scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Book ChapterDOI

Spatial Biases and the Haptic Experience of Surface Orientation

27 Jan 2012-pp 75
TL;DR: The two main purposes of this chapter are to review past evidence for a systematic spatial bias in the perception of surface orientation (geographical slant), and to report two new experiments documenting this biases in the manual haptic system.
Abstract: The two main purposes of this chapter are to review past evidence for a systematic spatial bias in the perception of surface orientation (geographical slant), and to report two new experiments documenting this bias in the manual haptic system. Orientation is a fundamental perceptual property of surfaces that is relevant both for planning and implementing actions. Geographical slant refers to the orientation (inclination or pitch along its main axis) of a surface relative to the gravitationally-defined horizontal. It has long been known that hills appear visually steeper than they are (e.g., Ross, 1974). Only recently has it been documented that (1) there is also bias in the haptic perception of surface orientation (Hajnal et al., 2011), and that (2) similar visual and haptic biases even exist for small surfaces within reach (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010).

Summary (8 min read)

1. Introduction

  • The two main purposes of this chapter are to review past evidence for a systematic spatial bias in the perception of surface orientation (geographical slant), and to report two new experiments documenting this bias in the manual haptic system.
  • Geographical slant refers to the orientation (inclination or pitch along its main axis) of a surface relative to the gravitationally-defined horizontal.
  • Only recently has it been documented that (1) there is also bias in the haptic perception of surface orientation (Hajnal et al., 2011), and that (2) similar visual and haptic biases even exist for small surfaces within reach (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010).
  • Following the presentation of the experimental results the authors will discuss issues of measurement in perception – especially pertaining to the interpretation of verbal reports, and conclude with a discussion of functional theories of perceptual bias in the perception of surface orientation.

2. Spatial bias in the perception of orientation: Surfaces within manual reach

  • Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010) reported a series of studies of a bias they called the “vertical tendency” in slant www.intechopen.com Haptics Rendering and Applications 76 perception.
  • When Durgin, Li and Hajnal asked participants to make estimates of the geographical slants of wooden surfaces within reach, they found that that they got approximately the same bias function whether the surfaces were at eye level (so that “frontal” and vertical coincided) or viewed with gaze declined by about 40°.
  • The estimates shown in Figure 1 are based on verbal/numeric estimates of orientation relative to horizontal, but the bias observed cannot be due to verbal coding.
  • Thus, it appears that several different perceptual representations of pitch contain a bias that expands the scale of differences near horizontal while compressing the scale near vertical.

3. Bias in the perceived orientation of locomotor surfaces: Hills and ramps

  • A difficulty with this view is that perceived hill orientation decreases as one approaches a hill (Li & Durgin, 2010), and nearer portions of hills appear shallower than farther portions (Bridgeman & Hoover, 2008).
  • In fact, as the authors will discuss below, there is continuity between orientation biases they have measured for small, near surfaces and those measured by Proffitt et al. for hills (Li & Durgin, 2010).
  • Proffitt further proposed that physical actions, such as stepping, were controlled by an unbiased perceptual representation (vision for action) that was contrasted with the exaggerated representation available for long-range cognitive planning.
  • This view has since been challenged by studies of the haptic perception of locomotor inclines.

3.1 Haptic bias in the perceived orientation of locomotor surfaces

  • Hajnal et al. (2011) asked participants to step onto ramps that they could not see.
  • (They were either wearing a blindfold or an occluding collar that blocked their view of the floor.).
  • The data are reproduced in Figure 4, along with a photograph of the experimental situation.
  • For somewhat steep ramps, the haptic exaggeration of perceived slant was even greater than the visual exaggeration observed when the same participants judged the orientations of the ramps when looking at them afterward.
  • To rule out the possibility that the haptic exaggerations were learned from calibrating haptic experience to visual experience, Hajnal et al. (2011) also tested a population of four congenitally blind individuals using verbal report.

3.2 Proprioceptive bias in the perceived orientation of locomotor surfaces

  • Proprioceptive error in the perceived declination of gaze was first reported in a study of downhill slant perception: Li and Durgin (2009) observed that standing back from the edge of an outdoor downhill surface made it appear steeper than when standing closer to the www.intechopen.com Haptics Rendering and Applications 80 edge.
  • Indeed, for a steep hill, the maximum perceived orientation seemed to occur when standing far enough back from the edge of the hill that one’s line of gaze was nearly coincident with the surface of the hill.
  • Fig. 4. Verbal and proprioceptive (hand gesture) estimates of the haptically-perceived orientation of a ramp while standing on it, blindfolded (from Hajnal et al., 2011).
  • Proprioceptive points are displaced to show the SEMs.
  • Deducing from these observations that the perceived direction of gaze might itself be distorted Li and Durgin (2009) tested this directly by asking people to look at targets at various declinations out of upper-floor windows and estimate the downward pitch of their gaze.

