scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Posted ContentDOI

Reflexive gaze following in common marmoset monkeys

27 Jun 2019-bioRxiv (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory)-pp 682971
TL;DR: Common marmosets follow the head gaze of conspecifics in order to establish joint attention and this support the assumption of an evolutionary old domain specific faculty shared within the primate order and underline the potential value of marmoset in studies of normal and disturbed joint attention.
Abstract: The ability to extract the direction of the others gaze allows us to shift our attention to an object of interest to the other and to establish joint attention. By mapping ones own expectations, desires and intentions on the object of joint attention, humans develop a Theory of (the others) Mind (TOM), a functional sequence possibly disrupted in autism. Although old world monkeys probably do not possess a TOM, they follow the others gaze and they establish joint attention. Gaze following of both humans and old world monkeys fulfills Fodors criteria of a domain specific function and is orchestrated by very similar cortical architectures, strongly suggesting homology. Also new world monkeys, a primate suborder that split from the old world monkey line about 35 million years ago, have complex social structures. One member of this group, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), has received increasing interest as a potential model in studies of normal and disturbed human social cognition. Marmosets are known to follow human head-gaze. However, the question is if they use gaze following to establish joint attention with conspecifics. Here we show that this is indeed the case. In a free choice task, head-restrained marmosets prefer objects gazed at by a conspecific and, moreover, they exhibit considerably shorter choice reaction times for the same objects. These findings support the assumption of an evolutionary old domain specific faculty shared within the primate order and they underline the potential value of marmosets in studies of normal and disturbed joint attention.nnHIGHLIGHTSO_LICommon marmosets follow the head gaze of conspecifics in order to establish joint attention.nC_LIO_LIBrief exposures to head gaze are sufficient to reallocate an animals attention.nC_LIO_LIThe tendency to follow the others gaze competes with the attractional binding of the conspecifics facenC_LI

Summary (2 min read)

INTRODUCTION

  • The adoption of CEFR onto education system needs to be comprehensive and involves a number of major changes.
  • Countries such as China, Taiwan, Netherland, Norway and Malaysia have begun to align their tests against the six levels of CEFR descriptors.
  • The linking of CEFR to a language test could be conducted from the aspects of familiarization, standardizations, specifications and empirical evidence (Council of Europe 2009).

THE STATUS OF CEFR IN MALAYSIA

  • English is regarded as the language of choice by most organizations and industries in Malaysia.
  • Some employers have shared their frustrations in employing graduates who are competent in English language (Mohd Shukri, Rafikul & Noor Hazilah 2014; Sheridan 2014; Yuen 2015).
  • This study was also interested to explore and link the content of English language courses to CEFR (Ramiaida, Noor Sazaai, Norhana, Fariza, Zarina & Juliana, 2017).
  • There are studies that include students as data source.
  • Therefore, this study is conducted to address the following objectives: 1. To identify the suitability of the current -writing and reading items against the CEFR level set by the Ministry of Education 2.

RESPONDENT

  • The respondents of this study were 331 high school English teachers.
  • The number of respondents of that study which was more or less the same to the present study.
  • The teachers were chosen and found to be suitable respondents because of their 15 to 20 years of teaching experience and they are familiar with the syllabus and format of English national examination papers.
  • The respondents involved in this study were also chosen because they have attended CEFR familiarization workshops organized by Ministry of Education, Malaysia.
  • This is important because respondents who are familiar with CEFR global scale could provide more accurate results compared to those who are not familiar with CEFR global scale and its concepts.

INSTRUMENT

  • This study uses an assessment checklist to collect the data.
  • The assessment checklist was constructed based on the compilation of SPM English papers from 2012 – 2017 table of specifications.
  • Then, the types of questions which were frequently asked were chosen and included in the assessment checklist.
  • The content expert from UKM has a PhD in Language Testing with more than 30 years of experience as an academic and researcher.
  • All items in the assessment checklist are considered good based on the high value of Cronbach Alpha.

PROCEDURES

  • The English teachers who were involved in this study were recommended by the schools’.
  • The teachers were briefed about the objectives of the study and their roles as subject matter experts.
  • Then, the assessment checklist along with the SPM English writing and reading papers of 2017 were given to the teachers.
  • The CEFR writing and reading scales were also provided to the teachers as a reference.
  • The whole process took approximately 3 weeks to complete.

DATA ANALYSIS

  • The data of this study were analyzed using two computer software, namely: Winstep and NVivo version 11.
  • Winstep was used in the data analysis in order to determine the suitability of the assessment items from the value of infit mean square (MnSq).
  • Second, items with the infit mnsq values which are higher than 1.4 are also found to be not suitable since the items are considered difficult and confusing, hence they should be removed.
  • The difficulty level of items decreases as the logit values get smaller.
  • Teachers’ comments and feedback were typed and transferred into the form of word document, then the comments and feedback were sorted out and coded according to writing items and reading items.

FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION

  • Table 5 shows the details of the assessment items measurement report which consist of logit, standard error (S.E) and infit mean square (mnsq) values.
  • These assessment items are considered as easy items because of the negative logit values, nevertheless these items are slightly difficult and not as easy as open topic, argumentative and novel because the S.E value is 23.
  • Apart from suggesting suitable CEFR level for the assessment items, the teachers also left their comments and feedback on the items.
  • Suggestions by teachers to replace informal letter with email is logic and make sense considering email is widely used as medium of communication in present days.
  • The teachers provided several reasons in which they believed reading assessment items of information transfer and reading comprehension could be removed.

CONCLUSION

  • In conclusion, all the writing and reading assessment items are suitable with a combination of three easy items, six intermediate items and four difficult items.
  • Some of the writing and reading assessment items are found to be relevant against CEFR global scale as well as the new CEFR aligned English syllabus.
  • Therefore, the suitable and relevant items should be retained instead of to be removed.
  • They have also proved that their judgements as subject matter experts could be used in deciding the appropriate CEFR level for the assessment items.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

1
Scientific RepoRtS | (2019) 9:15292 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51783-9
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Reexive gaze following in
common marmoset monkeys
Silvia Spadacenta
*
, Peter W. Dicke & Peter Thier
*
The ability to extract the direction of the other’s gaze allows us to shift our attention to an object of
interest to the other and to establish joint attention. By mapping one’s own intentions on the object of
joint attention, humans develop a Theory of (the other’s) Mind (TOM), a functional sequence possibly
disrupted in autism. Gaze following of both humans and old world monkeys is orchestrated by very
similar cortical architectures, strongly suggesting homology. Also new world monkeys, a primate
suborder that split from the old world monkey line about 35 million years ago, have complex social
structures and one member of this group, the common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are known to
follow human head-gaze. However, the question is if they use gaze following to establish joint attention
with conspecics. Here we show that this is indeed the case. In a free choice task, head-restrained
marmosets prefer objects gazed at by a conspecic and, moreover, they exhibit considerably shorter
choice reaction times for the same objects. These ndings support the assumption of an evolutionarily
old domain specic faculty shared within the primate order and they underline the potential value of
marmosets in studies of normal and disturbed joint attention.
Gaze following is a an important social skill, which enables humans, great apes and old world primates to establish
joint attention on an object of interest to the other. Although old world monkeys probably do not possess a TOM,
namely they are not able to map ones own intentions, desires and expectations on the object of interest, they fol-
low the others gaze and they establish joint attention
1
. Like humans, gaze following of old world monkeys fullls
Fodors criteria of a domain specic function. Common marmosets are also well known for having a peculiar
interest in faces
2,3
. Unlike macaques, the species of old world primates studied best, and other non-human pri-
mate species, they oen engage in mutual gaze, for example in the context of joint action tasks
4
. Many individuals
even seek eye contact with their human caretakers (personal observations). Common marmosets also care about
the orientation of a human face as demonstrated by the fact that human head-gaze biases choices in an object
selection task
5
. While this latter behavior may indicate an inherent capacity for gaze following, it remains to be
shown that it can also be triggered by conspecics. By the same token the lack of high resolution behavioral data
has as yet precluded well-founded inferences about the relationship of marmoset gaze following to gaze following
exhibited by humans and rhesus monkeys, the two species of old world primates for which detailed behavioral
and neuronal data are available
1,6
. Gaze following of macaques and humans is reex-like in the sense that it is fast
and hard to suppress, two features that have contributed to the assumption of a domain specic faculty
712
based
on a dedicated neural system
13
. Do marmosets follow the gaze of conspecics in the same reex-like manner? An
armative response would support the notion that gaze following in extant primate lines is homologous, i.e. a
reection of shared ancestry.
In order to address these questions, we trained 3 common marmosets (2 females, 1 male) to execute a free
choice task in a well-controlled experimental setup that allowed us to head-restrain the animals to precisely track
eye movements. A conspecic’s face, oriented either to the le or to the right, was presented on a monitor for a
variable time ranging between 100 and 600 ms in steps of 100 ms (Fig.1a) and the observing animal was allowed
to scrutinize the face with eye movements conned by the boundaries of the portrait. e facial portrait was fol-
lowed by the appearance of two targets placed at 5° and + 5° from the center on the horizontal axis. e animals
had to freely choose one of the two targets, a human face (2° × 3°extension), by making an indicative saccade into
a window of 2° centered on the target within 500 ms. Independent of the orientation of the conspecics face, both
possible choices were rewarded, provided that the eyes had met the xation requirements.
Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Department of Cognitive Neurology, Otfried-Müller-Str. 27, 72076,
Tübingen, Germany. *email: silvia.spadacenta@uni-tuebingen.de; thier@uni-tuebingen.de
open

