scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Processing Tense/Aspect-Agreement Violations On-Line in the Second Language: A Self-Paced Reading Study with French and German L2 Learners of English.

06 Nov 2013-Second Language Research (SAGE Publications)-Vol. 29, Iss: 4, pp 413-439
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors report the results of a self-paced reading experiment designed to investigate the question of whether or not advanced French and German learners of English as a second language (EASL) can read in a selfpaced manner.
Abstract: In this article, we report the results of a self-paced reading experiment designed to investigate the question of whether or not advanced French and German learners of English as a second language ...

Summary (5 min read)

Introduction

  • This is a repository copy of Processing tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the second language : a self-paced reading study with French and German L2 learners of English.
  • The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version.

Keywords

  • Aspect, syntactic processing, tense, transfer Corresponding author: Leah Roberts, Centre for Language Learning Research, Department of Education, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD, UK.
  • From earlier research, in particular data from corpus studies and acceptability judgment tasks, the authors know much about learners’ production of tense/aspect distinctions and of tense/aspect morphology in development both at early and more advanced stages of L2 acquisition, and more recently about how L2 learners interpret target language tense/aspect distinctions in general.
  • Initially, the cat ate/*has eaten only fish.
  • Both French and German have a compound past (the passé composé/the Perfekt)1 which is similar in surface form to the present perfect in English (have + past participle).
  • Before reporting the details of the current experiment and the results, the authors consider what is meant by implicit and explicit knowledge.

1 Implicit and explicit knowledge

  • The distinction between and knowledge refers to whether or not that knowledge is intuitive and available for automatic processing , or consciously available through effortful, controlled processing (e.g. R. Ellis et al., 2006; Hulstijn, 2005).
  • According to Tokowicz and MacWhinney (2005: 178) types of explicit knowledge include similarities between the L1–L2 pairings, and explicit grammar rules, which can be exploited by L2 learners when making linguistic judgements.
  • As such, experimental tasks such as off-line grammaticality judgment tasks (GJTs) are typically considered conducive to testing and measuring this type of knowledge (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005; Tokowicz and MacWhinney, 2005).
  • On the other hand, as learners cannot consciously tap into their implicit knowledge, on-line tasks such as real-time spontaneous oral production tasks (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005) and event-related potential (ERP) responses in sentence comprehension tasks (e.g. Tokowicz and MacWhinney, 2005) are considered appropriate for measuring implicit knowledge (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005).
  • The authors test explicit knowledge using an untimed GJT and implicit knowledge using a self-paced reading task in an attempt to give a better understanding of how learners use their knowledge in on-and off-line comprehension.

2 Temporal adverbs and temporal verbal morphology

  • Languages differ in how these temporal relations are established, ranging from languages with mandatory marking of tense and aspect, to those with no such marking but a full repertoire of temporal adverbials.
  • French also marks aspect, albeit differently to English.
  • (‘Maria slept’) perfective b. French: Maria a dormi.
  • French, on the other hand, has one form underlying the two meanings: the compound past encodes T[+past] for preterit meaning and T[–past] and Asp[+perfect] for present perfect meaning.

3 Acquisition of tense/aspect in the L2

  • The L2 acquisition of tense/aspect has been investigated from both functional and formal perspectives.
  • Researchers working within the functional tradition have been interested in how learners establish temporal relations rather than in their ability to acquire the formal morphological marking of tense/aspect (e.g. Dietrich et al., 1995; Giacalone Ramat, 1992; Klein and Perdue, 1992; Skiba and Dittmar, 1992; Starren, 2001).
  • In turn, this contributes to the discussion on how much, if any, the L1 influences the acquisition of abstract underlying grammatical properties at the level of parametric variation between the L1 and L2.
  • Turning to the present perfect, Liszka (2004, 2005) tested L1 Chinese, German and Japanese speakers of advanced L2 English on the acquisition of the English present perfect, using a form-interpretation task.
  • The Japanese alternated mainly between past simple use (55%) and present use (38%), with the remaining 7% made up of other forms.

4 Comprehending temporal relations on-line

  • One way to investigate learners’ implicit grammatical knowledge is to use methods that can tap into comprehenders’ moment-by-moment processing of sentences, while they are reading the input for comprehension (for an overview of L2 processing, see Roberts, 2012).
  • Last week, James went/*has gone swimming every day.
  • Of interest to the current experiment with L2 learners is how the processor handles mismatches between a fronted temporal adverb and a morphological marker of tense/aspect, and there are very few studies that have focused on the consequences of processing temporal or tense/aspect violations on-line.
  • Afgelopen zondag lakte Vincent de kozijnen van zijn landhuis.
  • Last Sunday Vincent painted the window frames of his country house.