3.3 Continuity between visual biases for near and far surfaces

  • Across a number of studies, Li and Durgin (2009, 2010; Durgin & Li, 2011a) have found systematic evidence suggesting that the visual perception of slant also has a gain of 1.5 in the low end of the geographical slant range.
  • Predictions of outdoor visual data from Proffitt et al. (1995) based on a model with only one free parameter.
  • By setting the slant gain to 1.5 and assuming an overall intercept of 0°, the authors can reduce the model to a single free parameter, based on the multiplier of log viewing distance.
  • This analysis provided by Li and Durgin (2010) shows how the apparent discrepancy between the perceived slants of hills and of near surfaces may be due to differences in viewing distance.
  • The most intriguing observation the authors can make about this concerns the discrepancy between the haptically perceived slant of the 16° ramp (~35°) and the visually perceived slant of that same ramp (~23°).

4. Problems with measuring perceived slant with haptic matching tasks

  • One current controversy in the study of slant perception concerns a popular method of assessing perceived slant.
  • Durgin, Hajnal, Li, Tonge and Stigliani (2010) reasoned that if palm boards were assessing accurate motor representations of space, then they ought to be particularly accurate for www.intechopen.com Spatial Biases and the Haptic Experience of Surface Orientation 83 matching near surfaces with which the hand could actually interact.
  • The hand was occluded from vision in all cases.
  • Because the axis of the palm board was near the wrist, the wrist had to be the principal joint for adjusting the palm board.
  • First, evidence of surprising accuracy between palm board matches and hills has turned out to be spurious.

4.1 Calibration between proprioception and the visual experience of slant

  • The function shown in Figure 6 for free-hand manual gestures was obtained with the same set of surfaces used to obtain the function in Figure 1 for verbal estimates of slant.
  • Nonetheless the verbal estimates show a great deal of bias (exaggeration of the deviation from horizontal), whereas the manual gestures appear to be well-calibrated.
  • That is, the perceived orientation of the hand ought to match the perceived orientation of the surface.
  • In support of this view, Li and Durgin have found that perceived hand orientation during free-hand gestures follows the same function as the verbal pattern shown in Figure 1.

4.2 An apparent discrepancy in the calibration account

  • So far the authors have suggested that hand gestures are calibrated to near surfaces, but are not calibrated for far surfaces (which seem steeper).
  • There is one apparent exception to this proposed guiding rule.
  • They collected verbal estimates of perceived slant.
  • Given the calibration account (based on the potential for shared visual and haptic experience in normal life) the authors should expect that haptic exploration of a surface by dynamic touch would reveal the same kind of spatial bias that is evident in vision and in static haptic contact .
  • In the left panel, the linear fit line originally plotted by Hajnal et al. is shown.

5. Experiment 1: Numeric estimation of real surface orientation in depth assessed by dynamic touch with the tip of the index finger

  • The main question of the present experiment is whether the haptic perception of surface orientation (geographical slant in the pitch axis relative to the observer) by dynamic touch will show the same kinds of spatial bias documented in vision by Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010).
  • As noted above, it is not clear that the linear fit they plotted is better justified by their data than a cubic fit, like that shown in Figure 7.
  • Moreover, examination of the raw data of Hajnal et al. suggested that participants relied nearly exclusively on angular estimates that were multiples of 5.
  • This may have contributed to distorting the lower end of the range.
  • Finally, because Hajnal et al. did not constrain their participants’ exploratory strategies, it is possible that the observed function was less exaggerated in some places because of a tendency for oblique paths of travel along the slanted surface.

5.1 Method

  • All experimental procedures were conducted in accord with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association and approved by a local institutional review board.
  • Participants made numeric estimates of the slant of surfaces explored haptically.

5.1.1 Participants

  • The participants were 20 Swarthmore College undergraduate students (13 female) who participated in partial fulfilment of a course requirement.
  • Half were assigned to the horizontal coding condition and half to the vertical coding condition.

5.1.2 Apparatus

  • The haptic surface was a varnished wooden board mounted on a mechanical adjustable slant device (see Li & Durgin, 2009).
  • The center of the surface was about 113 cm from the floor.
  • Participants stood in front of the apparatus wearing a blindfold (a plush sleep mask) throughout the experiment.

5.1.3 Design

  • Participants were assigned in alternation to the vertical or horizontal coding condition.
  • Following the practice of Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010), half the participants gave verbal estimates relative to horizontal and half gave estimates relative to vertical so that spatial biases could be distinguished from verbal biases.
  • Random orders were generated in advance for each participant.
  • Spatial Biases and the Haptic Experience of Surface Orientation 87.

