, then the popularity of the politician’s party (and his or her own popularity) will
become important.
Finally, we also consider whether the influence of directional goals depends on a
voter’s political awareness and knowledge (Shani 2006; Blais et al. 2010). On the one hand, it
may be that low levels of political sophistication mean that the reliance on partisanship as a
guide may be stronger; on the other hand, it may be precisely the more sophisticated voters
who have the motivation and ability to ‘refuse to internalise messages that they recognize as
inconsistent with their underlying predispositions’ (Zaller 1992: 121).
We carry out our empirical analysis using data from a specially conducted nationally
representative internet survey conducted in the UK in May 2011 that measured voter reactions
to three recent or on-going controversial episodes involving prominent politicians: the
Conservative Ken Clarke and the Liberal Democrats Chris Huhne and David Laws. Our
research design has a number of advantages for investigating voter reaction to political
controversy. Even though the three episodes of political controversy that we study are
different in nature, they all were marked by widespread public disapproval of the event and
ran the risk that the revelations would damage the participant’s reputation. They are thus
comparable episodes and fit Thompson’s (2000:13-14) admittedly broad definition of a
political scandal. Secondly, we use actual episodes involving real-life politicians as opposed
to hypothetical scenarios, which should increase the likelihood that responses will reflect
genuine feelings about the event as opposed to reactions to an ‘imagined’ scenario. We
therefore do not have to temper our findings with consideration of the impact of hypothetical
scenarios on voter response. Finally, each episode of political controversy took place very
near to the time of the survey or was currently on-going when the survey was fielded. This

Citations
More filters
Book ChapterDOI
01 Jan 1992

405 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the authors re-analyze Gerber and Huber's original data and collect new data from two additional U.S. presidential elections since the original study similarly shows no evidence of an effect.
Abstract: Partisans report different perceptions from the same set of facts. According to the perceptual screen hypothesis, this difference arises because partisans perceive different realities. An alternative hypothesis is that partisans take even fact-based questions as an opportunity to voice support for their team. In 2009, Gerber and Huber conducted the first behavioral test of the perceptual screen hypothesis outside of the lab. I re-analyze Gerber and HuberÂ’s original data and collect new data from two additional U.S. elections. Gerber and HuberÂ’s finding of a relationship between partisanship and economic behavior does not hold when observations from a single state-year (Texas in 1996) are excluded from their analysis. Out-of-sample replication based on the two U.S. presidential elections since the original study similarly shows no evidence of an effect. Given these results, the balance of evidence tips toward the conclusion that economic perceptions are not filtered through partisanship.

52 citations


Cites background from "Partisan bias in opinion formation ..."

  • ..., Enns and McAvoy, 2012; Evans and Pickup, 2010; Stanig, 2013), perception of corruption (Anduiza et al., 2013; Wagner et al., 2014), perception of government responsibility (Bisgaard, 2015) and factual beliefs regarding policy issues (e....

    [...]

  • ...…in perception of the economy (e.g., Enns and McAvoy, 2012; Evans and Pickup, 2010; Stanig, 2013), perception of corruption (Anduiza et al., 2013; Wagner et al., 2014), perception of government responsibility (Bisgaard, 2015) and factual beliefs regarding policy issues (e.g., Joslyn and…...

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors analyse whether and to what extent voters punish incumbents for high levels of corruption using data from the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems, and they find that while voters perceiving high level of corruption punish incumbent, corruption performance voting depends on individual-level attributes and the electoral context: it is most likely for nonpartisans, for voters who believe that government turnover will bring about change, and in systems where corruption is a salient issue.
Abstract: Fighting corruption is a vital aspect of good governance. When assessing government performance voters should thus withdraw electoral support from government parties that turn a blind eye to or even engage in corrupt practices. Whereas most accounts of performance-based voting focus on economic outcomes, we analyse whether and to what extent voters punish incumbents for high levels of corruption. Using data from the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems, we find that while voters perceiving high levels of corruption punish incumbents, corruption performance voting depends on individual-level attributes and the electoral context: it is most likely for non-partisans, for voters who believe that government turnover will bring about change, and in systems where corruption is a salient issue. Yet, corruption performance voting is not moderated by the clarity of political responsibility. Studying these conditions helps us to understand why corruption is more persistent in some contexts than in others.