2
Scientific RepoRtS | (2019) 9:15292 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51783-9
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Results and Discussion
Common marmosets follow the gaze of a conspecic in a quasi-reexive manner. Figure1b, le
panel, plots the percentage of target choices in the direction of the face orientation (“congruent choices”) as func-
tion of the duration of the availability of the portrait. e graph depicts data pooled over all three animals and the
two possible face orientations: congruent choices exceeded chance level signicantly (binomial probability test,
p < 0.05), indicating that the observing monkey tended to follow the gaze of the portrayed monkey. is prefer-
ence was already apparent aer a presentation duration of the portrait of only 100 ms and got stronger for longer
presentation durations peaking at 300 ms exposure time (see also Supplementary Fig.S1). is dependence on
exposure duration is similar to the one exhibited by human observers when exposed to symbolic central cues
such as pointing arrows. ey typically demonstrate a gradual buildup of their spatial target preferences cued by
central stimuli, reaching an optimum at 300 ms
14,15
. As shown in the right panel of Fig.1b, the dependence of the
choice bias on presentation duration was the same in all three animals for up to 300 ms. Only later, the individual
plots start to diverge: interestingly, two of our animals (M2 and M3) showed a clear second peak, overall convey-
ing the impression of an oscillatory pattern with a period of about 250–300 ms. Periodic uctuations of attention
between two locations with a period of 4 HZ have also been described for human and macaque spatial vision
16,17
.
Figure 1. Oriented faces bias the animal’s choices to targets congruent with gaze direction. (a) Behavioral
paradigm. e trial started with the presentation of a central xation dot. Once xation was established, the
oriented face of a conspecic (replaced by other stimuli in control experiments) appeared for a variable time
(100–600 ms in steps of 100 ms). e disappearance of the conspecic’s face or the control stimuli and the
simultaneous appearance of two peripheral targets was the go signal for the animals to freely choose one of two
peripheral targets presented on the horizontal axis at 5° right and le of the center respectively, by means of a
saccade. e animal received a reward if the xation requirements were met. e xation window for the central
dot had a size of 2° × 2°, a size of 2° × 3° for the peripheral targets portraits of a human and for the central
portraits/control stimuli it corresponded to the extent the centrally presented image (7° × 6°). e subject
depicted in gure has agreed for her portrait to be published. (b) Le panel: plot of the percentage of target
choices congruent with portrait orientation as function of the duration of presentation. Pooled data (monkeys
M1, M2, M3). Binomial probability: ***p < 0.001. Right panel: plot of the percentage of target choices
congruent with portrait orientation for the individual animals M1, M2 and M3. Binomial probability: *p < 0.05,
***p < 0.001. (c) Monkey specic plots of the percentage of target choices congruent with portrait orientation,
separating portraits oriented to the le and right respectively. In each panel (le, M1; center, M2; right, M3) the
solid line stands for le oriented portraits and the dashed one for right oriented portraits.

3
Scientific RepoRtS | (2019) 9:15292 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51783-9
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Yet, given the fact that the third animal exhibited a dierent pattern, characterized by an absence of a second gaze
following peak and a constant choice at chance level aer 300 ms, further studies will be needed to critically assess
the possibility of periodicity.
All three animals exhibited individually dierent directional biases for the le and the right respectively, mod-
ifying their choice behavior on top of the inuence of directional information provided by facial orientation.
Directional biases became apparent when plotting the dependence of choice preference on head gaze direction
for the three individual animals independently for head gaze to the le and to the right (Fig.1c). For example,
a bias to the le side boosted the correct responses for the le oriented head gaze portraits (M1 and M2, le
and central panel respectively), and for the right oriented when the bias fell on the other side (M3, right panel).
Nonetheless, the bias never altered the overall response curve shape with a peak for congruent choices at around
300 ms. A signicant dominance of congruent choices peaking at 300 ms could be seen in M1 and M2 even for
congruent choices prompted by portraits oriented towards the animals non preferred side (binomial probability,
p < 0.001). A comparable tendency in M3 did not reach signicance (binomial probability, p = 0.1). e basis of
the directional bias remains unclear. e fact that it diers between individuals indicates that hidden imbalances
in the setup that might bind attention can hardly matter.
When the animals were confronted with direct gaze of a conspecic with the face turned straight or alterna-
tively, with black or grey disks of a similar size, likewise lacking directional information, target choices of all three
animals did not dier signicantly from chance level at most exposure times with the exception of the shortest
one (Fig.2a, pooled data). In particular the choice peaks for 300 and 600 ms could no longer been seen. For
100 ms exposure, overall pooled choices to the le were signicantly more frequent than to the right. Two of the
Figure 2. Direct gaze and disk or triangular stimuli attract the animals’ attention towards the center. (a) Plot
of le target choice as function of the duration of a central stimulus, either a conspecics face looking straight
at the observer (direct gaze) or a circular grey or black disk of similar size. Data pooled over the three animals
(monkeys M1, M2, M3). A choice bias is evident at the shortest duration time, whereas for longer exposures
the animals chose the targets on the right and le at random. Binomial probability: ***p < 0.001. (b) Bar plot
of percentage of choices congruent with the darker half of a bipartite disk. Data pooled over the three animals
(monkeys M1, M2, M3). For 100 ms presentation duration, the animals exhibited a signicant preference for the
target on the brighter side, i.e. opposite to the side preference to be expected based on a mechanism exploiting
the luminance asymmetry associated with face orientation. At 300 ms the choices did not indicate a preference.
Binomial probability: **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. (c) Bar plot of percentage of choices congruent with the side
cued by the oriented triangles. Data pooled over the three animals (monkeys M1, M2, M3). No signicant bias
was detected.