5.1.4 Procedure

  • Participants were shown the apparatus with the surface in the horizontal position and the procedure was explained to them prior to signing an informed consent form.
  • Before each trial, the surfaces were set to the intended orientation manually using pre-set positions by the experimenter who then told the participant to explore the surface.
  • No time limit was specified for exploration.
  • Half were instructed that vertical was 0° and horizontal was 90°.
  • Trend lines are best fitting cubic polynomials.

5.2 Results

  • Mean estimates were computed for each presented orientation by condition.
  • Figure 9 shows the estimates for each condition.
  • It can be seen that the spatial bias was in the same direction in each condition inasmuch as participants overestimated deviations from horizontal and underestimated deviations from vertical.
  • The functions are strikingly similar, as predicted www.intechopen.com Haptics Rendering and Applications 88 by the calibration hypothesis.
  • Both functions seem to reflect a common underlying spatial coding bias.

5.3 Discussion

  • Using real surfaces with a demarcated axis of haptic exploration, the authors sought to extend the methods used by Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010) to the haptic domain.
  • The authors results indicate a close correspondence between visual and haptic spatial biases in the peception of orientation.
  • The authors results are somewhat at variance with those of Hajnal et al. (2011).
  • Because Hajnal et al. did not constrain the path of digital exploration, it is possible that participants tended to explore their surfaces along a somewhat oblique (and therefore less steep) axis.
  • The authors data are consistent with the proposal that there is a trend for there to be calibration between visual and haptic representations of 3D surface orientation.

6. Experiment 2: Horizontal/vertical bisection point for surface orientation in depth assessed by dynamic touch with the tip of the index finger

  • To avoid verbal biases, Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010) used a bisection task in which they presented surface visually and asked participants to indicate whether the surface was closer to vertical or to horizontal.
  • They reported a mean visual bisection of point of 34° from horizontal.
  • In fact the cubic fit to their verbal data predicted that the 45° point would have been at 36.3° in the visual case, and it seems likely that verbal reports tend to slightly underestimate the magnitude of the actual spatial bias (see also Durgin & Li, 2011a, 2011b).
  • The present experiment simply replicated the bisection procedure of Durgin, Li and Hajnal for the haptic case.
  • Spatial Biases and the Haptic Experience of Surface Orientation 89.

6.1.3 Design

  • Each participant gave responses to individual stimuli selected from an up-down staircase procedure.
  • There were 10 blocks of 6 trials each in which two trials from each of three staircases were randomly interleaved.
  • That is, if the presented orientation was deemed closer to vertical, the next orientation presented by that staircase was 18° lower, and if the presented orientation was judged closer to horizontal, the next presented orientation was 18° higher.
  • The three staircases together sampled orientation space with a resolution of 6° and approximated a method of constant stimuli that was centered on the apparent bisection point.

6.1.4 Procedure

  • Participants were shown the apparatus with the surface in the horizontal position and the procedure was explained to them prior to signing an informed consent form.
  • Participants were shown where to stand (directly in front of the apparatus) and then asked put on the blindfold.
  • Before each trial, the surfaces were set to the required orientation manually by the experimenter according to a computer instruction.
  • When the participant gave the forced choice response (“closer to vertical” or “closer to horizontal”), the experimenter pressed either the up-arrow key or the down arrow key on a keyboard, causing the computer to record the trial and update the staircase.
  • The computer then gave instruction to the experimenter concerning the orientation of the next stimulus.

6.2 Results

  • The responses for each participant were fitted with a logistic function and the subjective bisection point was calculated for each psychometric function as the point at which participants were equally likely to respond that the surface was closer to vertical and that it was closer to horizontal.
  • The average subjective horizontal/vertical bisection point was 31.2° (SEM = 2.0°) from horizontal.
  • Li and Hajnal (2010), this difference was not statistically reliable.

6.3 Discussion

  • Using real surfaces with a clearly-demarcated axis of haptic exploration, the authors sought to extend the bisection method used by Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010) to the haptic domain.
  • The authors results are consistent with a close correspondence between visual and haptic spatial biases in the perception of orientation.
  • The haptic results are also quite similar to the average perceived horizontal/vertical bisection point (31°) measured by Durgin and Li (2011a) for perceived gaze declination.
  • In other words, across a variety of modalities (proprioception of gaze declination, visual perception of 3D surface orientation in depth and haptic perception of 3D surface orientation in depth) the perceived bisection point between horizontal and vertical is very close to 30° from horizontal.

