scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Psychological Science Is Not Race Neutral.

02 Mar 2021-Perspectives on Psychological Science (SAGE PublicationsSage CA: Los Angeles, CA)-pp 1745691620979820-1745691620979820
TL;DR: In their analysis in a previous issue of Perspectives on Psychological Science, Roberts and colleagues argued that the editors, authors, and participants throughout subfields of psychological science as mentioned in this paper were biased.
Abstract: In their analysis in a previous issue of Perspectives on Psychological Science, Roberts and colleagues argued that the editors, authors, and participants throughout subfields of psychological scien...

Summary (2 min read)

The Problem of Racial Ignorance

  • This reality clearly reveals one fundamental challenge facing.
  • White authors, editors, and participants in the study of race: Racial ignorance.
  • Beyond segregation, motivational factors heighten racial ignorance.
  • White people to, across many contexts, deny racial privileges that they incur as a function of their elevated status (Lowery, Knowles, & Unzueta, 2007) .

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 6

  • Together, these structural and motivational conditions produce a veil of ignorance among.
  • White researchers, editors, and participants that stands in the way of scholarship "tackl[ing] diverse questions from diverse perspectives" (Roberts et al., 2020, p. 9) .
  • White authors from seeing the relevance of race to a myriad of social and psychological experiences; it prevents Ultimately, this veil of ignorance produces research that reinforces the notion that psychological processes are race-neutral-a self-perpetuating cycle of racial ignorance.

The Problem of Belonging

  • Racial inequality in psychological science undermines the ability of underrepresented minority scholars of color to feel a sense of belonging within their field.
  • Belonging is a fundamental human need (Baumeister & Leary, 1994; Edmondson, 1999) , and social psychologists have long studied the importance of belonging for underrepresented minorities in higher education (Good, Rattan, & Dweck, 2012; Steele, 1988; Walton & Cohen, 2007) .
  • The vast underrepresentation for scholars of color in their field has negative interpersonal, cognitive, and institutional impacts on these scholars as they attempt to conduct research.
  • Interpersonally, stereotypes continue to depict Black Americans as socially (Dupree et al., in press; Lei & Bodenhausen, 2017) or biologically (Williams & Eberhardt, 2008) inferior.
  • Cognitively, Black scholars must contend with additional cognitive demands that stem from deciphering whether the behavior of others is benign, or motivated by racial animus (Trawalter & Richeson, 2006; Shelton & Richeson, 2006) .

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 7

  • Structurally, research on tokenism highlights how being the only member of a social identity group leads to inferences and observations suggesting that institutions are hostile toward members of that group (King, Hebl, George, & Matusik, 2010) .
  • Their typical work activities are overloaded by service requests as universities attempt to make good on promises of campus diversity (Rodriguez, Campell, & Adelson, 2015; Syed, 2017) .
  • Moreover, tokenism on journal editorial boards means that scholars of color face additional burdens in their editorial and reviewing responsibilities, sometimes being called upon to prevent the publication of scientific racism within their journals.
  • Together these conditions threaten a sense of belonging for scholars of color, preventing retention and full participation in their field.
  • Importantly, the problem of belonging constitutes both a cause and cost of the racial inequality in psychological science.

The Problem of Assumed Racial Progress

  • The magnitude of racial inequality described by Roberts et al., (2020) in psychological science was undoubtedly shocking to many readers.
  • Sustainable racial justice and diversity among tenured psychological scientists will mean explicitly contending with a history of racial inequality within their field-whose spaces, canon, and methods have all been exclusionary up to this point in history.
  • Continued belief in the natural and automatic unfolding of racial progress makes authentic and effective embrace of diversity in their field impossible.

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 9

  • To summarize, racial ignorance among White scholars (due to structural and motivational factors), a lack of belonging among scholars of color (who are subject to additional interpersonal, cognitive, and institutional demands), and a presumption of automatic racial progress all conspire to create and maintain racial inequality in their field.
  • This means increasing the number of editors, reviewers, authors, and participants that hail from under-represented groups.
  • Because people of color are less subject to racial ignorance and the presumption of racial progress, diversifying the editors, reviewers, authors, and participants involved in such work will attenuate the systemic inequality in psychology research.
  • Any combination of these concrete steps can reduce the racial inequality pervasive in their science and departments.

