scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Social Media as Information Source: Recency of Updates and Credibility of Information

TL;DR: Data indicate that recency of tweets impacts source credibility; however, this relationship is mediated by cognitive elaboration, which suggests many implications for theory and application, both in computer-mediated communication and crisis communication.
Abstract: Social media are increasingly being used as an information source, including information related to risks and crises. The current study examines how pieces of information available in social media impact perceptions of source credibility. Specifically, participants in the study were asked to view 1 of 3 mockTwitter.compages that varied the recency with which tweets were posted and then to report on their perceived source credibility of the page owner. Data indicate that recency of tweets impacts source credibility; however, this relationship is mediated by cognitive elaboration. These data suggest many implications for theory and application, both in computer-mediated communication and crisis communication. These implications are discussed, along with limitations of the current study and directions for future research.

Summary (3 min read)

Social Media and Credibility Judgments

  • The types of information suggested in the first paragraph of this paper share at least one thing in common: they all deal with uncertain situations highlighted by potential danger.
  • During the January 2010 Haitian earthquake, social media played a key role in disseminating information about this tragedy (Bunz, 2010).
  • This is because with the increasing amount of information available through newer channels, the gatekeeping function seems to shift away from producers of content and onto consumers of that content (Haas & Wearden, 2003).
  • The continued growth of new media has meant that information consumers are now far less beholden to what passes through traditional gatekeepers and are able to bypass gatekeepers altogether and turn directly to primary information sources, many of which are information consumers themselves.
  • Because information provided in newer channels often lacks professional gatekeepers to check content, and thus, lacks some of the traditional markers used to determine source credibility, consumers become more responsible for making decisions about the credibility of information online.

The MAIN Model and Recency of Updates

  • The MAIN model (Sundar, 2008) describes technological affordances that allow people to heuristically process cues when making judgments about the credibility of an online source.
  • Agency cues capitalize on heuristics that emphasize credibility cues that, for example, are computer- (rather than user-) generated.
  • This leads to the first hypothesis of the current study: H1: Recency of updating on a social media site will be positively associated with source credibility of the site’s source.
  • A central tenet of involvement is the sense 174 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171–183 © 2013 International Communication Association that the individual partakes in an active psychological processing of that content.
  • Based on this possibility, the following hypothesis is posited: H3: There will be a positive association between recency and cognitive elaboration.

Overview

  • In order to test the hypotheses offered in the current study, a 3 condition experiment was designed.
  • A mock Twitter page for the American Heart Association was created to represent a page devoted to the dissemination of information regarding heart disease.
  • Participants viewed the Twitter page, and then responded to measures of cognitive elaboration (Perse, 1990) and source credibility (McCroskey & Teven, 1999).

Materials

  • Participants were asked to view one of three mock Twitter pages .
  • The pages were designed to appear as if the user was attempting to disseminate information and recent updates about heart disease.
  • The page was made to appear as one from the American Heart Association for two reasons: Second, the assumption was made that an ‘‘official’’ page associated with the topic 176 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171–183 © 2013 International Communication Association would be more realistic in terms of fast updates.
  • The three pages represented three different levels of update recency: fast (most recent post approximately 1 minute ago, n = 63), medium (most recent post approximately 1 hour ago, n = 56), and slow (most recent post approximately 1 day ago, n = 62).

Instrumentation

  • After viewing the mock Twitter page, participants were asked to respond to two measures: one for cognitive elaboration (Perse, 1990) and one for source credibility (McCroskey & Teven, 1999).
  • Cognitive elaboration was measured using a version of Perse’s (1990) five-item measure, modified to reflect the previously viewed Twitter page.
  • Using a five-point response scale (5 = strongly agree, 1 = strongly disagree), people reported their level of agreement with each item (i.e., When I looked at this page, I thought about it over and over again).
  • All five items formed a unidimensional solution with acceptable reliability (α = .68), so all five items were averaged to create an elaboration index.
  • McCroskey and Teven’s (1999) source credibility measure contains three separate constructs: competence, goodwill, and trustworthiness.

Procedure

  • Participants were informed about the research opportunity in class.
  • Participants went to the website, and read the informed consent.
  • After clicking on a button called ‘‘Begin Study,’’ they were directed to a program that randomly assigned participants to view one of the three mock Twitter pages.
  • After each participant had viewed the page, they were instructed to click on another link that sent them to the questionnaire.
  • This ensured participant’s names were kept separate from their responses.