4
Scientific RepoRtS | (2019) 9:15292 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51783-9
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
individual monkeys (M1, M2) exhibited this preference for the le, whereas the third one (monkey M3) a prefer-
ence for the right. e individual directional preferences for the le and the right corresponded to the direction
of the biases seen in the responses to oriented gaze (Fig.1c), yet, now conned to the shortest exposure only. We
think that the disappearance of the directional bias for longer exposure times might be a consequence of increas-
ing attraction towards the central object, overriding the bias, no matter if the central object is the neutral disk or
the portrait of a conspecic looking straight. is interpretation has interesting implications for the experiments
with oriented faces, which showed a persistence of the directional biases independent of exposure time. Here the
directional gaze seems to suppress the buildup of attraction to the central object, facilitating the readiness to look
elsewhere as determined by the resultant of the others gaze direction and an internal directional bias.
e white ear tus on the le and right of the darker central face of a straight ahead marmoset oer a symmet-
ric luminance prole. Once the animal turns the head to the side, symmetry is lost as the visible area of the ear
tu on the side of the head turn will decrease, whereas the area of the other one will increase (see Supplementary
Fig.S3a,b). Hence, gauging the extent of the luminance asymmetry may be a simple way to determine the others
head gaze direction without the need to process other aspects of the face. To test whether le-right dierences in
the luminance of an object prompt an orienting response of the observer, we exposed all 3 animals to bipartite
disks replacing the marmoset portraits. e disks were black on the le and light grey on the right or vice versa.
ese two versions of the bipartite disks were presented randomly interleaved for 100 ms or 300 ms, two portrait
exposure times that had prompted clear gaze following in the main experiment. Against the backdrop of the
preceding considerations, we had hypothesized that the animals might prefer the target on the side of the darker
half of the bipartite disk for both exposure times. However, contrary to our expectation, the animal preferred the
target on the side of the brighter half of the disk, independently if positioned on the right or le side and, more-
over, only for 100 ms exposure time. For 300 ms choices did not exhibit any preference (Fig.2b). is result does
not support the hypothesis that marmoset gaze following is determined by a simple mechanism, conned to the
comparison of the two ear tu areas.
Turning the head to the side will not only change the balance between paler and darker parts of the face but
also disrupt the symmetry of the facial contour. A face that appears symmetric when seen in a straight ahead
orientation will become asymmetric when turned to the side, an asymmetry that oers directional information.
If marmoset gaze following relied on an ability to exploit contour asymmetries, one might expect that even asym-
metric non-face shapes might redirect attention. In order to test this possibility we exposed our experimental
animals to the vision of lled out black triangles, covering an area similar to the one covered by the portraits. e
triangles were presented with one of their three corners pointing either to the le or to the right. e orientation
was chosen randomly interleaved and the triangles were presented for 100 ms or 300 ms in separate blocks of
trials, i.e. for the presentation durations that had provided the strongest eects of oriented marmoset faces. e
results clearly showed that both for the le and the right oriented triangle the target choice remained at chance
level independent of the presentation duration (see Fig.2c). In sum, the luminance and shape control stimuli,
mimicking isolated low level features of oriented faces, turned out to be unable to redirect spatial attention. In
other words, marmoset gaze following seems to require a more holistic perceptual interpretation of faces, based
on the integrative extraction of more than an isolated parameter.
Congruent choices are accompanied by faster reaction times already at short exposure
times. In the main experiment, the latencies of saccades indicating congruent choices were shorter than the
ones for incongruent choices for exposure times up to 400 ms duration (Fig.3). Actually, this facilitation eect
was strongest for the shortest exposure time, gradually decreased with exposure time and no longer reached sig-
nicance for the longest durations tested (500 and 600 ms; see Fig.3 legend for statistics), consistent with a time
course of reexive rather than volitional orienting. A similar facilitation eect for comparably short exposure
durations has been seen in studies of macaque monkeys
11
and humans
8
. However, these studies did not report a
gradual increase of reaction times with the time of exposure seen in our experiments on marmosets. is dier-
ence might be a consequence of the specic paradigm we used. In our experiments, the animals had to choose
between two targets of equal appearance, rather than to follow the other’s gaze to a specic target as in the work
on macaque monkeys and humans. Hence, our animals may have tended to extract additional information from
the other’s face beyond gaze direction in an attempt to facilitate their choices, provided that this portrait was avail-
able long enough. is increased interest in the other’s face, gated by longer exposure times, can be expected to
compromise the ability to quickly disengage attention at the time of the go-signal. is interpretation is supported
by the experiments with control stimuli and the eye movements prompted by the appearance of the portraits we
discuss below.
Saccades associated with the straight ahead face (“direct gaze”) exhibited latencies that were not dierent from
the ones associated with incongruent choices to oriented faces. Interestingly, latencies of saccades associated with
neutral disks showed an inuence of exposure time that was qualitatively opposite to the inuence on saccades for
congruent choices: while being similar to saccades for straight faces for short presentation durations, they became
shorter with increasing exposure time (see Fig.3). e same held for the bicolor disk control stimuli and for the
oriented triangles (Supplementary Fig.S2). ese results indicate that for marmosets, the attraction of the other’s
face but not the attraction of non-biological objects increases with time exposure and correspondingly attentional
disengagement is delayed. Neutral objects were associated with relatively long saccadic reaction times when pre-
sented briey, probably because of the need to scrutinize the object in order to assess its signicance. Once its
irrelevance is established aer some 200 ms of presentation, the observer disengages her/his attention in order to
prepare a fast saccadic choice. A short exposure to the oriented face can cause a profound shortening of saccadic
reaction time, because the drive to follow gaze direction is already fully expressed whereas facial attraction is still
building up. e idea that the development of facial attraction and in general the perception of faces may need a
much longer time is also supported by a consideration of the pattern of saccadic exploration of the portraits (see