II The current study

  • In the current study the authors investigate the knowledge of the English past simple and present perfect by advanced French and German L2 learners; specifically, they ask whether the learners are able to access and apply this knowledge in the on-line processing of English sentences with tense–aspect violations.
  • The underlying assumption is that if the learners have fully acquired the semantics underlying the morphological marking of tense and aspect, then they should be sensitive to the mismatch between a fronted temporal adverbial and the tensed clause that follows in both off- and on-line comprehension.

1 Participants

  • The learners were asked to read 10 sets of 6 sentences in which the verb was missing, and then to choose the correct verb from a set of infinitival forms, inserting it in its correct morphological form .
  • Twenty French and 20 German L2 learners who all scored above 60% on the task were selected.
  • The accuracy for both groups was above chance.
  • All of the French learners and the majority of the German learners were studying English at university in their home country.

2 Materials

  • Twenty-four past simple (11) and 24 present perfect (12) experimental items were created .7.
  • The first was the critical sentence and it contained a temporal adverbial (a prepositional phrase or adverbial expression) in the topic position, thus modifying the time being talked about (the Topic Time, TT; see Klein, 1994).
  • Last week, James went swimming every day.
  • Mismatch b. * Since last week, James went swimming every day, also known as Past simple.

3 Tasks and procedure

  • For the main experiment, two tasks were undertaken: an off-line acceptability judgment task measuring explicit knowledge and a self-paced reading experiment to tap into implicit knowledge.
  • Each session began with a cross in the centre of the screen.
  • All of the experimental items and half of the fillers were followed by a yes/no comprehension question, requiring equal numbers of ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses (e.g.
  • The L2 learners were also required to complete the cloze production task and the English proficiency test (The Oxford Placement Test; Allan, 1992).
  • This feedback was provided at least three weeks after the participants had fully completed the study.

4 Results

  • For all analyses, an ANOVA was run on the data, with the within-participants factor Type (match/mismatch) and the between-participants factor Group with 3 levels (native English speakers/German L2 learners/French L2 learners).
  • This factor was treated as a within-participants factor in the items analyses.
  • The authors report the results below separately for each because the critical regions differ by one word between the past simple and the present perfect, as the latter comprises an auxiliary plus past participle.

5 Acceptability judgment task

  • Table 1 shows the three groups’ mean responses on the acceptability judgment task for the past simple items.
  • Post-hoc Tukey HSD test found no significant differences between the groups (ps > .07).

6 Self-paced reading task

  • Before analyses were run on the reading time data, to remove outlying data points, responses that fell 2 standard deviations away from an individual’s mean were removed per segment that was analysed, affecting 1.22% of the English, 0.92% of the French and 1.23% of the German data.
  • There were no other significant effects following this segment.9.
  • As with the past simple items, analyses were run on the segments from the VP (here starting with the auxiliary verb) and then across the three segments following it, the past participle and the two subsequent words.
  • Table 4 shows the mean reading times for these conditions, and the processing cost effects are visualized in Figure 2.

IV General discussion

  • The authors presented the results from an off-line acceptability judgment task and an on-line self-paced reading study designed to investigate the explicit and implicit knowledge of English tense/aspect violations of German and French L2 learners.
  • The main results are summarized as follows: .
  • Thus they all demonstrated their explicit knowledge of the English past simple and present perfect.
  • At University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from Roberts and Liszka 427 Despite this, the two L2 groups patterned differently from each other in their online processing of the experimental sentences.
  • The French L2 learners’ process- ing reflected their off-line, metalinguistic judgments: they found the mismatch conditions more difficult to process than the match conditions of both the past simple and the present perfect items.

1 L1 influences

  • The major finding of the current study was that the French but not the German L2 learners were sensitive during on-line processing to the mismatch between the fronted temporal adverbial and the inflected verb.
  • If indeed L1 influence underlies the observed on-line differences in the current study, the question then arises as to what it is that is transferred between the L1 and the L2.
  • If the learners were directly interpreting the English sentences according to their L1 grammar, this could explain the difference between the two L2 learners in their processing of the past simple items.
  • Turning now to the present perfect, for the French, as [+/−perfect] is specified unlike in German, the authors might expect the mismatch conditions to be more difficult to process than the match conditions.
  • Depuis la semaine dernière je suis malade, also known as present perfect b. French.