Results

  • Hypothesis 1 predicted that recency of updating on a social media page would be positively associated with source credibility for the page’s owner.
  • The results of the planned contrast analysis for competence suggested that the linear pattern predicted between recency and competence was not evident in the data, t (171) = 0.188, p = .426 (one-tailed).
  • To test this hypothesis, the bivariate correlations between cognitive elaboration and each of the three credibility measures were analyzed.
  • Thus the data were consistent with hypothesis 2.
  • Hypothesis 3 predicted that a positive relationship would exist between recency of updates and cognitive elaboration.

Post hoc analyses

  • The results of the hypothesis tests suggested that a possible mediation effect between recency of updates and credibility measures was present in the data.
  • The data seemed, at a descriptive level, to suggest that recency of updates indirectly affected credibility judgments through cognitive elaboration.
  • A path analysis evaluated the possibility that the effect of recency of updates on credibility was mediated by the amount of cognitive elaboration in which a participant was engaged.

Discussion

  • The current study was designed to examine the impact that the recency/speed of updates on a social media page had on judgments of source credibility and the amount of cognitive elaboration a viewer 178 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171–183 © 2013 International Communication Association had after exposure to the page.
  • Specifically, participants looked at a Twitter page about heart disease in one of three recency conditions, and then responded to measures of cognitive elaboration (Perse, 1990) and source credibility (McCrosky & Tevern, 1999).
  • Data are consistent with the notion that recency of updates impacted cognitive elaboration, which in turn impacted source credibility.

Study Findings

  • The current study predicted that recency of updating would have a positive linear effect on perceived source credibility such that faster updates would lead to increased source credibility.
  • The data were consistent with this post hoc model.
  • The findings suggest that recency of updates might not have a direct impact on source credibility, but instead, that cognitive elaboration is a mediator in the relationship between recency of updates and credibility.
  • It also suggests that Sundar’s (2008) notion of the machine heuristic may operate in part because system-generated cues can create a situation that consumers of information need to think more about, and this thinking leads to higher judgments of credibility.
  • The immediacy of updating that is a hallmark of Twitter (Levinson, 2009) is likely a major reason this channel is growing in use for informational purposes, including under situations of risk and crisis.

Limitations and Future Directions

  • Perhaps the biggest limitation is the small effect size of the linear relationship between recency of updates and cognitive elaboration.
  • College students were used for this research in part because they are heavy users of social media.
  • One other potential limitation of the current study is the cognitive elaboration scale (Perse, 1990) utilized.

Conclusion

  • The current study examined how recency of updates on a social media page impacted source credibility and cognitive elaboration after exposure to the page.
  • As social media becomes a more heavily used information source, even for things as critical as risks and crises, the gatekeeping function of that information also falls more into the hands of the page users, rather than the page creators.
  • As such, it is important to continue learning more about this process, and learning about how and why credibility judgments are made about social media information.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

University of Kentucky
From the SelectedWorks of Patric R. Spence

% " ($%'#) %$%*'$-%
&)($'  " )-%$%'#) %$
+ ()'#$ North Dakota State University
)' &$ University of Kentucky
'$%$$' 
+ "") .&(,%'!(&'((%#&)' (&$