Conclusion

  • The authors field has two choices in interpreting the scholarship the authors have outlined here:.
  • The authors can continue with business as usual, ignoring the evidence suggesting that the racial inequalities outlined by Roberts et al., (2020) represent a significant and problematic organization of their subfields and its practices.
  • Or, the authors can proceed as scientists, taking to heart the available evidence suggesting that these conditions damage their collective capacity to understand the psychological experiences of people throughout the world.
  • As two people who see the inherent value and importance of psychological approaches to the study of race and racism, the authors urge the field to take the latter path, for it is one that takes us all toward greater equity and justice.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 1
Psychological Science is Not Race Neutral: Comment on Roberts et al., (2020)
Cydney H. Dupree1 and Michael W. Kraus1,2
Yale University, School of Management1 and Department of Psychology2

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 2
Abstract
In their analysis, Roberts and colleagues (2020) argue that the editors, authors, and participants
throughout subfields of psychological science are overwhelmingly White. In this comment, we
consider some of the drivers and consequences of this racial inequality. Drawing on race
scholarship from in and outside the field, we highlight three phenomena that create and maintain
racial inequality in psychology: (1) racial ignorance, (2) threats to belonging, and (3) racial
progress narratives. We close by exploring steps that journals and authors can take to reduce
racial inequality in our field, ending with an appeal to consider the experience of scholars of
color in race scholarship and in psychological science more broadly.
Keywords: inequality, race, psychology, meta-science

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 3
Psychological Science is Not Race Neutral: Comment on Roberts et al., (2020)
If psychological science is to tackle diverse questions from diverse perspectives, it must
diversify. (Roberts, Bareket-Shavit, Dollins, Goldie, & Mortenson, 2020, p. 9)
In Roberts et al., (2020), the authors marshal an impressive collection of evidence that
describes the overwhelming Whiteness of psychological science. The work reveals that race
scholarship is exceedingly rare in our field, and those who participate in itincluding authors,
editors, and participantsare all overwhelmingly categorized as White. In this comment, we
draw on the available evidence from race scholarship to help us understand the drivers and costs
of the racial inequality that permeates the field of psychology.
Racial inequality is a defining challenge of both society and our field. We acknowledge,
however, that our own sense of urgency will not be shared by many. Because psychological
science has connections to disciplines that vary from computer science to sociology, some may
view race scholarship as peripheral to our disciplinea sort of niche research area (or me-
search”), with little bearing on the complex cognitive and affective processes that guide human
psychology. Those who do see race as more central to the field may, nevertheless, view work
from White authors, editors, and participants as just as informative as that produced by scholars
and participants of coloror even more so due to its supposed objectivity (King, Avery, Hebl, &
Cortina, 2018; Torrez, Kraus, & Dupree, 2020; Zuberi & Bonilla-Silva, 2008).
Here, we turn to scholarship on race and racism to highlight established societal and
psychological phenomena that call into question these assumptions and reinforce that our field
does indeed face an urgent need to overcome the racial inequality that is deeply embedded within

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 4
it. Ultimately, research on racial ignorance, threats to belonging, and the assumption of racial
progress all suggest that racial inequality will continue to stand as a significant and sustained
barrier in the study of cognitive, developmental, and social psychology. We end by highlighting
promising recommendations made by Roberts et al., (2020) that call for our field to deeply and
explicitly consider author, editor, and participant race in its research practices and organizational
structure.
The Problem of Racial Ignorance
Researchers are, in fact, people first. This reality clearly reveals one fundamental
challenge facing White authors, editors, and participants in the study of race: Racial ignorance.
Racial ignorance, a concept long explored in sociological traditions, refers to a general lack of
knowledge about how race shapes the experiences of people in society (Mills, 2014; Mueller,
2020)and it serves as both driver and consequence of the extreme Whiteness in psychology. A
combination of structural and motivational factors conspire to heighten racial ignorance among
White people relative to people of color. In structural terms, the US context is set up so that
racial segregation is the rule (Massey & Denton, 1993). AmericansWhite Americans
especiallygrow, live, and work in racially homogenous neighborhoods, schools, and
workplaces, spending relatively little time in integrated contexts. When integration is proposed
(or even passed) as law, White people tend to resist these policies through collective action
(DuBois, [1899], 1973; Ewing, 2018; Shedd, 2015). The segregation that characterizes much of
the US context obscures the magnitude of racial inequality in society, allowing White people to
go about much of their everyday lives without confronting racial privilege (Shedd, 2015).
Segregation therefore constitutes a significant barrier to the understanding of race and racism.

