scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Structural representations of fingers rely on both anatomical and spatial reference frames.

01 Feb 2020-Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (American Psychological Association)-Vol. 46, Iss: 2, pp 125-130
TL;DR: Investigating finger identification using the "in-between" test demonstrates that BSRs rely on both anatomical and external reference frames, and finger identification was modulated by anatomical and spatial proximity in external space between touches.
Abstract: Finger agnosia refers to a neurological condition in which patients with left posterior parietal lesions fail to identify their fingers, despite having relatively preserved abilities in sensation and skilled action. This dissociation suggests that the structural body representations (BSRs) may be distinct from sensorimotor representations. However, recent research has reported that postural changes modulate representation of hand structure, revealing dynamic interactions between structural and sensorimotor body representations. However, it is unknown how and to what extent anatomical and spatial proximity contribute to shape the hand structural representation. We investigate this question using the "in-between" test in which participants estimate how many unstimulated fingers are in between 2 touched fingers of the left hand placed palm down. The first phalange of the participants' fingers was touched on the left or right side. Judged finger numerosity was greater when fingers were stimulated on far sides (i.e., opposite sides of the 2 fingers) compared to when they were stimulated on close (i.e., sides facing each other's) or middistance (i.e., sides facing in the same direction) sides. Therefore, finger identification was modulated by anatomical and spatial proximity in external space between touches. This demonstrates that BSRs rely on both anatomical and external reference frames. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).

Summary (2 min read)

Introduction

  • The authors knowledge about the spatial relationships between body parts (e.g., limbs and fingers) is thought to be mediated by a representation known as the body structural description.
  • Here the authors explored the nature of BSRs and the spatial reference frames used for finger structural representation using an adapted version of the in-between test in which they stimulated the sides of fingers .
  • Participants’ task was to judge how many unstimulated fingers were touched in-between the two touched fingers.

Method

  • Participants reported normal touch and normal or corrected to normal vision.
  • Procedures were similar to their previous study (Tamè, Dransfield, et al., 2017).
  • Between trials there was a variable interstimulus interval, ranging from 1200 to 2200 ms, after participant’s response.
  • The participant’s task was to estimate how many unstimulated fingers there were in-between the two touched fingers, responding as quickly and accurately as possible.
  • The raw data are publicly available at https://osf.io/k6q5h.

Results

  • As Figure 1B illustrates, participants generally underestimated the number of unstimulated fingers.
  • The authors participants’ judgements closely corresponded to the actual changes in the spatial distance between stimuli, when there were one or two fingers in-between.
  • This suggests that BSRs in such a context, rely also on external spatial coordinates.

Discussion

  • The authors used the in-between test to examine the reference frames used by structural body representations when coding touch on the fingers.
  • Judgements were higher when the far sides were stimulated compared to when middistance or close sides were stimulated.
  • These results corroborate the notion that both the anatomical and the external spatial coordinates significantly contribute to finger representation.
  • Neighbouring finger stimulation resulted in overestimation of finger numerosity, but only when far finger sides were stimulated.
  • Note that for the three fingers space condition this can be over weighted given that there could be no more than three spaces.