7. A descriptive model of the slant bias function for manual reaching space

  • The main purpose of their present study has been to clear up an apparent discrepancy in the coding of near-space orientation.
  • That is, for manual reaching space, there had seemed to be a discrepancy between the dynamic touch results of Hajnal et al. (2011) and the static haptic results of Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010).
  • The present results support the idea that the same bias exists for dynamic touch as has been found for visual slant perception and static haptic slant perception.
  • The bias function found previously for the evaluation of visual slants and for haptic slants experienced by static contact has now been replicated for dynamic touch by fingertip.
  • In combination with recent evidence that the proprioceptively-perceived orientation of a freely-extended hand shows a very similar bias function (Li & Durgin, submitted), the present results seem to point to a stable and systematic bias in the perception of 3D orientation in reachable space.

7.1 A family of biases

  • The authors description is intentionally limited to 3D slant in manual reaching space because it is fairly clear that the underfoot haptic perception of ramp orientation, for example, follows a rather steeper function than the one for manually reachable surfaces (Hajnal et al., 2011).
  • The shape of that function has only been explored over a limited range, however.
  • There seems to be some continuity between the nearspace bias function and biases shown in hill perception, once distance is taken into account.
  • Durgin and Li (2011a) have argued that the 1.5 gain also applies to perceived gaze declination in the relevant range of declinations (i.e. out to about 45°).
  • Second, in the present study, the bias function was very similar in shape when a very different set of numbers was required to produce it as a result www.intechopen.com Spatial Biases and the Haptic Experience of Surface Orientation 91 of labelling vertical as 0°.

7.2 Sine function scaling predicts the gain of 1.5

  • The characteristic shape of the error functions the authors have observed somewhat resembles the first quarter cycle of a sine function.
  • It appears fairly linear at the low end of the scale and compressive at the high end.
  • It turns out that the sine function produces a bias function with a gain of essentially 1.5 at the low end of the scale.
  • If the optical distance from the eye to a target is held fixed, then the sine function is proportional to the frontal vertical extent created between the www.intechopen.com Haptics Rendering and Applications 92 target and the horizontal plane at eye level.
  • At present the authors only propose that the sine function appears to capture the shape of the 3D angular bias function remarkably well.

7.3 An implied direction of calibration?

  • The idea that haptic scaling of perceived orientation during dynamic touch might be based on the ratio between the vertical extent of (finger-tip) travel and the total extent of travel is intriguing.
  • Gravitational forces are highly relevant to haptics.
  • The coding of orientation in this form would serve to express a useful ratio.
  • When the vertical component equals the total extent of travel, the surface is vertical.
  • An implied direction of calibration does not require that calibration always go in this direction, but only supposes that there is a natural basis for sinusoidal scaling of manual haptic slant perception and that this basis could then drive the visual scaling.

7.4 Scale expansion theory

  • For large scale space, Durgin and Li (2011a) have proposed that the special role of proprioception of gaze direction in estimating distance (e.g., Wallach & O’Leary, 1982) may encourage scale expansion near horizontal with a gain of 1.5.
  • Because their model provides impressive quantitative predictions of perceptual matching tasks (Li, Phillips & Durgin, 2011), it seems to capture an important feature of locomotor space perception.
  • Durgin and Li have proposed that the 1.5 gain in the scaling of perceived slant may be driven by the 1.5 gain in gaze proprioception.
  • That is, for horizontal ground surfaces to look flat requires a 1.5 gain in the optical slant.
  • Thus, it might be argued that the expanded scale of perceived gaze declination also creates pressure for an expanded scale of visual slant.

8. Conclusion

  • In this chapter the authors reviewed basic knowledge concerning spatial biases in the perception of slant and then presented novel experimental results.
  • The authors experiments tested whether the perceived 3D orientation bias function for surfaces explored by dynamic touch was similar to that for visually perceived slant and static haptic touch.
  • The authors have further suggested that the orientation bias function in manual reaching space resembles the first quarter cycle of a sine function.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Swarthmore College Swarthmore College
Works Works
Psychology Faculty Works Psychology
2012
Spatial Biases And The Haptic Experience Of Surface Orientation Spatial Biases And The Haptic Experience Of Surface Orientation
Frank H. Durgin
Swarthmore College
, fdurgin1@swarthmore.edu
Z. Li
Follow this and additional works at: https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-psychology
Part of the Psychology Commons
Let us know how access to these works bene<ts you
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Frank H. Durgin and Z. Li. (2012). "Spatial Biases And The Haptic Experience Of Surface Orientation".
Haptics Rendering And Applications
. 75-94.
https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-psychology/533
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
This work is brought to you for free by Swarthmore College Libraries' Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in
Psychology Faculty Works by an authorized administrator of Works. For more information, please contact
myworks@swarthmore.edu.