5
Scientific RepoRtS | (2019) 9:15292 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51783-9
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Supplementary Fig.S3a,b for details), whose complexity keeps growing with exposure time. Hence, the question
is why the drive to follow gaze is fully expressed in saccadic reaction times for short exposure times, arguably
too short to allow a detailed scrutiny of the face whereas the choice bias increases further with exposure time for
up to 300 ms. We think that this dissociation between reaction times and choice probabilities might reect the
concerted action of two systems controlling gaze following. e rst is fast, probably subcortical, controlling gaze
following based on a rough and potentially error prone analysis of the other’s face, too limited to provide informa-
tion on other aspects of the face like the identity or mood of the agent. With longer exposure and concomitantly
processing time, this information becomes available, on the one hand binding attention but, on the other hand,
also improving the directional precision of decisions.
Conclusions
Gaze following is prevalent among numerous species but its strength and exibility varies substantially between
them
18
. As shown here, gaze following is also well developed in common marmosets, a new world monkey spe-
cies. Marmoset gaze following is characterized by strong similarities with the gaze following behavior of the two
old world primate species studied extensively, macaques and humans. e strongest argument for correspond-
ence is the similar dependence on the time of exposure to the others gaze direction. In all three species the other’s
gaze biases decisions on potential targets already aer only 100 ms of exposure to the other’s gaze, too short to
accommodate a more detailed scrutiny of the others face. However, given more time to explore the other’s face,
the bias gets stronger in all three, in line with the assumption that primate gaze following is a faculty, consisting of
an early reex-line component that is complemented by a later, more exible component, arguably also respon-
sible for the more sophisticated emotional and cognitive control known to modulate gaze following
1921
. e
behavioral similarities between the gaze following behavior of marmosets, macaque monkeys and humans are
in principle in line with the assumption of a homologous faculty, already available before the split of the new and
old world monkey primate lines. is conclusion may strengthen the view that the marmoset may indeed become
a useful model system for research into the underpinnings of disturbed human social interactions like autism,
related to decient gaze following and joint attention
22
. However, although compelling, the behavioral similarities
established in our study may as well reect behavioral convergence. Hence, comparative physiological and genetic
studies of the underlying neural systems will be needed to strengthen the case for homology.
Materials and Methods
Experimental model and subject details. Common marmosets. We trained 3 adult common marmoset
monkeys (Callithrix jacchus; two females and one male, aged 7 years) to voluntarily enter a custom made monkey
chair by means of positive reinforcement training and to accept the restriction of head movements through a head
holder. Animals were all born in captivity and kept in a marmoset husbandry at approximately 26 °C, 40%–60%
relative humidity and a 12 h:12 h light-dark cycle. Access to water was always ad libitum, while food intake was
controlled according to body weight (weight loss never less than 10% of the ad libitum weight) and amount of
reward received in the experiment. Food consisted in fresh fruits and vegetables and standard commercial chow.
Figure 3. Oriented faces speed up reaction times for congruent choices. Plots of saccadic reaction times as
function of the duration of presentation of the central stimuli. Data pooled over the three animals (monkeys
M1, M2, M3) and choice direction. Saccadic reaction times indicating congruent choices were signicantly
shorter compared to the incongruent ones up to 400 ms of presentation duration (Wilcoxon rank-sum test,
100 ms: zval = 4.8221, p < 0.001; 200 ms: zval = 3.8449, p < 0.001; 300 ms: zval = 2.0341, p = 0.04; 400 ms:
zval = 2.4745, p = 0.01). e individual plots are tted with 2nd degree polynomial functions in an attempt to
improve the visibility. e two ts that showed a signicant dependence of saccade latency on the presentation
duration of the central stimulus were the one for congruent choices prompted by oriented faces and the one for
neutral disk stimuli. e former exhibited a gradual increase with duration from a substantially shorted reaction
time for a duration of 100 ms (adjusted r
2
= 0.86). e latter, on the other hand, showed a gradual decrease with
duration (adjusted r
2
= 0.89).