2 Native speakers’ processing of past simple vs. present perfect violations

  • Another striking finding of this study was the unexpected processing cost asymmetry that was observed in the native speakers’ processing of the experimental items.
  • That is, it was only the mismatch condition in the present perfect sentences that caused a processing cost for the native speakers (Last year, Jill has wanted … vs. *Since last year, Jill wanted), even though both mismatch conditions were assessed as significantly less acceptable than the corresponding match conditions in the off-line acceptability judgment task.
  • The authors suggest that it is because the present tense component in the present perfect constructions has wanted means the time that is being talked about (TT) includes the time of the utterance and, therefore, one cannot use an adverbial that singles out a specific time in the past (last year, yesterday, at five).
  • Also, the fact that this construction may be in the process of becoming more widely used in British English would explain why the French learners performed differently from the English native speakers with the past simple items: it is plausible to assume that a learner may need more and naturalistic exposure in the L2 environment to fully acquire such constructions.
  • What is of interest is that this difference was only observable in the native speakers’ on-line comprehension: when all interpretative processes are brought into play, and a metalinguistic judgment needed to be made, these items were classed as ‘unacceptable’, like the past simple items.

3 Further L2 processing research

  • Further research is necessary to address many issues that are raised in this study.
  • It is not clear what the L2 learners’ and the native speakers’ final interpretations for the mismatch conditions were, because the authors did not specifically ask the participants for their interpretations of either the match or the mismatch sentences.
  • One possibility is that because lexical means for expressing temporal relations characterize early learners’ tense/aspect productions, seemingly irrespective of the properties of the L1 (e.g. Starren, 2001), then such lexical means may ultimately exert more influence in L2 learners’ interpretations Perhaps in contrast, in monolingual processing the morphology ‘wins out’.
  • 14 at University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from Roberts and Liszka 431.

V Conclusions

  • Based on the results of this study, it was argued that the learners’.
  • In addition to an effect of direct transfer, i.e. whether or not the L1 encodes Asp[+/−perfect], it was further suggested that indirect transfer also plays a role.
  • This was proposed to account for the results in terms of the differences between English and French, and similarities between French and German, complementing the direct transfer account, which considers the similarities between English and French and the differences with German.
  • It was argued that whether or not the learners’.
  • L1 has grammaticized aspect or not more generally may underlie the differences in sensitivity to tense/aspect agreement violations, rather than the effect being driven only by the L1 and the L2 making exactly the same distinctions.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

This is a repository copy of Processing tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the
second language : a self-paced reading study with French and German L2 learners of
English.
White Rose Research Online URL for this paper:
https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/77040/
Version: Submitted Version
Article:
Roberts, Leah orcid.org/0000-0002-5666-6667 and Liszka, Sarah (2013) Processing
tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the second language : a self-paced reading
study with French and German L2 learners of English. Second Language Research. 413–
439. ISSN 0267-6583
https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658313503171
eprints@whiterose.ac.uk
https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/
Reuse
Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless
indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by
national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of
the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record
for the item.
Takedown
If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by
emailing eprints@whiterose.ac.uk including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request.

http://slr.sagepub.com/
Second Language Research
http://slr.sagepub.com/content/29/4/413
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0267658313503171
2013 29: 413Second Language Research
Leah Roberts and Sarah Ann Liszka
English
oflanguage: A self-paced reading study with French and German L2 learners
Processing tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the second
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
can be found at:Second Language ResearchAdditional services and information for
http://slr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://slr.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://slr.sagepub.com/content/29/4/413.refs.htmlCitations:
What is This?
- Nov 6, 2013Version of Record >>
at University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Second Language Research
29(4) 413 –439
© The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0267658313503171
slr.sagepub.com
second
language
research
Processing tense/aspect-
agreement violations on-
line in the second language:
A self-paced reading study
with French and German L2
learners of English
Leah Roberts
University of York, UK
Sarah Ann Liszka
University of Greenwich, UK
Abstract
In this article, we report the results of a self-paced reading experiment designed to investigate
the question of whether or not advanced French and German learners of English as a second
language (L2) are sensitive to tense/aspect mismatches between a fronted temporal adverbial and
the inflected verb that follows (e.g. *Last week, James has gone swimming every day) in their on-line
comprehension. The L2 learners were equally able to distinguish correctly the past simple from
the present perfect as measured by a traditional cloze test production task. They were also both
able to assess the mismatch items as less acceptable than the match items in an off-line judgment
task. Using a self-paced reading task, we investigated whether they could access this knowledge
during real-time processing. Despite performing similarly in the explicit tasks, the two learner
groups processed the experimental items differently from each other in real time. On-line, only
the French L2 learners were sensitive to the mismatch conditions in both the past simple and the
present perfect contexts, whereas the German L2 learners did not show a processing cost at all
for either mismatch type. We suggest that the performance differences between the L2 groups
can be explained by influences from the learners’ first language (L1): namely, only those whose L1
has grammaticized aspect (French) were sensitive to the tense/aspect violations on-line, and thus
could be argued to have implicit knowledge of English tense/aspect distinctions.
Keywords
aspect, syntactic processing, tense, transfer
Corresponding author:
Leah Roberts, Centre for Language Learning Research, Department of Education, University of York,
Heslington, York YO10 5DD, UK.
Email: leah.roberts@york.ac.uk
503171SLR29410.1177/0267658313503171Second Language ResearchRoberts and Liszka
2013
Article
at University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