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
Social Media as Information Source: Recency
of Updates and Credibility of Information
David Westerman
Department of Communication, North Dakota State University, Department #2310, PO Box 6050, Fargo, ND
58108
Patric R. Spence
Division of Instructional Communication & Research, College of Communication and Information, University of
Kentucky, 310 Lucille Little Library (LCLI), Lexington, KY 40506-0224
Brandon Van Der Heide
School of Communication, The Ohio State University, 3016 Derby Hall, 154, North Oval Mall Columbus, OH
43210-1339
Social media are increasingly being used as an information source, including information related
to risks and crises. The current study examines how pieces of information available in social media
impact perceptions of source credibility. Specifically, participants in the study were asked to view 1 of
3 mock Twitter.com pages that varied the recency with which tweets were posted and then to report
on their perceived source credibility of the page owner. Data indicate that recency of tweets impacts
source credibility; however, this relationship is mediated by cognitive elaboration. These data suggest
many implications for theory and application, both in computer-mediated communication and crisis
communication. These implications are discussed, along with limitations of the current study and
directions for future research.
Key words: System-Generated Cues, Social Media, Recency, Twitter.
doi:10.1111/jcc4.12041
Newer communication technologies have increased the possibilities for how people can send and receive
information. Social mediaare one such technology that has seen increased usage as an information source
(Pepitone, 2010). For example, social media are being used to seek information about serious topics,
such as circulating up-to-the minute information about cholera outbreaks in Haiti and identifying clean
water sources during this outbreak (Sutter, 2010). Social media has also seen a great deal of usage by
those seeking health information, with 59% of adult Americans (80% of internet users) reporting that
they have accessed this type of information online (Fox, 2011). As this Pew Report suggests ‘‘people use
online social tools to gather information, share stories, and discuss concerns’’ (Fox, 2011, p. 5). Similarly
health professions and organizations are seeing the advantages of adopting social media because it is seen
as an information equalizer allowing access to health care information to populations who, in the past,
would not have this access (McNab, 2009). It provides a sense of privacy for the information seeker in
that he/she does not have to disclose personal information in order to obtain health related information.
Accepted by previous editor Maria Bakardjieva
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171183 © 2013 International Communication Association 171

However, a major question surrounding the use of social media as an information source is how
people assess the source credibility of this information (Westerman, Spence & Van Der Heide, 2012).
This question becomes especially important to answer for users of social media, as the gatekeeping
function switches from producers to consumers of information for newer technologies (Haas &
Wearden, 2003). These newer channels provide new pieces of information not available in ‘‘legacy’’
channels which may be used to make credibility judgments, such as the ability to see how quickly and
recently a page host updates their page. The current study examines how this piece of information
impacts a viewer’s cognitive elaboration and their perceived credibility of the source.
Social Media and Credibility Judgments
The types of information suggested in the first paragraph of this paper share at least one thing
in common: they all deal with uncertain situations highlighted by potential danger. Overall, when
uncertainty represents potential danger, people actively engage in information seeking (Brashers,
Neidig, Haas, Dobbs, Cardillo, & Russell, 2000; Spence, Lachlan & Griffin, 2007). They will seek
information from a variety of sources, and will constantly update their information. Mass media have
historically been a dominant source (Murch, 1971), possibly because they are generally thought to
provide credible, valuable, and timely information (Heath, Liao, & Douglas, 1995). However, along
with traditional forms of media, newer media are increasingly available for information seeking. One
channel that provides many opportunities for this purpose is the Internet. Research suggests that people
use the Internet in seeking information about crises (Spence, Westerman, Skalski, Seeger, Sellnow, &
Ulmer, 2006). More recently, social media have provided a new and potentially powerful platform for
people to use in seeking such information.
Social media are a general category of channels and applications that highlight collaboration and
working together to create and distribute content. This collaboration not only consists of creating
content together, but also discussing the content in an attempt to improve it collaboratively and to
come to a shared understanding of it. Thus, social media are built upon a fundamental characteristic of
Web 2.0: they are sites for harnessing collective intelligence (O’Reilly & Battelle, 2009). Many examples
of social media exist (e. g., Digg, Facebook, Youtube, Flickr), but one that holds great promise as a
social medium for information is Twitter (http://www.twitter.com). Twitter is a micro-blogging service
that began in March of 2006 (twitter.com).
The presence of risk raises an important issue for the consumption of social media. Often the
front lines of information come from eyewitnesses who are reporting on very recent events. In many
cases, even traditional mass media sources such as major news outlets glean information from these
sources prior to breaking news and providing information. Technological challenges in areas afflicted
by crises (i.e., down satellite connections, etc.) may slow official news correspondent reports, but
social media reports may be much more swiftly distributed. For example, during the January 2010
Haitian earthquake, social media played a key role in disseminating information about this tragedy
(Bunz, 2010). As Sutton, Palen, and Shklovski (2008) suggest, social media are gaining prominence as
an information source in disaster and risk time even though the accuracy of the information shared
through these channel is often unclear. This makes it imperative to learn more about how people
evaluate the information they consume on social media websites, especially judgments of the credibility
of this information.
Perceived source credibility has been defined as ‘‘judgments made by a perceiver ...concerning the
believability of a communicator’’ (O’Keefe, 1990, p. 181). Although there is debate about the precise
factor-structure of source credibility (see Cronkhite & Liska, 1976), one factor structure commonly
found includes three dimensions of source credibility: expertise/competence (i. e., the degree to which
172 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171 183 © 2013 International Communication Association