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper studied gaze understanding in children who recovered from early-onset near-complete blindness through late cataract surgery and found that they acquired sufficient visual acuity for detailed pattern recognition, but they failed to develop automatic gaze following.
Abstract: Significance Early in life, humans spontaneously learn to extract complex visual information without external guidance. Current vision models fail to replicate such learning, relying instead on extensive supervision. A classic example is gaze understanding, an early-learned skill useful for joint attention and social interaction. We studied gaze understanding in children who recovered from early-onset near-complete blindness through late cataract surgery. Following treatment, they acquired sufficient visual acuity for detailed pattern recognition, but they failed to develop automatic gaze following. Our computational modeling suggests that their learning is severely limited due to reduced availability of internal self-supervision mechanisms, which guide learning in normal development. The results have implications to understanding natural visual learning, potential rehabilitation, and obtaining unsupervised learning in vision models.

4 citations

Dissertation
01 Aug 2018
TL;DR: The authors argued that infants can already reason about beliefs and showed that the capacity to reason about what people think emerges early on in the development of a person's brain, and argued that the fact that young children fail direct false-belief tests can be explained in either of two ways.
Abstract: At what point in development does the capacity to reason about what people think emerge? While developmental psychologists have been investigating this question for more than thirty years, the evidence they have gained so far is conflicting. On the one hand, the results of traditional, direct false-beliefs tests, which involve asking participants how a person with a false belief will act, suggest that most children under four years of age are still unaware that beliefs can be false. On the other hand, false-belief tests using indirect measures, such as, for example, looking times or anticipatory looking, suggest that even infants ascribe false beliefs to other people. As many have noted, these results pose a deep developmental puzzle. In this work, I defend the claim that infants can already reason about beliefs. On the one hand, I argue that alternative interpretations of indirect false-belief tests fall short of the mark. On the other, I argue that the fact that young children fail direct false-belief tests can be explained in either of two ways, both of which are compatible with the claim that the capacity to reason about beliefs emerges early on. The first option is to maintain that young children fail because of performance difficulties. This type of position has been defended by other authors, but I argue that the particular proposal I put forward (which I call the processing-time account) offers a better account of the evidence. In contrast, the second option (which I call they hybrid approach) is one that, to the best of my knowledge, no one else has defended so far. This consists in arguing that direct and indirect false-belief tests recruit distinct cognitive systems, each of which can independently sustain the ability to reason about beliefs, but which follow different developmental trajectories. After exploring these two options, I consider which is best supported by the evidence.

3 citations


Cites background from "Reflexive gaze following in common ..."

  • ...• Rachel Razza and Clancy Blair (2009) conducted a longitudinal study of the relationship between mentalising and social competence....

    [...]

  • ...possible, I will be focusing here on their most recent proposal, advanced by Peipei Setoh, Rose Scott and Renée Baillargeon (2016) and known as the Expanded Processing-Demands (EPD for short) account....

    [...]