414 Second Language Research 29(4)
I Introduction
In the field of second language (L2) acquisition research, the acquisition of tense and
aspect has long been a topic of intensive investigation (for overviews, see for example
Bardovi-Harlig, 2000; Slabakova, 2002). This is unsurprising given that temporal
expression is fundamental to communication, yet its formal and functional marking
varies dramatically from language to language. The acquisition of tense and aspect is
therefore a core task for all language learners. From earlier research, in particular data
from corpus studies and acceptability judgment tasks, we know much about learners’
production of tense/aspect distinctions and of tense/aspect morphology in develop-
ment both at early and more advanced stages of L2 acquisition, and more recently
about how L2 learners interpret target language tense/aspect distinctions in general.
However, little is known about the nature of learners’ knowledge of the grammatical
marking of tense and aspect once it is observed off-line. In other words, it is unclear
whether the knowledge that learners display can be applied automatically in real-time
comprehension. This is a topic that can be conceptualized in terms of the debate in the
field on the nature of ‘implicit’ versus ‘explicit’ knowledge (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005), and is
an important issue because language acquisition is a complex task that requires both
the acquisition of knowledge as well as the ability to put this knowledge to use in real
time. Focusing on the latter, in this article we investigate whether French and German
L2 learners of English are sensitive to mismatches in agreement between fronted tem-
poral adverbials (e.g. Initially/Since last week) and the tense/aspect of immediately
following verbs (ate/has eaten) as shown in (1), in their on-line comprehension of the
target language.
(1) a. Initially, the cat ate/*has eaten only fish.
b. Since last week, the cat *ate/has eaten only fish.
Both French and German have a compound past (the passé composé/the Perfekt)
1
which
is similar in surface form to the present perfect in English (have + past participle).
However, both French and German contrast with English in that the English present
perfect cannot be used with an adverbial specifying definite past time (e.g. Yesterday I
have danced all night; cf. ‘the present perfect puzzle’, Klein, 1992). On the other hand,
French and English pattern together and both differently from German in that aspect is
grammaticized in the former, but not in the latter language, a difference that may impact
L2 processing (Papadopoulou et al., 2008). In the current study, we investigate the poten-
tial role of the learners’ first language in their ability to put their (explicit) knowledge of
English tense and aspect – as measured by traditional off-line production tasks – to use
in on-line comprehension.
Before reporting the details of the current experiment and the results, we consider
what is meant by implicit and explicit knowledge. We then briefly present an overview
on the cross-linguistic expression of tense/aspect, summarize findings on the study of the
acquisition of tense/aspect in the L2 with particular reference to first language (L1) influ-
ences, and present a brief review of the processing of tense/aspect in real-time
comprehension.
at University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Roberts and Liszka 415
1 Implicit and explicit knowledge
The distinction between implicit and explicit knowledge refers to whether or not that
knowledge is intuitive and available for automatic processing (implicit), or con-
sciously available through effortful, controlled processing (explicit) (e.g. R. Ellis et
al., 2006; Hulstijn, 2005). According to Tokowicz and MacWhinney (2005: 178)
types of explicit knowledge include similarities between the L1–L2 pairings, and
explicit grammar rules, which can be exploited by L2 learners when making linguis-
tic judgements. As such, experimental tasks such as off-line grammaticality judg-
ment tasks (GJTs) are typically considered conducive to testing and measuring this
type of knowledge (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005; Tokowicz and MacWhinney, 2005). On the
other hand, as learners cannot consciously tap into their implicit knowledge, on-line
tasks such as real-time spontaneous oral production tasks (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005) and
event-related potential (ERP) responses in sentence comprehension tasks (e.g.
Tokowicz and MacWhinney, 2005) are considered appropriate for measuring implicit
knowledge (e.g. R. Ellis, 2005). In this spirit, we test explicit knowledge using an
untimed GJT and implicit knowledge using a self-paced reading task in an attempt to
give a better understanding of how learners use their knowledge in on-and off-line
comprehension.
2 Temporal adverbs and temporal verbal morphology
According to (Klein, 1994)
2
past, present and future tense is established by the temporal
relation between the topic time (TT) and the time of the utterance (TU), and this abstract
temporal relation may or may not be grammatically marked, for instance by verbal mor-
phology. Klein distinguishes the topic time from the situation time (TSit): both are time
spans, but the former relates to the time (past, present, future) for which an assertion is
made (e.g. I had danced – past time – before TU), and the latter, which is associated with
the non-finite part of the utterance, refers to the time span at which the situation occurs.
Aspect in this system is established by the relationship between the topic time and the
situation time, creating temporal relations such as BEFORE, AFTER, (partly)
SIMULTANEOUS (in ‘I had danced’, the TT is AFTER the time of the situation (TSit)
dance).
Lexical devices such as temporal adverbs can also mark such temporal relations.
Temporal adverbs can specify the internal and the external properties of a time span, and
also, as shown in (2) below they can modify either the TT (yesterday) or the TSit (all
night), depending on their position in a sentence.
(2) Yesterday, I slept all night.
Languages differ in how these temporal relations are established, ranging from languages
with mandatory marking of tense and aspect, to those with no such marking but a full
repertoire of temporal adverbials. For instance, and relevant to the current study, in
German, French, and English, tense is mandatorily marked grammatically. For example,
(3a–c) below demonstrate the present simple in English, French and German. In terms of
preterit meaning in English it is realized by the past simple (3d). French also has a past
at University of York on November 11, 2013slr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper found a reliable sensitivity to L2 morphosyntax at advanced proficiencies (d =.20, 95% CIs.15,.25), with some evidence that this was reliably lower than for native speakers (NSs).
Abstract: Despite extensive theoretical and empirical research, we do not have estimations of the magnitude of sensitivity to grammatical information during L2 online processing. This is largely due to reliance on null hypothesis significance testing (Plonsky, 2015). The current meta-analysis draws on data from one elicitation technique, self-paced reading, across 57 studies (N = 3,052), to estimate sensitivity to L2 morphosyntax and how far L1 background moderates this. Overall, we found a reliable sensitivity to L2 morphosyntax at advanced proficiencies (d = .20, 95% CIs .15, .25), with some evidence that this was reliably lower than for native speakers (NSs). These patterns were not generally moderated by linguistic feature or sentence region. However, effects for anomaly detection were larger among NSs than L2 learners and the effects among L2 learners appeared to show a trend toward L1 influence. Finding smaller effects than in other subdomains, we provide an initial framework of reference for L2 reading time effect sizes.