49 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This article conducted an analysis of survey data on three ballot measures in Washington State, testing hypothesized relationships between voters' empirical beliefs about political issues, news and campaign messages, political knowledge, political values, and partisanship, as well as vote choices on the ballot measures.
Abstract: Objectives Voters develop not only different opinions about politics but also different sets of empirical beliefs. It is less clear how falsifiable beliefs take hold. In particular, it remains unclear as to whether news and campaign messages, moderated by political knowledge, drive the process, or whether deep-seated values principally sway voters' acceptance of factual claims. These contrasting views point to a set of testable hypotheses that we use to refine a model of ideologically-biased empirical belief generation, which we call “knowledge distortion.” Methods We conduct an analysis of survey data on three ballot measures in Washington State, testing hypothesized relationships between voters' empirical beliefs about political issues, news and campaign messages, political knowledge, political values, and partisanship, as well as vote choices on the ballot measures. Results Our analysis reveals that voters' values and partisanship had the strongest associations with distorted beliefs, which then influenced voting choices. Self-reported levels of exposure to media and campaign messages played a surprisingly limited role. Conclusion Our findings provide further evidence of politically motivated factual misperceptions on political issues, which have an independent effect on voters' ballot decisions. These misperceptions do not seem to be driven by news media and campaign messages, suggesting that citizens may be generating relevant empirical beliefs based on their underlying political values and ideology.

34 citations

Journal Article
TL;DR: The first attempt to examine the effects of political scandals via meta-analysis was made by as mentioned in this paper, who examined the effect of scandal types on political scandal effects via meta analysis and found that five central moderators (candidate characteristics, behaviors, prior attitudes, context and scandal type) significantly influence scandal effects.
Abstract: This article represents the first attempt to examine the effects of political scandals via meta-analysis Seventy-eight studies, collectively including more than 54,000 participants, were identified and examined A quantitative analysis revealed that the number of studies has steadily increased Research predominantly stems from North America and Europe, and more than two-thirds of studies are based on student samples Publication outlets are mostly political science and psychology journals, whereas communication journals play only a minor role A qualitative analysis shows that two central outcome variables are frequently studied (evaluation of politicians/electoral consequences) Overall, studies generally reveal negative evaluative effects for politicians However, five central moderators (candidate characteristics, behaviors, prior attitudes, context, and scandal type) significantly influence scandal effects It is also apparent that research has largely neglected to precisely conceptualize the major independent variable in scandal-effects studies: news coverage and its intensity Central research gaps are identified, and avenues for future research are discussed

22 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
Ziva Kunda1
TL;DR: It is proposed that motivation may affect reasoning through reliance on a biased set of cognitive processes--that is, strategies for accessing, constructing, and evaluating beliefs--that are considered most likely to yield the desired conclusion.
Abstract: It is proposed that motivation may affect reasoning through reliance on a biased set of cognitive processes—that is, strategies for accessing, constructing, and evaluating beliefs. The motivation to be accurate enhances use of those beliefs and strategies that are considered most appropriate, whereas the motivation to arrive at particular conclusions enhances use of those that are considered most likely to yield the desired conclusion. There is considerable evidence that people are more likely to arrive at conclusions that they want to arrive at, but their ability to do so is constrained by their ability to construct seemingly reasonable justifications for these conclusions. These ideas can account for a wide variety of research concerned with motivated reasoning. The notion that goals or motives affect reasoning has a long and controversial history in social psychology. The propositions that motives may affect perceptions (Erdelyi, 1974), attitudes (Festinger, 1957), and attributions (Heider, 1958) have been put forth by some psychologists and challenged by others. Although early researchers and theorists took it for granted that motivation may cause people to make self-serving attributions and permit them to believe what they want to believe because they want to believe it, this view, and the research used to uphold it, came under concentrated criticism in the 1970s. The major and most damaging criticism of the motivational view was that all research purported to demonstrate motivated reasoning could be reinterpreted in entirely cognitive, nonmotivational terms (Miller & Ross, 1975; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Thus people could draw self-serving conclusions not because they wanted to but because these conclusions seemed more plausible, given their prior beliefs and expectancies. Because both cognitive and motivational accounts could be generated for any empirical study, some theorists argued that the hot versus cold cognition controversy could not be solved, at least in the attribution paradigm (Ross & Fletcher, 1985; Tetlock & Levi, 1982). One reason for the persistence of this controversy lies in the failure of researchers to explore the mechanisms underlying motivated reasoning. Recently, several authors have attempted to rectify this neglect (Kruglanski & Freund, 1983; Kunda, 1987; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1987; Sorrentino & Higgins, 1986). All these authors share a view of motivation as having its effects through cognitive processes: People rely on cognitive processes and representations to arrive at their desired conclusions, but motivation plays a role in determining which of these will be used on a given occasion.