a perceiver believes a sender to know the truth), trustworthiness (i. e., the degree to which a perceiver
believes a sender will tell the truth as he or she knows it), and goodwill (i. e., the degree to which a
perceiver believes a sender has his or her best interests at heart.)
Perceived source credibility becomes an increasingly important variable to examine within social
media, especially in terms of crisis and risk information. This is because with the increasing amount
of information available through newer channels, the gatekeeping function seems to shift away from
producers of content and onto consumers of that content (Haas & Wearden, 2003). First conceptualized
and coined by Lewin (1947), and applied to the study of news by White (1950), gatekeeping is the
process through which content creators decide what stories will be covered and reported, and thus,
what information is released to consumers. Traditionally there are many people who act as gatekeepers,
including journalists, editors, and possibly even advertisers and owners (Shoemaker & Vos, 2009).
Along the way, these gatekeepers are assumed to be checking information for veracity, and can be an
important part in the process of ensuring the credibility of that information (Salcito, 2009), and are
likely perceived to be doing so by the public (Reese & Ballinger, 2001).
The continued growth of new media has meant that information consumers are now far less
beholden to what passes through traditional gatekeepers and are able to bypass gatekeepers altogether
and turn directly to primary information sources, many of which are information consumers themselves.
Because information provided in newer channels often lacks professional gatekeepers to check content,
and thus, lacks some of the traditional markers used to determine source credibility, consumers become
more responsible for making decisions about the credibility of information online. Therefore in new
media environments the gates are now located not only with the information providers but also with the
information consumers, who in the new media environment are acting as their own gatekeepers (Kovach
& Rosenstiel, 1999). This change has created a shift from the traditional notion of ‘‘gatekeeping’’ to
what Bruns (2008) has referred to as ‘‘gatewatching.’’ Gatewatchers are unable to control the gates
through which information passes, but instead keep a constant eye at the gates, and pass what flows
through those gates onto others who then make the choice about the topic relevance and usefulness.
Therefore gatewatchers fundamentally promote or diffuse information by making sources or stories
known to others in the new media environment. Rather than publishing unique information, they make
others’ information known and add to it. This can be seen in environments such as Facebook when a
user publishes a link and then comments on it, and similarly in Twitter.com where one does the same
thing or where the user reposts a link. In many respects, this is a hallmark of social media in general;
cocreation of content. This notion of ‘‘gatewatching’’ is echoed by Sundar (2008), who stated ‘‘The
digital media universe thus presents a dual challenge: (1) the overload of information, entertainment,
and other offerings that constantly need organizing and (2) the lack of assurance of any uniformity in
content quality, which necessitates a continual monitoring of credibility on the part of users’’ (p. 77).
It is important to consider that credibility is a perception, and thus is not a quality inherent within
a channel or source itself (Fogg & Tseng, 1999). Therefore, many things can impact the perceived
credibility of online materials (Metzger, Flanagan, Eyel, Lemus, & McCann, 2003). One model created
to articulate and explain the process of making credibility judgments in online settings, and thus a
useful framework for explaining ways in which consumers may enact their own personal process of
gatekeeping with this type of information, is the MAIN model (Sundar, 2008).
The MAIN Model and Recency of Updates
The MAIN model (Sundar, 2008) describes technological affordances that allow people to heuristically
process cues when making judgments about the credibility of an online source. According to the model,
system-generated pieces of information known as metrics are one type of affordance which can be used
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171183 © 2013 International Communication Association 173