6 citations

Dissertation
09 Mar 2017
TL;DR: Six empirical studies investigating the processing of temporal violations through different designs and techniques are conducted, preceded by a theoretical chapter which deals with the description of Tense and deictic temporal adverbs from a semantic and syntactic point of view.
Abstract: The aim of this thesis deals with the investigation of the cognitive mechanisms underlying the online processing of adverb-verb temporal agreement, namely the coherence in temporal features between the verb and a deictic temporal adverb (e.g. YesterdayPAST I wentPAST/*will goFUT to the jazz concert), during sentence comprehension. There are at least two reasons that make the investigation of this phenomenon interesting and challenging at the same time. The first reason is more theoretical. Differently from well-studied phenomena such as subject-verb agreement or anaphora, the nature of the adverb-verb relation is still debated in theoretical linguistics. The debated nature of the adverb-verb temporal agreement probably relies on the peculiar properties of the constituents involved in the relation. The relation between temporal adverbs and verbs has been traditionally defined anaphoric in nature, since verbs can be bound to temporal antecedents (e.g. adverb) which allow to set a reference time that the event expressed by the verb must refer to. However, other accounts have hypothesized a structural parallelism between the adverb-verb and the subject-verb relation. One question is thus whether this similarities or dissimilarities between subject-verb agreement and temporal agreement at the theoretical level can mirror a similarity/dissimilarity at the cognitive level. The second reason is more empirical and comes from past experimental literature investigating the processing of temporal agreement. Basically, experimental evidence mainly comes from ERPs studies and results are rather sparse and heterogeneous. These ERP studies report an early detection of the temporal violation (around 200 milliseconds after the stimulus onset) but qualitatively different ERP waveforms were elicited by the target word (i.e. the verb) in the different experiments (e.g. LAN, N400, right lateralized negativities). One licit question is thus where the source of heterogeneity resides and how this phenomenon can be better investigated. Given the debated theoretical and experimental past evidence on the processing of this relation, the current work was conducted (i) investigating the pattern elicited by adverb-verb temporal violations compared to other better-studied agreement phenomena, as the one between the subject and the verb, during sentence comprehension (ii) adopting only behavioral techniques since a detailed investigation of the behavioral costs elicited by a temporal violation needs to be established prior to facing the detailed neurophysiological correlates of these processes, which are known to be subject to a larger interpretive freedom with respect to reading time differences. The core of the thesis, namely six empirical studies investigating the processing of temporal violations through different designs and techniques, is preceded by a theoretical chapter which deals with the description of Tense and deictic temporal adverbs from a semantic and syntactic point of view. The main aim of the theoretical chapter is to give a brief overview of the main linguistic theories which have investigated the nature of Tense and temporal adverbs, but also a motivation for considering the syntactic interaction between Tense and temporal adverbs, which is fundamental to preserve the grammaticality of the sentence.The first set of self-paced reading studies, in Italian, addressed two main questions: how different is the processing of adverb-verb temporal agreement with respect to other better-studied phenomena such as subject-verb number agreement? Is the different configuration between the verb and the temporal adverb that has led to heterogeneous results in past experimental literature? In the second (eye-tracking) study, in Spanish, three other questions were addressed: how differently the parser deals with the processing of number, tense and (crucially) person features when encountering a violation on the inflected verb? Does the