6,643 citations


"Partisan bias in opinion formation ..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Voters form their opinions on the moral acceptability and appropriate consequences of a politician’s actions in the same way they form opinions about other political events: through a process of motivated reasoning (Kunda 1990; Lodge and Taber 2000)....

    [...]

  • ...Motivated reasoning and episodes of political controversy Individuals generally develop political opinions through the cognitive process known as motivated reasoning (Kunda 1990, Lodge and Taber 2000)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A survey of the top three political science journals from 1998 to 2002 suggests that the execution of these models is often flawed and inferential errors are common as discussed by the authors, and that scholars follow the simple checklist of dos and don'ts for using multiplicative interaction models presented in this article.
Abstract: Multiplicative interaction models are common in the quantitative political science literature. This is so for good reason. Institutional arguments frequently imply that the relationship between political inputs and outcomes varies depending on the institutional context. Models of strategic interaction typically produce conditional hypotheses as well. Although conditional hypotheses are ubiquitous in political science and multiplicative interaction models have been found to capture their intuition quite well, a survey of the top three political science journals from 1998 to 2002 suggests that the execution of these models is often flawed and inferential errors are common. We believe that considerable progress in our understanding of the political world can occur if scholars follow the simple checklist of dos and don'ts for using multiplicative interaction models presented in this article. Only 10% of the articles in our survey followed the checklist.

5,235 citations


"Partisan bias in opinion formation ..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...10 We present results graphically following the recommendations of Brambor et al. (2006)....

    [...]

Book
27 Mar 1996
TL;DR: Carpini and Keeter as mentioned in this paper found that whites, men, and older, financially secure citizens have substantially more knowledge about national politics than do blacks, women, young adults, and financially less-well-off citizens.
Abstract: This book is the most comprehensive analysis ever written about the American public's factual knowledge of politics. Drawing on extensive survey data, including much that is original, two experts in public opinion and political behavior find that many citizens are remarkably well informed about the details of politics, while equally large numbers are nearly ignorant of political facts. And despite dramatic changes in American society and politics, citizens appear no more or less informed today than half a century ago. Michael X. Delli Carpini and Scott Keeter demonstrate that informed persons are more likely to participate, better able to discern their own interests, and more likely to advocate those interests through political actions. Who, then, is politically informed? The authors provide compelling evidence that whites, men, and older, financially secure citizens have substantially more knowledge about national politics than do blacks, women, young adults, and financially less-well-off citizens. Thus citizens who are most disadvantaged socially and economically are least able to redress their grievances politically. Yet the authors believe that a broader and more equitably informed populace is possible. The challenge to America, they conclude, lies in providing an environment in which the benefits of being informed are clearer, the tools for gaining information more accessible, and the opportunities to learn about politics more frequent, timely, and equitable.

4,123 citations


"Partisan bias in opinion formation ..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Relatively unsophisticated voters will not be interested enough in politics to put much effort into reaching a ‘correct’ conclusion (Delli-Carpini and Keeter, 1996: 114, Lodge and Taber 2000, Shani 2006)....

    [...]