  • ...The evidence that Westra (2016b) and Carruthers (2016) discuss falls short of establishing their claim – i....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper , the authors developed a protocol to acclimate the marmoset to sit calmly in a primate chair, and a method to calibrate the eye-tracking system while marmosets were freely viewing the screen.
Abstract: The marmoset is a small‐bodied primate with behavioral capacities and brain structures comparable to macaque monkeys and humans. Its amenability to modern biotechnological techniques like optogenetics, chemogenetics, and generation of transgenic primates have attracted neuroscientists' attention to use it as a model in neuroscience. In the past decade, several laboratories have been developing and refining tools and techniques for performing behavioral and electrophysiological experiments in this new model. In this regard, we developed a protocol to acclimate the marmoset to sit calmly in a primate chair; a method to calibrate the eye‐tracking system while marmosets were freely viewing the screen; and a procedure to map motor field of neurons in the SC in freely viewing marmosets. Using a squeeze‐walled transfer box, the animals were acclimatized, and chair trained in less than 4 weeks, much shorter than what other studies reported. Using salient stimuli allowed quick and accurate calibration of the eye‐tracking system in untrained freely viewing marmosets. Applying reverse correlation to spiking activity and saccadic eye movements, we were able to map motor field of SC neurons in freely viewing marmosets. These refinements shortened the acclimation period, most likely reduced stress to the subjects, and allowed more efficient eye calibration and motor field mapping in freely viewing marmosets. With a penetration angle of 38 degrees, all 16 channels of the electrode array, that is, all recorded neurons across SC layers, had overlapping visual receptive and motor fields, indicating perpendicular penetration to the SC.
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article , focal electrical and pharmacological perturbations were applied to the GFP to disrupt gaze-following if the monkeys had been instructed to follow gaze, along with the ability to suppress it if vetoed by the context.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is argued that these findings are consistent with the operation of a reflexive, stimulus-driven or exogenous orienting of an observer's visual attention.
Abstract: Four experiments investigate the hypothesis that cues to the direction of another's social attention produce a reflexive orienting of an observer's visual attention. Participants were asked to make a simple detection response to a target letter which could appear at one of four locations on a visual display. Before the presentation of the target, one of these possible locations was cued by the orientation of a digitized head stimulus, which appeared at fixation in the centre of the display. Uninformative and to-be-ignored cueing stimuli produced faster target detection latencies at cued relative to uncued locations, but only when the cues appeared 100 msec before the onset of the target (Experiments 1 and 2). The effect was uninfluenced by the introduction of a to-be-attended and relatively informative cue (Experiment 3), but was disrupted by the inversion of the head cues (Experiment 4). It is argued that these findings are consistent with the operation of a reflexive, stimulus-driven or exogenous orient...

538 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Electrophysiological evidence that perception at attended locations is actually modulated periodically is presented, suggesting that “sustained” attention in fact exerts its facilitative effect on perception in a periodic fashion.
Abstract: An important effect of sustained attention is the facilitation of perception Although the term “sustained” suggests that this beneficial effect endures continuously as long as something is attended, we present electrophysiological evidence that perception at attended locations is actually modulated periodically Subjects detected brief light flashes that were presented peripherally at locations that were either attended or unattended We analyzed the correlation between detection performance for attended and unattended stimuli and the phase of ongoing EEG oscillations, which relate to subsecond fluctuations of neuronal excitability Although on average, detection performance was improved by attention—indicated by reduced detection thresholds at attended locations—we found that detection performance for attended stimuli actually fluctuated over time along with the phase of spontaneous oscillations in the θ (≈7 Hz) frequency band just before stimulus onset This fluctuation was absent for unattended stimuli This pattern of results suggests that “sustained” attention in fact exerts its facilitative effect on perception in a periodic fashion

511 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The present evidence for time differences between the two cuing conditions suggests that more than one process is involved in the spatial precuing of attention.
Abstract: There are suggestions in the literature that spatial precuing of attention with peripheral and central cues may be mediated by different mechanisms. To investigate this issue, data from two previous papers were reanalysed to investigate the complete time course of precuing target location with either: (1) a peripheral cue that may draw attention reflexively, or (2) a central, symbolic cue that may require attention to be directed voluntarily. This analysis led to predictions that were tested in another experiment. The main result of this experiment was that a peripheral cue produced its largest effects on discrimination performance within 100 msec, whereas a central cue required approximately 300 msec to achieve maximum effects. In conjunction with previous findings, the present evidence for time differences between the two cuing conditions suggests that more than one process is involved in the spatial precuing of attention.

376 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Human and nonhuman animals appear to share basic gaze-following behaviors, suggesting the foundations of human social cognition may also be present in nonhuman brains.
Abstract: In general, individuals look where they attend and next intend to act. Many animals, including our own species, use observed gaze as a deictic ("pointing") cue to guide behavior. Among humans, these responses are reflexive and pervasive: they arise within a fraction of a second, act independently of task relevance, and appear to undergird our initial development of language and theory of mind. Human and nonhuman animals appear to share basic gaze-following behaviors, suggesting the foundations of human social cognition may also be present in nonhuman brains.