Psychology is Not Race Neutral 5
Critically, for White people, this segregation means that a fuller understanding of racism
must be discovered in books rather than through lived experience. This challenge must not be
minimized. Personal experiences can be particularly informative for rejecting dominant
narratives of race and racism in the context of social science (e.g., Dubois, 1903; Hill Collins,
1991) and for learning how race shapes seemingly neutral or objective scientific practices,
including the methods used to collect data for our studies (Zuberi & Bonilla-Silva, 2008).
Beyond segregation, motivational factors heighten racial ignorance. One such factor, as
social psychological research reveals, is the tendency to deny racial privilege. Specifically,
White people benefit from obscuring the existence of the racial inequality that they benefit from
(Mueller, 2020). This creates a motivated tendency among White people to, across many
contexts, deny racial privileges that they incur as a function of their elevated status (Lowery,
Knowles, & Unzueta, 2007). For example, White parents tend to avoid mentioning race in
conversations with their children because they view such conversations to be negative or
unnecessary (Abaied & Skinner, 2020; Perry, Skinner, & Abaied, 2019).
We see a similar pattern with adults: White people tend to discount their racial privileges
and endorse more meritocratic conceptions of society except in very specific circumstances
where for example, their personal characteristics are affirmed (e.g., Knowles & Lowery, 2012;
Phillips & Lowery, 2018). This pattern of racial ignorance aligns with research demonstrating
that White people underestimate racial inequality’s magnitude in society (Kraus, Rucker, &
Richeson, 2017) and in the institutions that they are a part of (Boykin et al., 2020; Ray, 2019).
Those who are more likely to underestimate such inequality are also less likely to hire racial
minorities or support equity-enhancing policies (Dupree, Torrez, Obioha, & Fiske, in press).

Citations
More filters
Posted ContentDOI
TL;DR: The social and behavioral sciences have long suffered from a lack of diversity in the samples used to study a broad array of phenomena as discussed by the authors, which has been addressed in an attempt to move toward a more contextually-informed app.
Abstract: The social and behavioral sciences have long suffered from a lack of diversity in the samples used to study a broad array of phenomena. In an attempt to move toward a more contextually-informed app...

30 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This article found that younger adults were more likely to recall words from previous lists than older adults, indicating that older adults are more susceptible to retroactive interference and that a buildup of proactive interference arising from previously studied words reduced memory selectivity in older adults.
Abstract: We are often presented with more information than we can remember, and we must selectively focus on the most valuable information to maximize memory utility. Most tests of value-based memory involve encoding and then being tested on a list of recently studied information. Thus, people are focused on memory for the current list and are encouraged to forget information from earlier lists. However, prior learning can influence later memory, in both interfering and beneficial ways, and there may be age-related differences in how younger and older adults are influenced by the costs and benefits of prior learning and interference. In the present study, we presented younger and older adults with words paired with point values to remember for a later test but rather than asking participants to only recall words from the just-studied list, participants were asked to recall all studied words on each recall test. Results revealed that younger adults were more likely to recall words from previous lists than older adults, indicating that older adults were more susceptible to retroactive interference. Moreover, although selectivity is often preserved in older adults when study-test cycles are independent, a buildup of proactive interference arising from previously studied words reduced memory selectivity in older adults. Thus, when presented with more information than one can remember, younger adults are better at combating interference and recalling valuable information, while older adults may engage in selective forgetting of prior lists to enhance a "present-focused" form of memory, possibly as a result of impaired inhibitory control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).

13 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper applied the white racial identity development (WRID) model among white U.S. youth (8-14 years old) to address this research gap, finding age-related change over time, with some evidence of increasing resistance to racism.
Abstract: Research on racial identity among Youth of Color has expanded considerably in recent years, but a parallel examination of racial identity among white youth has not occurred, reiterating whiteness as normative. We applied Janet Helms's White Racial Identity Development (WRID) model among white U.S. youth (8-14 years old) to address this research gap. WRID centers racism and white supremacy, offering a framework to analyze white racial identity in the context of systemic inequity. Using longitudinal, qualitative analysis, we found age-related change over time, with some evidence of increasing resistance to racism. There was high participant variability, however, indicating that socio-cognitive abilities alone cannot predict anti-racist white identity development. We discuss implications for racial identity research and social justice-orientated developmental science.

11 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper , a six-point lens on precarity that facilitates a deeper understanding of knowledge production in hegemonic social psychology and academia at large is proposed. But the focus of this paper is on the early career researchers of colour who inhabit the margins of social psychology.
Abstract: In this paper, we invite psychologists to reflect on and recognize how knowledge is produced in the field of social psychology. Engaging with the work of decolonial, liberation and critical psychology scholars, we provide a six-point lens on precarity that facilitates a deeper understanding of knowledge production in hegemonic social psychology and academia at large. We conceptualize knowledge (re)production in psychology as five interdependent 'cogs' within the neoliberal machinery of academia, which cannot be viewed in isolation; (1) its epistemological foundations rooted in coloniality, (2) the methods and standards it uses to understand human thoughts, feelings and behaviours, (3) the documentation of its knowledge, (4) the dissemination of its knowledge and (5) the universalization of psychological theories. With this paper we also claim our space in academia as early career researchers of colour who inhabit the margins of hegemonic social psychology. We join scholars around the world in calling for a much-needed disciplinary shift that centres solutions to the many forms of violence that are inflicted upon marginalized members of the global majority. To conclude, we offer four political-personal intentions for the reorientation for the discipline of hegemonic social psychology with the aim to disrupt the politics of knowledge production and eradicate precarity.