Interplay of reference frames

  • The authors findings suggest that anatomical and external spatial reference frames are integrated and used to locate touch on the fingers and that these coordinates are used in finger structural representation based on touch.
  • Evidence discussed earlier supports this idea by demonstrating that the authors can employ multiple reference frames to represent touch on the body or body parts in space (e.g., Badde & Heed, 2016; Haggard et al., 2006; Tamè, Farnè, & Pavani, 2011b).
  • It has been proposed that spatial touch perception is achieved through the integration of multiple location codes that are weighted on the basis of the availability and reliability of all the spatial information (Badde & Heed, 2016).
  • In conclusion, their findings clearly demonstrate that a combination of the anatomical and the external spatial coordinates of touch are used in finger structural representation based on touch on the fingers of the same hand.
  • The present study supports the view that body structural representations are more flexible rather than fixed as previously thought.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Kent Academic Repository
Full text document (pdf)
Copyright & reuse
Content in the Kent Academic Repository is made available for research purposes. Unless otherwise stated all
content is protected by copyright and in the absence of an open licence (eg Creative Commons), permissions
for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher, author or other copyright holder.
Versions of research
The version in the Kent Academic Repository may differ from the final published version.
Users are advised to check http://kar.kent.ac.uk for the status of the paper. Users should always cite the
published version of record.
Enquiries
For any further enquiries regarding the licence status of this document, please contact:
researchsupport@kent.ac.uk
If you believe this document infringes copyright then please contact the KAR admin team with the take-down
information provided at http://kar.kent.ac.uk/contact.html
Citation for published version
Dolgilevica, Karina and Longo, Matthew R. and Tamè, Luigi (2019) Structural representations
of fingers rely on both anatomical and spatial reference frames. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance . ISSN 0096-1523. (In press)
DOI
Link to record in KAR
https://kar.kent.ac.uk/77647/
Document Version
Author's Accepted Manuscript

1
Structural representations of fingers rely on both anatomical and
spatial reference frames
Karina Dolgilevica
1
, Matthew R. Longo
1
, and Luigi Tamè
1,2
1
Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
2
School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, United Kingdom
Address for correspondence:
Luigi Tamè
School of Psychology
University of Kent
CT2 7NP, Canterbury, United Kingdom
E-Mail: l.tame@kent.ac.uk
Pages: 19; Figures: 1; Abstract: 200/200; Words: 2,468/2,500
Keywords: TACTILE, FINGER AGNOSIA, BODY STRUCTURAL REPRESENTATIONS, SIDE, SPACE

2
Abstract
Finger agnosia refers to a neurological condition in which patients with left posterior parietal
lesions fail to identify their fingers, despite having relatively preserved abilities in sensation
and skilled action. This dissociation suggests that the structural body representations (BSRs)
may be distinct from sensorimotor representations. However, recent research has reported
that postural changes modulate representation of hand structure, revealing dynamic
interactions between structural and sensorimotor body representations. However, it is
unknown how and to what extent anatomical and spatial proximity contribute to shape the
hand structural representation. We investigate this question using the in-betweentest in
which participants estimate how many unstimulated fingers are in-between two touched
fingers of the left hand placed palm down. The first phalange of the participants’ fingers was
touched on the left or right side. Judged finger numerosity was greater when fingers were
stimulated on far sides (i.e., opposite sides of the two fingers) compared to when they were
stimulated on close (i.e., sides facing each other’s) or mid-distance (i.e., sides facing in the
same direction) sides. Therefore, fingers identification was modulated both by anatomical
and spatial proximity in external space between touches. This demonstrates that BSRs rely on
both anatomical and external reference frames.

3
Significance Statement
Traditionally body representation research focuses on dissociation between the structural
body representations (BSRs) and the sensorimotor representations (e.g., body schema). Our
research supports the idea of a dynamic relationship between different body representations
as well as a more flexible representation of body’s structure in the bodily experience, which
are not fixed but varies as a function of the spatial relationships between the fingers. In the
context of finger BSRs based on touch, for instance, the adoption of an anatomical reference
frame is associated with neighbouring fingers, while an external reference frame with non-
neighbouring fingers. Despite this reference frame bias, we show that both the anatomical
and the external spatial coordinates of touch are considered when representing the fingers’
structure based on touch.