distance between the two constituents of the dependency play any role in the detection of the violation? Finally, in the third set of eye-tracking studies in English, the processing of the adverb-verb temporal relation was tested in a more complex sentential environment, namely in sentence where the temporal adverb and the verb are separated by an embedded relative clause containing a distracting temporal element. In this set of studies, several questions were addressed: how different can be the processing of adverb-verb temporal agreement at a conspicuous distance? Is the temporal adverb-verb relation sensitive to interference effects from an illicit intervener? How differently this relation behaves with respect to subject-verb agreement and anaphora during memory retrieval? All findings collected in this work provide further evidence for a differentiation in the processing of agreement mechanisms entailing a covariance of features between two constituents within a sentence. This evidence is in line with previous accounts showing a differentiation in the processing of different features (i.e. number, person) within the same relation such as subject-verb agreement (Mancini et al. 2013), and in the processing of the same feature (e.g. number) across different relations such as subject-verb agreement and anaphora (Dillon et al. 2013). This evidence can be particularly relevant for the development of a new model of sentence parsing. In fact, among mainstream models of parsing, only Construal (Frazier & Clifton, 1996) model assumes a relation-sensitive language system. However, a differentiation in the processing of agreement phenomena is not explicitly addressed in terms of feature-related properties. On the other hand, some recent accounts have proposed different processing mechanisms depending on the feature under computation (e.g. Mancini et al., 2013; Carminati, 2005) but a specific formalization of the role of different features properties within a model of parsing has not been provided yet. The second challenge that this current work tried to face was to add more complexity into the agreement configuration testing the adverb-verb agreement relation at different linear distance. The findings here collected seems to give positive evidence on the role played by word order in the processing of the adverb-verb relation, but further investigation needs to address whether other factors may play a role and whether adverb-verb agreement is the only agreement relation which is sensitive to word order. This puzzle thus opens new questions about whether the agreement relation may change even being both the relation and the feature under computation equal. The role played by linear distance in the detection of adverb-verb temporal anomalies also suggests that parsing routines are not “stagnant”, and the language system can deal with redundant information in a very dynamic fashion.

6 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the acquisition of Chinese imperfective markers (zai, -zheP and −zheR) by English-speaking learners at three proficiency levels was studied.
Abstract: This article reports on an empirical study on the acquisition of Chinese imperfective markers (zai, -zheP and -zheR) by English-speaking learners at three proficiency levels. Compared to English, C...

5 citations


Cites background from "Processing Tense/Aspect-Agreement V..."

  • ...Most of them are on L2 acquisition of Indo-European languages, such as English (Roberts and Liszka, 2013; Salaberry, 2000; Slabakova, 2000), Spanish (e.g. Chin, 2008; Domínguez et al., 2017; Montrul and Slabakova, 2002, 2003) and French (Izquierdo and Collins, 2008; McManus, 2015), while less…...

    [...]

  • ...Some recent studies (e.g. Domínguez et al., 2017; Gabriele, 2009; Gabriele and McClure, 2011; Roberts and Liszka, 2013) have shown that the aspectual properties and the way of aspectual coding in the L1 transferred into the L2 influence L2 acquisition....

    [...]

  • ...Roberts and Liszka (2013) report a self-paced reading study designed to investigate whether or not advanced French and German learners of English as an L2 are sensitive to tense/aspect mismatches between a fronted temporal adverbial and the inflected verb that follows (e.g. *Last week, James has…...