2,900+
OPEN ACCESS BOOKS
100,000+
INTERNATIONAL
AUTHORS AND EDITORS
94+ MILLION
DOWNLOADS
BOOKS
DELIVERED TO
151 COUNTRIES
AUTHORS AMONG
TOP 1%
MOST CITED SCIENTIST
12.2%
AUTHORS AND EDITORS
FROM TOP 500 UNIVERSITIES
Selection of our books indexed in the
Book Citation Index in Web of Science
Core Collection (BKCI)
Chapter from the book
Haptics Rendering and Applications
Downloaded from: http://www.intechopen.com/books /haptics -rendering-and-
applications
PUBLISHED BY
World's largest Science,
Technology & Medicine
Open Access book publisher
Interested in publishing with InTechOpen?
Contact us at book.department@intechopen.com

4
Spatial Biases and the Haptic
Experience of Surface Orientation
Frank H. Durgin and Zhi Li
Swarthmore College,
USA
1. Introduction
The two main purposes of this chapter are to review past evidence for a systematic spatial
bias in the perception of surface orientation (geographical slant), and to report two new
experiments documenting this bias in the manual haptic system. Orientation is a
fundamental perceptual property of surfaces that is relevant both for planning and
implementing actions. Geographical slant refers to the orientation (inclination or pitch along
its main axis) of a surface relative to the gravitationally-defined horizontal. It has long been
known that hills appear visually steeper than they are (e.g., Ross, 1974). Only recently has it
been documented that (1) there is also bias in the haptic perception of surface orientation
(Hajnal et al., 2011), and that (2) similar visual and haptic biases even exist for small surfaces
within reach (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010).
To provide a context for understanding the present experiments, we will first provide an
overview of the prior experimental evidence concerning bias in the perception of geographical
slant. First we will discuss findings from both vision and haptic perception that have
documented perceptual bias for surfaces in reach. We will then review the literature on the
visual and haptic biases in the perception of the greographical slant of locomotor surfaces such
as hills and ramps. At the intersection of these two literatures is the historical use of haptic
measures of perceived geographical slant, and we will therefore review these measures with
particular attention to understanding some pitfalls in the use of haptic measures of perception.
We next contrast these haptic measures with proprioceptive measures of perceived orientation
and discuss the problem in interpreting calibrated actions as measures of perception.
Having laid out these various past findings we will then report two novel experiments that
demonstrate spatial biases in the haptic experience of real surfaces. The experiments include
both verbal and non-verbal methods modelled on similar findings we have reported in the
visual domain. Following the presentation of the experimental results we will discuss issues
of measurement in perception especially pertaining to the interpretation of verbal reports,
and conclude with a discussion of functional theories of perceptual bias in the perception of
surface orientation.
2. Spatial bias in the perception of orientation: Surfaces within manual reach
What is meant by a spatial bias in the perception of surface orientation? Durgin, Li and
Hajnal (2010) reported a series of studies of a bias they called the “vertical tendency” in slant
www.intechopen.com

Haptics Rendering and Applications
76
perception. Specifically, they found that small, irregularly-shaped wooden surfaces
appeared steeper than they actually were both when viewed visually and when experienced
haptically while blindfolded. The term “vertical tendency” was used to distinguish the
observed effect from what has been called “frontal tendency” in the literature (Gibson,
1950). For many years it has been argued that surfaces viewed visually, appear compressed
along the depth axis of visual regard and thus appear more frontal to gaze than they are.
However, when Durgin, Li and Hajnal asked participants to make estimates of the
geographical slants of wooden surfaces within reach, they found that that they got
approximately the same bias function whether the surfaces were at eye level (so that
“frontal” and vertical coincided) or viewed with gaze declined by about 40°. Moreover, the
same kinds of bias were found when surfaces were experienced haptically by placing the
palm of the hand on them, though their measurements of this were limited to the angle of 0-
45°. The typical bias function for vision is shown in Figure 1.
Fig. 1. Surface orientation estimates for near visual surfaces presented within reach of the
hand (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010, Experiment 1). Symbol size approximates SEM.
The estimates shown in Figure 1 are based on verbal/numeric estimates of orientation
relative to horizontal, but the bias observed cannot be due to verbal coding. Essentially the
same spatial function was found if participants instead estimated orientation relative to
vertical and their responses were then subtracted from 90° in order to express them relative
to horizontal. Thus for example, a surface that was actually at a 42° orientation from
horizontal (and thus 48° from vertical), was estimated as being about 60° from horizontal by
one group of participants and about 30° from vertical by another. Clearly both groups saw it
as much steeper than its actual slant. When the same 42° surface was explored, haptically,
by a third group of participants by each placing the palm of the right hand against it while
blindfolded, it was also judged to be 60° from horizontal (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010,
Experiment 4).
www.intechopen.com