as a heuristic in making credibility judgments. One metric that may be especially heuristically appealing
to people is an agency cue. Agency cues capitalize on heuristics that emphasize credibility cues that, for
example, are computer- (rather than user-) generated.
One heuristic Sundar (2008) argues is often utilized is known as the machine heuristic,whichisa
shortcut through which people assign greater credibility to information that is verified or chosen by a
machine or computer than by a person. People likely use this shortcut because a machine is seen as
something that has no thoughts, emotions, or other biases, and therefore is perceived to be free from
bias (whether or not the algorithm is actually free from bias.) This lack of perceived bias from a machine
leads to a greater trust in the information provided by machines compared to the information provided
by people such as editors, producers, and the like (Sundar & Nass, 2001). Similarly, this heuristic
may also impact the way that consumers of online information process system-generated cues. These
cues can even influence credibility judgments more strongly than the content of the message itself,
depending on the degree to which such a heuristic is activated and how heuristically or peripherally a
message is processed by a user. Past research exploring impression formation on Facebook has found
that system-generated cues can be important determinants of social judgments about social media users
(Kleck, Reese, Behnken, & Sundar, 2007; Tong, Van Der Heide, Langwell, & Walther, 2008; Utz, 2010),
suggesting that system-generated cues can affect interpersonal judgments of a profile owner.
One system-generated cue that could be important for credibility judgments is the recency (or
immediacy) of postings in this type of social media. As Sundar (2008) suggests, ‘‘[m]ore complex
examples of autogenerated cues appear in the form of navigational aids offered by algorithms used
in search-engine and aggregator sites such as Google News, which transmits cues about the relative
recency of the information, among other attributes. These appear as part ofor surroundingthe
central content of the site, and emit ‘‘information scent’’ helpful in making quick decisions about the
quality of the information available for consumption” (p. 78).
Social media seem designed to cater to those who want information in real time. As Levinson (2009)
has pointed out, one of Twitter’s hallmarks is the immediacy of messages. One important avenue to
study is how this immediacy, or recency of updates, acts as a cue that can impact credibility. Fogg
et al. (2001) found that something they call the ‘‘amateurism’’ of a website has the biggest impact of
decreasing credibility. One of the biggest markers of ‘‘amateurism’’ as they present it is the speed (or
recency) of updates. In fact, as they put together their amateurism scale, the two items with the biggest
impact on credibility deal with recency of updates, such that updating more frequently is associated with
higher credibility. As credibility is a perception, and not something inherent in the channel or website
itself, there is no reason to believe that the recency of updating should not also apply to the source of the
information presented on a social media site. Furthermore, if, as the machine heuristic (Sundar, 2008)
suggests, information provided by a machine (or a system-generated cue) offers especially valuable
credibility information because of people’s general operating heuristic that ‘‘machines don’t lie,’’ and
when this heuristic is paired with the recency principle highlighted by Fogg et al. (2001), a strong
influence on credibility judgments may exist. This leads to the first hypothesis of the current study:
H1: Recency of updating on a social media site will be positively associated with source credibility
of the site’s source.
Another important concept to examine for social media and credibility is cognitive elaboration.
Cognitive elaboration is demonstrated in active participation in information processing (Defleur &
Ball-Rokeach, 1989). This involvement process manifests in the mental process of attention, recognition
and subsequent elaboration (Greenwald & Leavitt, 1984). A central tenet of involvement is the sense
174 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 19 (2014) 171 183 © 2013 International Communication Association

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The authors provide an overview of research into social media rumours with the ultimate goal of developing a rumour classification system that consists of four components: rumour detection, rumor tracking, rumour stance classification, and rumour veracity classification.
Abstract: Despite the increasing use of social media platforms for information and news gathering, its unmoderated nature often leads to the emergence and spread of rumours, i.e., items of information that are unverified at the time of posting. At the same time, the openness of social media platforms provides opportunities to study how users share and discuss rumours, and to explore how to automatically assess their veracity, using natural language processing and data mining techniques. In this article, we introduce and discuss two types of rumours that circulate on social media: long-standing rumours that circulate for long periods of time, and newly emerging rumours spawned during fast-paced events such as breaking news, where reports are released piecemeal and often with an unverified status in their early stages. We provide an overview of research into social media rumours with the ultimate goal of developing a rumour classification system that consists of four components: rumour detection, rumour tracking, rumour stance classification, and rumour veracity classification. We delve into the approaches presented in the scientific literature for the development of each of these four components. We summarise the efforts and achievements so far toward the development of rumour classification systems and conclude with suggestions for avenues for future research in social media mining for the detection and resolution of rumours.