10 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Existing evidence supports the hypothesis that the need to belong is a powerful, fundamental, and extremely pervasive motivation, and people form social attachments readily under most conditions and resist the dissolution of existing bonds.
Abstract: A hypothesized need to form and maintain strong, stable interpersonal relationships is evaluated in light of the empirical literature. The need is for frequent, nonaversive interactions within an ongoing relational bond. Consistent with the belongingness hypothesis, people form social attachments readily under most conditions and resist the dissolution of existing bonds. Belongingness appears to have multiple and strong effects on emotional patterns and on cognitive processes. Lack of attachments is linked to a variety of ill effects on health, adjustment, and well-being. Other evidence, such as that concerning satiation, substitution, and behavioral consequences, is likewise consistent with the hypothesized motivation. Several seeming counterexamples turned out not to disconfirm the hypothesis. Existing evidence supports the hypothesis that the need to belong is a powerful, fundamental, and extremely pervasive motivation.

17,492 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors introduce the construct of team psychological safety, a shared belief held by members of a team that the team is safe for interpersonal risk taking, and test it in a multimethod field study.
Abstract: This paper presents a model of team learning and tests it in a multimethod field study. It introduces the construct of team psychological safety—a shared belief held by members of a team that the team is safe for interpersonal risk taking—and models the effects of team psychological safety and team efficacy together on learning and performance in organizational work teams. Results of a study of 51 work teams in a manufacturing company, measuring antecedent, process, and outcome variables, show that team psychological safety is associated with learning behavior, but team efficacy is not, when controlling for team psychological safety. As predicted, learning behavior mediates between team psychological safety and team performance. The results support an integrative perspective in which both team structures, such as context support and team leader coaching, and shared beliefs shape team outcomes.

6,953 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors argue that racial segregation is crucial to explaining the emergence of the urban underclass during the 1970s and that a strong interaction between rising rates of poverty and high levels of residential segregation explains where, why and in which groups the underclass arose.
Abstract: This article argues that racial segregation is crucial to explaining the emergence of the urban underclass during the 1970s. A strong interaction between rising rates of poverty and high levels of residential segregation explains where, why and in which groups the underclass arose. This argument is developed with simulations that replicate the economic conditions observed among blacks and whites in metropolitan areas during the 1970s but assume different conditions of racial and class segregation. These data show how a simple increase in the rate of minority poverty leads to a dramatic rise in the concentration of poverty when it occurs within a racially segregated city. Increases in poverty concentration are, in turn, associated with other changes in the socioeconomic character of neighborhoods, transforming them into physically deteriorated areas of high crime, poor schools, and excessive mortality where welfare-dependent, female-headed families are the norm. Thus, policies to solve the socioeconomic pr...

5,621 citations

Book ChapterDOI
TL;DR: The authors analyzes the way coping processes restore self-regard rather than the way they address the provoking threat itself, focusing on the way people cope with the implications of threat to their self-reward.
Abstract: Publisher Summary Self-affirmation processes are being activated by information that threatens the perceived adequacy or integrity of the self and as running their course until this perception is restored through explanation, rationalization, and/or action. The purpose of these constant explanations (and rationalizations) is to maintain a phenomenal experience of the self-self-conceptions and images as adaptively and morally adequate—that is, as competent, good, coherent, unitary, stable, capable of free choice, capable of controlling important outcomes, and so on. The research reported in this chapter focuses on the way people cope with the implications of threat to their self-regard rather than on the way they cope with the threat itself. This chapter analyzes the way coping processes restore self-regard rather than the way they address the provoking threat itself.

3,295 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Two experiments tested how belonging uncertainty undermines the motivation and achievement of people whose group is negatively characterized in academic settings, and an intervention that mitigated doubts about social belonging in college raised the academic achievement of Black students but not of White students.
Abstract: Stigmatization can give rise to belonging uncertainty. In this state, people are sensitive to information diagnostic of the quality of their social connections. Two experiments tested how belonging uncertainty undermines the motivation and achievement of people whose group is negatively characterized in academic settings. In Experiment 1, students were led to believe that they might have few friends in an intellectual domain. Whereas White students were unaffected, Black students (stigmatized in academics) displayed a drop in their sense of belonging and potential. In Experiment 2, an intervention that mitigated doubts about social belonging in college raised the academic achievement (e.g., college grades) of Black students but not of White students. Implications for theories of achievement motivation and intervention are discussed.

1,534 citations