Posted Content
TL;DR: This article offers an approach, built on the technique of statistical simulation, to extract the currently overlooked information from any statistical method and to interpret and present it in a reader-friendly manner.
Abstract: Social Scientists rarely take full advantage of the information available in their statistical results. As a consequence, they miss opportunities to present quantities that are of greatest substantive interest for their research and express the appropriate degree of certainty about these quantities. In this article, we offer an approach, built on the technique of statistical simulation, to extract the currently overlooked information from any statistical method and to interpret and present it in a reader-friendly manner. Using this technique requires some expertise, which we try to provide herein, but its application should make the results of quantitative articles more informative and transparent. To illustrate our recommendations, we replicate the results of several published works, showing in each case how the authors' own conclusions can be expressed more sharply and informatively, and, without changing any data or statistical assumptions, how our approach reveals important new information about the research questions at hand. We also offer very easy-to-use Clarify software that implements our suggestions.

2,938 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, a model of motivated skepticism is proposed to explain when and why citizens are biased-information processors, and two experimental studies explore how citizens evaluate arguments about affirmative action and gun control, finding strong evidence of a prior attitude effect such that attitudinally congruent arguments are evaluated as stronger than attitudes incongruent arguments.
Abstract: We propose a model of motivated skepticism that helps explain when and why citizens are biased-information processors. Two experimental studies explore how citizens evaluate arguments about affirmative action and gun control, finding strong evidence of a prior attitude effect such that attitudinally congruent arguments are evaluated as stronger than attitudinally incongruent arguments. When reading pro and con arguments, participants (Ps) counterargue the contrary arguments and uncritically accept supporting arguments, evidence of a disconfirmation bias. We also find a confirmation bias—the seeking out of confirmatory evidence—when Ps are free to self-select the source of the arguments they read. Both the confirmation and disconfirmation biases lead to attitude polarization—the strengthening of t2 over t1 attitudes—especially among those with the strongest priors and highest levels of political sophistication. We conclude with a discussion of the normative implications of these findings for rational behavior in a democracy.

2,354 citations


"Partisan bias in opinion formation ..." refers background in this paper

  • ...This motivation already manifests itself in how individuals collect information: those guided by directional goals actively seek out facts that supports their predispositions (confirmation bias), see confirmatory arguments as compelling and opposing arguments as unsatisfactory (prior attitude effect) and use their reasoning powers to argue against arguments that contradict their desired conclusion (disconfirmation bias) (Olson and Zanna 1993; Taber and Lodge 2006)....

    [...]

  • ...…shown to have a large impact on how voters process information, form opinions and perceive facts (Campbell et al. 1960, Zaller 1992, Fischle 2000, Taber and Lodge 2006, Gaines et al. 2007, Nyhan and Reifler 2009), also outside the United States (Evans and Andersen 2006; Marsh and Tilley 2010,…...

    [...]

  • ...This indicates that the arguments made by Taber and Lodge (2006) and Shani (2006) have a strong foundation, also in a non-US context....

    [...]

  • ...…(confirmation bias), see confirmatory arguments as compelling and opposing arguments as unsatisfactory (prior attitude effect) and use their reasoning powers to argue against arguments that contradict their desired conclusion (disconfirmation bias) (Olson and Zanna 1993; Taber and Lodge 2006)....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (2)
Q1. What are the contributions mentioned in the paper "Partisan bias in opinion formation on episodes of political controversy: evidence from great britain abstract voters form judgments about political controversies through a process of motivated reasoning" ?

The authors use voters ’ underlying tolerance of political misconduct as an indicator of accuracy goals and party identification as a measure of directional goals. The authors find that partisan predispositions and tolerance of political misconduct are both important in shaping voter opinions and that partisanship has the strongest influence among the more knowledgeable and interested voters. These findings further their understanding of how voters react to political controversies and how they process new political information. 

Future research could investigate further how partisanship influences how voters react to new information, especially in more complex situations characterised by multiparty systems and coalition governments. Overall, their findings have highlighted both the relevance of general normative standards and the impact of partisanship on voter reactions to political controversy, and future work should consider when, how and why the influence of partisanship can vary.