498 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The authors provide an overview of the current state of the literature on the relationship between social media; political polarization; and political "disinformation", a term used to encompass a wide range of types of information about politics found online.
Abstract: The following report is intended to provide an overview of the current state of the literature on the relationship between social media; political polarization; and political “disinformation,” a term used to encompass a wide range of types of information about politics found online, including “fake news,” rumors, deliberately factually incorrect information, inadvertently factually incorrect information, politically slanted information, and “hyperpartisan” news. The review of the literature is provided in six separate sections, each of which can be read individually but that cumulatively are intended to provide an overview of what is known — and unknown — about the relationship between social media, political polarization, and disinformation. The report concludes by identifying key gaps in our understanding of these phenomena and the data that are needed to address them.

494 citations


Cites background from "Social Media as Information Source:..."

  • ...In another experiment based on a student sample, Westerman et al. (2014) found that subjects exposed to different Twitter feeds were more likely to trust those that were more recently updated....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Results show that social media recommendations improve levels of media trust, and also make people want to follow more news from that particular media outlet in the future.
Abstract: Polls show a strong decline in public trust of traditional news outlets; however, social media offers new avenues for receiving news content. This experiment used the Facebook API to manipulate whether a news story appeared to have been posted on Facebook by one of the respondent's real-life Facebook friends. Results show that social media recommendations improve levels of media trust, and also make people want to follow more news from that particular media outlet in the future. Moreover, these effects are amplified when the real-life friend sharing the story on social media is perceived as an opinion leader. Implications for democracy and the news business are discussed.

399 citations


Cites background from "Social Media as Information Source:..."

  • ...…worth noting that scholars have recently found that the recency of social media activity enhances the perceived credibility of the message sender (Westerman et al., 2014); although we elected to utilize a friend with frequent social media interactions with the participant to increase the…...

    [...]

  • ...It is also worth noting that scholars have recently found that the recency of social media activity enhances the perceived credibility of the message sender (Westerman et al., 2014); although we elected to utilize a friend with frequent social media interactions with the participant to increase the external validity of the manipulation, future research should test this across interaction levels....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is indicated that social media use can significantly increase preventive behaviors via the two self-relevant emotions and the public’s risk perception through the mediating role of fear and anger.
Abstract: While there has been increasing attention to the role of social media during infectious disease outbreaks, relatively little is known about the underlying mechanisms by which social media use affects risk perception and preventive behaviors during such outbreaks. Using data collected during the 2015 Middle East Respiratory Syndrome coronavirus (MERS-CoV) outbreak in South Korea, this study explores the relationships among social media use, risk perception, and preventive behaviors by examining the mediating role of two self-relevant emotions: fear and anger. The findings demonstrate that social media use is positively related to both of these emotions, which are also positively related to the public's risk perception. The findings also indicate that social media use can significantly increase preventive behaviors via the two self-relevant emotions and the public's risk perception.

235 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Findings suggest that Twitterbots are perceived as credible, attractive, competent in communication, and interactional and there were no differences in the perceptions of source credibility, communication competence, or interactional intentions between the bot and human Twitter agents.

225 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

3,041 citations


"Social Media as Information Source:..." refers background in this paper

  • ...First conceptualized and coined by Lewin (1947), and applied to the study of news by White (1950), gatekeeping is the process through which content creators decide what stories will be covered and reported, and thus, what information is released to consumers....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In one of the first studies of its kind, all the wire stories used and all those rejected by a non-metropolitan newspaper over a seven-day period are classified by content, and the reasons given by editors as mentioned in this paper.
Abstract: In one of the first studies of its kind, all the wire stories used and all those rejected by a non-metropolitan newspaper over a seven-day period are classified by content, and the reasons given by...