263 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Analysis of eye movements revealed that both subjects inspected the target (object or position attended by the stimulus monkey) more often than the distractor (nonattended object or position).
Abstract: Gaze and attention direction provide important sources of social information for primates. Behavioral studies show that chimpanzees spontaneously follow human gaze direction. By contrast, non-ape species such as macaques fail to follow gaze cues. The authors investigated the reactions of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) to attention cues of conspecifics. Two subjects were presented with videotaped images of a stimulus monkey with its attention directed to 1 of 2 identical objects. Analysis of eye movements revealed that both subjects inspected the target (object or position attended by the stimulus monkey) more often than the distractor (nonattended object or position). These results provide evidence that rhesus monkeys follow gaze and use the attention cues of other monkeys to orient their own attention to objects.

251 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (20)
Q1. What are the contributions in "Reflexive gaze following in common marmoset monkeys" ?

Also new world monkeys, a primate suborder that split from the old world monkey line about 35 million years ago, have complex social structures and one member of this group, the common marmosets ( Callithrix jacchus ) are known to follow human head-gaze. However, the question is if they use gaze following to establish joint attention with conspecifics. Here the authors show that this is indeed the case. These findings support the assumption of an evolutionarily old domain specific faculty shared within the primate order and they underline the potential value of marmosets in studies of normal and disturbed joint attention. 

Yet, given the fact that the third animal exhibited a different pattern, characterized by an absence of a second gaze following peak and a constant choice at chance level after 300 ms, further studies will be needed to critically assess the possibility of periodicity. Hence, the question is why the drive to follow gaze is fully expressed in saccadic reaction times for short exposure times, arguably too short to allow a detailed scrutiny of the face whereas the choice bias increases further with exposure time for up to 300 ms. The first is fast, probably subcortical, controlling gaze following based on a rough and potentially error prone analysis of the other ’ s face, too limited to provide information on other aspects of the face like the identity or mood of the agent. 

Turning the head to the side will not only change the balance between paler and darker parts of the face but also disrupt the symmetry of the facial contour. 

Neutral objects were associated with relatively long saccadic reaction times when presented briefly, probably because of the need to scrutinize the object in order to assess its significance. 

A short exposure to the oriented face can cause a profound shortening of saccadic reaction time, because the drive to follow gaze direction is already fully expressed whereas facial attraction is still building up. 

For 100 ms presentation duration, the animals exhibited a significant preference for the target on the brighter side, i.e. opposite to the side preference to be expected based on a mechanism exploiting the luminance asymmetry associated with face orientation. 

A choice bias is evident at the shortest duration time, whereas for longer exposures the animals chose the targets on the right and left at random. 

A face that appears symmetric when seen in a straight ahead orientation will become asymmetric when turned to the side, an asymmetry that offers directional information. 

The disappearance of the conspecific’s face or the control stimuli and the simultaneous appearance of two peripheral targets was the go signal for the animals to freely choose one of two peripheral targets presented on the horizontal axis at 5° right and left of the center respectively, by means of a saccade. 

The orientation was chosen randomly interleaved and the triangles were presented for 100 ms or 300 ms in separate blocks of trials, i.e. for the presentation durations that had provided the strongest effects of oriented marmoset faces. 

In all three species the other’s gaze biases decisions on potential targets already after only 100 ms of exposure to the other’s gaze, too short to accommodate a more detailed scrutiny of the other’s face. 

Gaze following is a an important social skill, which enables humans, great apes and old world primates to establish joint attention on an object of interest to the other. 

Here the directional gaze seems to suppress the buildup of attraction to the central object, facilitating the readiness to look elsewhere as determined by the resultant of the other’s gaze direction and an internal directional bias. 

If marmoset gaze following relied on an ability to exploit contour asymmetries, one might expect that even asymmetric non-face shapes might redirect attention. 

The animals had to freely choose one of the two targets, a human face (2° × 3°extension), by making an indicative saccade into a window of 2° centered on the target within 500 ms. 

In order to test this possibility the authors exposed their experimental animals to the vision of filled out black triangles, covering an area similar to the one covered by the portraits. 

The white ear tufts on the left and right of the darker central face of a straight ahead marmoset offer a symmetric luminance profile. 

latencies of saccades associated with neutral disks showed an influence of exposure time that was qualitatively opposite to the influence on saccades for congruent choices: while being similar to saccades for straight faces for short presentation durations, they became shorter with increasing exposure time (see Fig. 3). 

This increased interest in the other’s face, gated by longer exposure times, can be expected to compromise the ability to quickly disengage attention at the time of the go-signal. 

Binomial probability: **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. (c) Bar plot of percentage of choices congruent with the side cued by the oriented triangles.