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper conducted a self-paced reading study with advanced German, Dutch and French second language learners of English and found that their online comprehension of early closure (EC) sentences which are initially misanalysed by native English speakers was affected by whether or not, like English, their first language (L1) encodes aspect grammatically (French) or only via lexical means (German, Dutch).
Abstract: The results of a self-paced reading study with advanced German, Dutch and French second language (L2) learners of English showed that their online comprehension of early closure (EC) sentences which are initially misanalysed by native English speakers (e.g. While John hunted the frightened rabbit escaped) was affected by whether or not, like English, their first language (L1) encodes aspect grammatically (French) or only via lexical means (German, Dutch). The English and the higher proficiency French participants showed a processing asymmetry in their online reading of the temporarily ambiguous sentences, assumed to be caused by the difference in the aspectual perspective a comprehender takes when initial verbs appear in the past simple vs. the past progressive. In contrast, the German and Dutch learners, irrespective of proficiency, treated both progressive and simple sentences in the same way, despite the fact that all the L2 learners were matched according to their metalinguistic knowledge of English aspectual distinctions. Furthermore, despite patterning with the German learners online, the Dutch L2 learners’ offline judgments were more akin to those of the English native speakers and the French L2 learners, showing an effect of aspect, which could be argued to lend support to the idea that progressive aspect may be becoming grammaticalized in Dutch. Taken together, the results of this study add to our growing understanding of cross-linguistic influences during online L2 sentence processing, and differences between L2 parsing and learners’ metalinguistic L2 performance.

5 citations


Cites background or methods or result from "Processing Tense/Aspect-Agreement V..."

  • ...Since French encodes the distinction via Asp[+/–perfective], albeit indirectly, and given the findings in Roberts and Liszka (2013), we predict their processing to be most similar to the English control group....

    [...]

  • ...…processes and acceptability judgments to assess explicit knowledge, were used to gain a more composite picture of learners’ knowledge of the constructions under investigation (Ellis, 2005; Ellis et al., 2006; Hulstijn, 2005; Rebuschat, 2015; Roberts and Liszka, 2013; Tokowicz and MacWhinney, 2005)....

    [...]

  • ...The French group were expected to perform most like the native speakers in the past progressive, whereas we predicted the Germans would not be affected by the progressive/simple distinction, since this was found in the online results of Roberts and Liszka (2013)....

    [...]

  • ...18 It may be that (post-puberty) L2 learners’ knowledge of distinctions not instantiated in their L1 may not become fully acquired, or may at least be delayed (Hawkins and Liszka, 2003; Roberts and Liszka, 2013)....

    [...]

  • ...The current study also investigates L2 implicit knowledge using SPR, but rather than using a violation paradigm (e.g. Hopp, 2010; Juffs and Harrington, 1995, 1996; Roberts and Liszka, 2013) we tested learners’ online commitments during processing....

    [...]

References
More filters
Book
01 Jan 1947

1,950 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This article is a defence of the Full Transfer/Full Access (FT/FA) model, arguing that the FT/FA model provides the most coherent picture of the L2 initial cognitive state.
Abstract: This article is a defence of the Full Transfer/Full Access (FT/FA) model. FT/FA hypothesizes that the initial state of L2 acquisition is the final state of L1 acquisition (Full Transfer) and that failure to assign a representation to input data will force subsequent restructurings, drawing from options of UG (Full Access). We illustrate the FT/FA model by reviewing our analysis of the developmental Turkish-German Interlanguage data of Schwartz and Sprouse (1994) and then turn to other data that similarly receive straightforward accounts under FT/FA.We also consider two other competing hypotheses, both of which accept Full Access but not Full Transfer: the Minimal Trees hypothesis (no transfer of functional categories) of Vainikka and Young-Scholten (1994; 1996) and the Weak Transfer hypothesis (no transfer of the values associated with functional categories) of Eubank (1993/94). We provide an example of (extremely robust) L2 acquisition data that highlight the inadequacy of the Minimal Trees hypothesis in...

1,464 citations


"Processing Tense/Aspect-Agreement V..." refers background in this paper

  • ...…to inform debates on whether Universal Grammar is fully available to older L2 learners (e.g. Lardiere, 1998a, 1998b, 2000; Prévost and White, 2000; Schwartz and Sprouse, 1996; Vainikka and Young-Scholten, 1996a) or whether it is only partially available (e.g. Hawkins and Chan, 1997; Hawkins and…...

    [...]