Spatial Biases and the Haptic Experience of Surface Orientation
77
Moreover, to emphasize that these biases did not depend on generating verbal estimates,
Durgin, Li and Hajnal (2010) asked a fourth set of participants to judge whether various
oriented planar surfaces were closer to horizontal or to vertical. They fit a psychometric
function to the resulting choice data and found that a surface slanted by only 34.3° from
horizontal was, on average, visually perceived to be equidistant from vertical and
horizontal.
This spatial bias function for near surfaces closely matches the observed proprioceptive
function for the perceived declination of gaze. That is, when people are asked to report the
pitch of their gaze, verbal reports provide evidence of an exaggerated deviation from
horizontal that closely matches the bias function shown above for perceived surface slant
(Durgin & Li, 2011a; Li & Durgin, 2009). Thus, it appears that several different perceptual
representations of pitch contain a bias that expands the scale of differences near horizontal
while compressing the scale near vertical.
Fig. 2. Surface orientation estimates for surfaces (0-48°) felt with the palm of the hand
(Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010, Experiment 4). Error bars indicate ±1 SEM.
In the haptic domain and in the proprioception of gaze, the perceptual scale of the bias
function for perceived pitch has mostly only been measured for orientations within about
50° from horizontal (e.g., Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010). In this range the scaling of pitch
tends to closely approximate a linear scale with a gain of 1.5 (Durgin & Li, 2011a). For
example the haptic data of Durgin, Li and Hajnal are shown in Figure 2. These data are
based on numeric estimates of orientation in deg (relative to horizontal) made based on
placing the palm of the hand on various real slanted surfaces while blindfolded. Durgin
and Li have reported a very similar function for explicit estimates of the pitch of gaze over
a similar range. Durgin and Li (like Durgin, Li & Hajnal) supplemented their verbal
estimation data with a horizontal-vertical bisection task and again found that a rather
shallow gaze declination of about 30° from horizontal was perceived as the bisection point
between vertical and horizontal gaze.
www.intechopen.com

Citations
More filters
01 Dec 2011
TL;DR: In this paper, it is argued that the navigational problems in three dimensions are qualitatively as well as quantitatively different from those in two dimensions, and evidence suggests that, perhaps for this reason, horizontal and vertical space are processed separately in the vertebrate brain.
Abstract: A central theme in the study of animal navigation has been the extent to which such navigation requires the formation of an internal representation of space, the so-called " cognitive map." Although its properties remain disputed, it is now generally accepted that a map-like representation exists in the brain, and neurobiological studies, conducted in tandem with behavioral investigations, have done much to elucidate the neural substrate of the map as it operates in two dimensions. However, to date little is known about how the map encodes real-world, three-dimensional space. Using recent neurobiological and behavioral fi ndings, this issue is explored here. It is argued that the navigational problems in three dimensions are qualitatively as well as quantitatively different from those in two dimensions, and evidence suggests that, perhaps for this reason, horizontal and vertical space are processed separately in the vertebrate brain. The possible adaptive consequences of such an anisotropic. © 2011 Massachusetts Institute of Technology and the Frankfurt Institute for Advanced Studies. All rights reserved.

105 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Evidence was collected from 120 observers showing that directly manipulating physiological potential, while controlling for experimental demand effects, does not alter the perception of slant.
Abstract: There is a current debate concerning whether people's physiological or behavioral potential alters their perception of slanted surfaces. One way to directly test this is to physiologically change people's potential by lowering their blood sugar and comparing their estimates of slant to those with normal blood sugar. In the first investigation of this (Schnall, Zadra, & Proffitt, 2010), it was shown that people with low blood sugar gave higher estimates of slanted surfaces than people with normal blood sugar. The question that arises is whether these higher estimates are due to lower blood sugar, per se, or experimental demand created by other aspects of the experiment. Here evidence was collected from 120 observers showing that directly manipulating physiological potential, while controlling for experimental demand effects, does not alter the perception of slant. Indeed, when experimental demand went against behavioral potential, it produced judgmental biases opposite to those predicted by behavioral potential in the low blood sugar condition. It is suggested that low blood sugar only affects slant judgments by making participants more susceptible to judgmental biases.

41 citations


Cites background from "Spatial Biases and the Haptic Exper..."

  • ...The studies of Li and Durgin (2010) and Durgin and Li (2012) have shown that Proffitt et al.’s hill data can be quantitatively modeled, as shown in Figure 2, by taking both slant and viewing distance into account....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In the current work, 4 replications carried out by 2 different laboratories tested an alternative anchoring hypothesis that manual action measures give low estimates because they are always initiated from horizontal, indicating that the bias from response anchoring can entirely account for the difference between manual and verbal estimates.
Abstract: People verbally overestimate hill slant by ~15–25° whereas manual estimates (e.g., palm board measures) are thought to be more accurate. The relative accuracy of palm boards has contributed to the widely cited theoretical claim that they tap into an accurate, but unconscious motor representation of locomotor space. In the current work, four replications (total N = 204) carried out by two different laboratories tested an alternative, anchoring hypothesis that manual action measures give low estimates because they are always initiated from horizontal. The results of all four replications indicate that the bias from response anchoring can entirely account for the difference between manual and verbal estimates. Moreover consistent correlations between manual and verbal estimates given by the same observers support the conclusion that both measures are based on the same visual representation. Concepts from the study of judgment under uncertainty apply even to action measures in information rich environments.