1,217 citations


"Social Media as Information Source:..." refers background in this paper

  • ...First conceptualized and coined by Lewin (1947), and applied to the study of news by White (1950), gatekeeping is the process through which content creators decide what stories will be covered and reported, and thus, what information is released to consumers....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The members of the European Respiratory Society Task Force on Exercise Testing in Clinical Practice have read with interest the letter from J.E. Cotes and J.W. Reed and are of the opinion that any response to the points raised therein should be placed in the context of a recently published Task Force 1.
Abstract: The members of the European Respiratory Society Task Force on Exercise Testing in Clinical Practice have read with interest the letter from J.E. Cotes and J.W. Reed and are of the opinion that any response to the points raised therein should be placed in the context of the purpose of a recently published Task Force 1. As stated in the introduction of this Task Force 1: “The purpose of this document is to present recommendations on the clinical use of exercise testing in patients with cardiopulmonary disease, with particular emphasis on the evidence base for functional evaluation, prognosis and assessment of interventions. While the scope of the document is broad, consideration will focus only on those indices which have demonstrable predictive power”. The key phrase here is “evidence base”, which represents a clear departure from the objectives of the 1997 Task Force 2. In recent years, there has been an accumulating body of evidence across a broad range of chronic lung and heart diseases ( e.g. chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), interstitial lung disease (ILD), pulmonary vascular disease and chronic heart failure (CHF)) from studies using cycle ergometer protocols and field tests. It is upon this collective evidence base that the Task Force has formulated its recommendations for exercise testing in clinical practice. Furthermore, the Task Force was …

1,207 citations

Reference EntryDOI
22 Jan 2015
TL;DR: Several models have been proposed to describe diffusion of innovation as mentioned in this paper, which is the process by which an innovative product spreads within a market, and the diffusion rate is influenced by product characteristics and diffusion progresses by word of mouth.
Abstract: Diffusion of innovation is the process by which an innovative product spreads within a market. Several models have been proposed to describe this process. According to Rogers' model, the diffusion rate is influenced by product characteristics and diffusion progresses by word of mouth. Moore's crossing-the-chasm model focuses on differences between customer segments, and notes that word-of-mouth may not necessarily flow from early to later adopters. Both Rogers' and Moore's models offer managerial insights and implications. Diffusion models, such as Bass's model of durable goods sales, are often used to predict the rate of diffusion of innovative products into the market. Intriguing questions still remain regarding global diffusion of products. Keywords: diffusion of innovation; product diffusion; barriers to diffusion; diffusion models; crossing the chasm; Bass model; product life cycle

1,141 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The goal for this paper is to present a discussion of the assumptions of multiple regression tailored toward the practicing researcher that are not robust to violation, and that researchers can deal with if violated.
Abstract: Most statistical tests rely upon certain assumptions about the variables used in the analysis. When these assumptions are not met the results may not be trustworthy, resulting in a Type I or Type II error, or overor under-estimation of significance or effect size(s). As Pedhazur (1997, p. 33) notes, "Knowledge and understanding of the situations when violations of assumptions lead to serious biases, and when they are of little consequence, are essential to meaningful data analysis". However, as Osborne, Christensen, and Gunter (2001) observe, few articles report having tested assumptions of the statistical tests they rely on for drawing their conclusions. This creates a situation where we have a rich literature in education and social science, but we are forced to call into question the validity of many of these results, conclusions, and assertions, as we have no idea whether the assumptions of the statistical tests were met. Our goal for this paper is to present a discussion of the assumptions of multiple regression tailored toward the practicing researcher. Several assumptions of multiple regression are “robust” to violation (e.g., normal distribution of errors), and others are fulfilled in the proper design of a study (e.g., independence of observations). Therefore, we will focus on the assumptions of multiple regression that are not robust to violation, and that researchers can deal with if violated. Specifically, we will discuss the assumptions of linearity, reliability of measurement, homoscedasticity, and normality.

1,039 citations


"Social Media as Information Source:..." refers result in this paper

  • ...However, this actually suggests that relationships in the current study may be stronger than reported, as low reliabilities cause underestimation of relationships in simple correlation and regression analyses (Osborne & Waters, 2002)....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (2)
Q1. What are the contributions mentioned in the paper "Social media as information source: recency of updates and credibility of information" ?

This paper examined how recency of updates on a social media page impacted source credibility and cognitive elaboration after exposure to the page. 

Future studies examining a topic that requires faster updates might see an increased effect size between recency and cognitive elaboration. Thus, future studies can include this recency of updating as an important concept to study, and can more completely examine issues of topic and relevance in these studies. It is possible that future research, using topics with more personal relevance, may result in different findings, especially in regard to the effects sizes found. It would be interesting for future research to examine what specific thoughts the recency of updating leads to, and what specific thoughts lead to credibility judgments.