  • ...Such selective differences help to inform debates on whether Universal Grammar is fully available to older L2 learners (e.g. Lardiere, 1998a, 1998b, 2000; Prévost and White, 2000; Schwartz and Sprouse, 1996; Vainikka and Young-Scholten, 1996a) or whether it is only partially available (e....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This article reported on a new study of the effects of implicit and explicit corrective feedback on the acquisition of past tense -ed, which was measured by means of an oral imitation test (designed to measure implicit knowledge) and both an untimed grammaticality judgment test and a metalinguistic knowledge test (both designed to measure explicit knowledge).
Abstract: This article reviews previous studies of the effects of implicit and explicit corrective feedback on SLA, pointing out a number of methodological problems. It then reports on a new study of the effects of these two types of corrective feedback on the acquisition of past tense -ed. In an experimental design (two experimental groups and a control group), low-intermediate learners of second language English completed two communicative tasks during which they received either recasts (implicit feedback) or metalinguistic explanation (explicit feedback) in response to any utterance that contained an error in the target structure. Acquisition was measured by means of an oral imitation test (designed to measure implicit knowledge) and both an untimed grammaticality judgment test and a metalinguistic knowledge test (both designed to measure explicit knowledge). The tests were administered prior to the instruction, 1 day after the instruction, and again 2 weeks later. Statistical comparisons of the learners' performance on the posttests showed a clear advantage for explicit feedback over implicit feedback for both the delayed imitation and grammaticality judgment posttests. Thus, the results indicate that metalinguistic explanation benefited implicit as well as explicit knowledge and point to the importance of including measures of both types of knowledge in experimental studies.This research was funded by a Marsden Fund grant awarded by the Royal Society of Arts of New Zealand. Researchers other than the authors who contributed to the research were Jenefer Philip, Satomi Mizutami, Keiko Sakui, and Thomas Delaney. Thanks go to the editors of this special issue and to two anonymous SSLA reviewers of a draft of the article for their constructive comments.

920 citations


"Processing Tense/Aspect-Agreement V..." refers background in this paper

  • ...The distinction between implicit and explicit knowledge refers to whether or not that knowledge is intuitive and available for automatic processing (implicit), or consciously available through effortful, controlled processing (explicit) (e.g. R. Ellis et al., 2006; Hulstijn, 2005)....

    [...]

Book
01 Jan 1983
TL;DR: In this paper, a comprehensive analysis of sentence-level anaphora is presented, with a focus on the relations between the structural properties of sentences and their semantic interpretation within the hypotheses of the autonomy of syntax and of interpretative semantics.
Abstract: First published in 1983, this book examines anaphora — a central issue in linguistic theory as it lies at the crossroads of several major problems. On the one hand it is believed that the same conditions that govern the interpretation of anaphora also govern syntactic movement rules but on the other, while anaphora is known to interact with various discourse and semantic considerations, it also provides a clear instance of the dependency of the semantic interpretation of sentences upon semantic properties of natural language. This book has two major goals: the first is a comprehensive analysis of sentence-level anaphora that addresses the questions posed above, and the second is an examination of the broader issues of the relations between the structural properties of sentences and their semantic interpretation within the hypotheses of the autonomy of syntax and of interpretative semantics shown by Chomsky.

915 citations


"Processing Tense/Aspect-Agreement V..." refers result in this paper

  • ...These findings fit with the view that the fronted temporal adverbial functions as a topic (Chafe, 1984; Partee, 1984; Reinhart, 1983; Virtanen, 1992) and, as such, creates a new discourse segment.4 (8) a....

    [...]

Book
01 Jan 1994
TL;DR: This book looks at the various ways in which time is reflected in natural language - the verbal categories of tense and aspect; inherent lexical features of the verb; and various types of temporal adverbs.
Abstract: This book looks at the various ways in which time is reflected in natural language. All natural languages have developed a rich repetoire of devices to express time, but linguists have tended to concentrate on tense and aspect, rather than discourse principles. Klein considers the four main ways in which language expresses time - the verbal categories of tense and aspect; inherent lexical features of the verb; and various types of temporal adverbs. Klein looks at the interaction of these four devices and suggests new or partly new treatments of these devices to express temporality.

859 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (2)
Q1. What are the contributions in this paper?

In this article, the authors report the results of a self-paced reading experiment designed to investigate the question of whether or not advanced French and German learners of English as a second language ( L2 ) are sensitive to tense/aspect mismatches between a fronted temporal adverbial and the inflected verb that follows ( e. g. * Last week, James has gone swimming every day ) in their on-line comprehension. Using a self-paced reading task, the authors investigated whether they could access this knowledge during real-time processing. The authors suggest that the performance differences between the L2 groups can be explained by influences from the learners ’ first language ( L1 ): namely, only those whose L1 has grammaticized aspect ( French ) were sensitive to the tense/aspect violations on-line, and thus could be argued to have implicit knowledge of English tense/aspect distinctions. 

Further research is necessary to address many issues that are raised in this study. One possibility is that because lexical means for expressing temporal relations characterize early learners ’ tense/aspect productions, seemingly irrespective of the properties of the L1 ( e. g. Starren, 2001 ), then such lexical means may ultimately exert more influence in L2 learners ’ interpretations The authors leave these open questions to future research. This may be so even when the inflectional morphology appears to be in place, and can be used in real-time processing.