22 citations


Additional excerpts

  • ...… of  haptically-‐perceived  and  visually-‐perceived  near   surface  slant  have  shown  little  evidence  of  response  anchoring  when  manipulating   whether  vertical  or  horizontal  was  defined  as  0°  (Durgin  et  al.,  2010b;  Durgin  &  Li,   2012;  see  also  Durgin  &  Li,  2011a)....

    [...]

01 Jan 2017
TL;DR: This chapter reviews current knowledge of the phenomenology of slant misperception in relation to both functionalist and mechanistic accounts of this perceptual bias with respect to not only slant, but also other angular variables relevant to the biological measurement of surface layout.
Abstract: Hills look much steeper than they are. This chapter reviews current knowledge of the phenomenology of slant misperception in relation to both functionalist and mechanistic accounts of this perceptual bias. Recent discoveries suggest that this misperception of the geometry of our environment may be related to useful biological information coding strategies with respect to not only slant, but also other angular variables relevant to the biological measurement of surface layout. Even in the absence of hills, people misperceive the direction of their gaze systematically in ways that seem to contribute to the vertical expansion of the perceived environment.

20 citations


Cites background from "Spatial Biases and the Haptic Exper..."

  • ...In fact, a gain of 1.5 is consistent with the first quarter cycle of a sinusoidal function, which also seems to approximate the function that relates physical slant to perceived slant for small surfaces in reach (Durgin & Li, 2012; Durgin, Li, et al., 2010)....

    [...]

  • ...Spatial bias in the haptic perception of surface slant has recently been reviewed (Durgin & Li, 2012)....

    [...]

  • ...Evidence for Constancy Across Modalities As reviewed by us elsewhere, a sine-based model can apply to haptic surface perception by hand as well (Durgin & Li, 2012) and even to proprioception of hand orientation (Li & Durgin, 2012b)....

    [...]

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Recalibration of the transformation relating awareness and actions was found to occur over long-term changes in physiological potential (fitness level, age, and health) but not with transitory changes (fatigue and load).
Abstract: In 4 experiments, it was shown that hills appear steeper to people who are encumbered by wearing a heavy backpack (Experiment 1), are fatigued (Experiment 2), are of low physical fitness (Experiment 3), or are elderly and/or in declining health (Experiment 4). Visually guided actions are unaffected by these manipulations of physiological potential. Although dissociable, the awareness and action systems were also shown to be interconnected. Recalibration of the transformation relating awareness and actions was found to occur over long-term changes in physiological potential (fitness level, age, and health) but not with transitory changes (fatigue and load). Findings are discussed in terms of a time-dependent coordination between the separate systems that control explicit visual awareness and visually guided action.

577 citations


"Spatial Biases and the Haptic Exper..." refers background or methods in this paper

  • ...Similarly, the bias function for the perceived orientation of palm boards (Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999) has been shown to differ (especially at steeper orientations) from the haptic perception of stable surfaces (Durgin, Li & Hajnal, 2010)....

    [...]

  • ...Proffitt et al. (1995) developed a method of assessing perceived slant that they initially referred to as a haptic measure, but also (e.g., Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999) described as an action measure....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The writer has recently proposed the theory that visual space-perception is reducible to the perception of visual surfaces, and that distance, depth, and orientation, together with the constancy of objects, may all be derived from the properties of an array of surfaces.
Abstract: The writer has recently proposed the theory that visual space-perception is reducible to the perception of visual surfaces, and that distance, depth, and orientation, together with the constancy of objects, may all be derived from the properties of an array of surfaces.' The notion of completely empty space is asserted to be irrelevant for the theory of space-perception. The perception of space between objects, in this theory, depends on the experience of contours against a general background, and on the impression of one surface behind another. The fundamental 'sensations' of space are assumed to be the impressions of surface and edge. If the theory is valid, there should be discoverable variables in stimulation for the essential properties or qualities of a surface, and these should prove to be the effective stimuli for space-perception. What is called for is a listing of the properties or qualities-a phenomenological study-and a search for the corresponding stimulus variables-a program of psychophysical experiments.

455 citations


"Spatial Biases and the Haptic Exper..." refers background or methods in this paper

  • ...The term “vertical tendency” was used to distinguish the observed effect from what has been called “frontal tendency” in the literature (Gibson, 1950)....

    [...]

  • ...The palm board was originally developed by Gibson (1950) as a non-verbal measure of perceived slant....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is proposed that the perceived exaggeration of geographical slant preserves the relationship between distal inclination and people’s behavioral potential, thereby enhancing sensitivity to the small inclines that must actually be traversed in everyday experience.
Abstract: People judged the inclination of hills viewed either out-of-doors or in a computer-simulated virtual environment Angle judgments were obtained by having people (1) provide verbal estimates, (2) adjust a representation of the hill’s cross-section, and (3) adjust a tilt board with their unseen hand Geographical slant was greatly overestimated according to the first two measures, but not the third Apparent slant judgments conformed to ratio scales, thereby enhancing sensitivity to the small inclines that must actually be traversed in everyday experience It is proposed that the perceived exaggeration of geographical slant preserves the relationship between distal inclination and people’s behavioral potential Hills are harder to traverse as people become tired; hence, apparent slant increased with fatigue Visually guided actions must be accommodated to the actual distal properties of the environment; consequently, the tilt board adjustments did not reflect apparent slant overestimations, nor were they influenced by fatigue Consistent with the fact that steep hills are more difficult to descend than to ascend, these hills appeared steeper when viewed from the top

436 citations


"Spatial Biases and the Haptic Exper..." refers background or methods in this paper

  • ...…settings is no more than ± 10° from horizontal, and physical/biomechanical limitations restrict bipedal locomotion to surfaces of less than about 35°, Proffitt et al. (1995) proposed that the exaggerated perception of hill orientation was an adaptive strategy to represent the range of human…...

    [...]

  • ...In their studies of hills, Proffitt et al. (1995) placed the palm board at waist level so that it was at the edge of the field of view of the observer....

    [...]

  • ...Proffitt et al. (1995) developed a method of assessing perceived slant that they initially referred to as a haptic measure, but also (e.g., Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999) described as an action measure....

    [...]

  • ...Predictions of outdoor visual data from Proffitt et al. (1995) based on a model with only one free parameter....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The findings suggest that backpack effects, and other reported effects of effort on perception, are judgmental biases that result from the social, not physical, demands of the experimental context.
Abstract: A growing literature argues that wearing a heavy backpack makes slopes look steeper and distances seem longer (e.g., Proffitt, 2006). To test for effects of experimental demand characteristics in a backpack experiment, we manipulated the experimental demand of the backpack and then used a postexperiment questionnaire to assess participants’ beliefs about the purpose of the backpack. For participants in the low-demand condition, an elaborate deception was used to provide an alternative explanation of the requirement to wear a heavy backpack (i.e., that it held EMG equipment). The highest slope judgments were found for those undeceived participants who guessed that the backpack was intended to affect their slope perception and also reported that they thought they were affected by it. When persuaded that the backpack served another purpose, participants’ slope estimates were no different from those of participants not wearing a backpack. These findings suggest that backpack effects, and other reported effects of effort on perception, are judgmental biases that result from the social, not physical, demands of the experimental context.

231 citations


"Spatial Biases and the Haptic Exper..." refers background in this paper

  • ...The same pattern (higher estimates based on haptic perception) was found for a 14.5° ramp by Durgin et al. (2009) who collected visual estimates before having people step onto the ramp....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (8)
Q1. What are the contributions in "Spatial biases and the haptic experience of surface orientation" ?

In this paper, the authors reviewed the literature on the visual and haptic biases in the perception of the greographical slant of locomotor surfaces such as hills and ramps and reported two new experiments documenting this bias in the manual haptic system. 

By setting the slant gain to 1.5 and assuming an overall intercept of 0°, the authors can reduce the model to a single free parameter, based on the multiplier of log viewing distance. 

The guiding rule might be that calibration occurs when there is some real possibility for action with immediate spatial feedback from more than one modality. 

In the present study the authors used real surfaces and provided a ridge along the main axis of the surface to ensure that the steepest direction of inclination was felt. 

In other words, across a variety of modalities (proprioception of gaze declination, visual perception of 3D surface orientation in depth and haptic perception of 3D surface orientation in depth) the perceived bisection point between horizontal and vertical is very close to 30° from horizontal. 

For large scale (locomotor) space, Durgin and Li (2011a) have proposed that the special role of proprioception of gaze direction in estimating distance (e.g., Wallach & O’Leary, 1982) may encourage scale expansion near horizontal with a gain of 1.5. 

Across a number of studies, Li and Durgin (2009, 2010; Durgin & Li, 2011a) have found systematic evidence suggesting that the visual perception of slant also has a gain of 1.5 in the low end of the geographical slant range. 

Because their model provides impressive quantitative predictions of perceptual matching tasks (Li, Phillips & Durgin, 2011), it seems to capture an important feature of locomotor space perception.