4
Introduction
Our knowledge about the spatial relationships between body parts (e.g., limbs and
fingers) is thought to be mediated by a representation known as the body structural
description. Neuropsychological cases of autotopagnosia, in which patients show a specific
deficit in pointing to their own or another’s body parts (Semenza & Goodglass, 1985) and
finger agnosia, a selective deficit in differentiating between one’s own or another’s fingers
(Kinsbourne & Warrington, 1962) provide evidence for the existence of such representations.
Notably, these patients often demonstrate relatively unimpaired skilled actions (Buxbaum &
Coslett, 2001).
The fact that patients with autotopagnosia are impaired at identifying body parts,
despite unaltered sensation and skilled action has traditionally been interpreted as evidence
that structural body representations (BSRs) are mediated by mechanisms different from
sensorimotor representations, such as the body schema (Anema et al., 2008; de Vignemont,
2010; Longo, Azañón, & Haggard, 2010; Schwoebel & Coslett, 2005; Tamè, Azañón, & Longo,
2019). This is intuitive, since body posture changes constantly but the structural configuration
of the body remains stable. Recent evidence from our laboratory has shown that BSRs are not
as fixed as previously believed, but are modulated by the relative positions of body parts - i.e.,
fingers (Tamè, Dransfield, Quettier, & Longo, 2017), suggesting that “on-line” and “off-line”
representations of the body are not completely distinct, but may dynamical interact.
A classic measure of finger agnosia is the so called in-betweentest (IBT) in which
participants judge the number of unstimulated fingers in-between two simultaneously
touched fingers (Kinsbourne & Warrington, 1962). We recently used this task to show that
the judged number of fingers in-between is generally underestimated, though increases when
the fingers are splayed compared to when they are pressed together (Tamè, Dransfield, et al.,

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
02 Jul 2020-PLOS ONE
TL;DR: A strong effect of Light Touch was found, which resulted in improved stability following a perturbation, which indicates an adaptive process following exposure to repetitive trials of perturbations.
Abstract: Light touch with an earth-fixed reference point improves balance during quite standing. In our current study, we implemented a paradigm to assess the effects of disrupting the right posterior parietal cortex on dynamic stabilization of body sway with and without Light Touch after a graded, unpredictable mechanical perturbation. We hypothesized that the benefit of Light Touch would be amplified in the more dynamic context of an external perturbation, reducing body sway and muscle activations before, at and after a perturbation. Furthermore, we expected sway stabilization would be impaired following disruption of the right Posterior Parietal Cortex as a result of increased postural stiffness. Thirteen young adults stood blindfolded in Tandem-Romberg stance on a force plate and were required either to keep light fingertip contact to an earth-fixed reference point or to stand without fingertip contact. During every trial, a robotic arm pushed a participant's right shoulder in medio-lateral direction. The testing consisted of 4 blocks before TMS stimulation and 8 blocks after, which alternated between Light Touch and No Touch conditions. In summary, we found a strong effect of Light Touch, which resulted in improved stability following a perturbation. Light Touch decreased the immediate sway response, steady state sway following re-stabilization, as well as muscle activity of the Tibialis Anterior. Furthermore, we saw gradual decrease of muscle activity over time, which indicates an adaptive process following exposure to repetitive trials of perturbations. We were not able to confirm our hypothesis that disruption of the rPPC leads to increased postural stiffness. However, after disruption of the rPPC, muscle activity of the Tibialis Anterior is decreased more compared to sham. We conclude that rPPC disruption enhanced the intra-session adaptation to the disturbing effects of the perturbation.

5 citations


Cites background from "Structural representations of finge..."

  • ...Dolgilevica and colleagues [31] proposed a conceptual framework which emphasizes the role of body representations such as the postural configuration of the body as well as the size and shape of body segments in the spatial localization of touch....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article , the authors investigated the implicit processing of body parts by manipulating either the body part stimuli' posture (conditions a and b) or the participants' response body posture(conditions c, d, and e) in healthy participants (N = 70) using a spatial compatibility task called Sidedness task.
Abstract: Abstract The way human bodies are represented is central in everyday activities. The cognitive system must combine internal, visceral, and somatosensory, signals to external, visually driven information generated from the spatial placement of others’ bodies and the own body in the space. However, how different body representations covertly interact among them when observing human body parts is still unclear. Therefore, we investigated the implicit processing of body parts by manipulating either the body part stimuli’ posture (conditions a and b) or the participants’ response body posture (conditions c, d, and e) in healthy participants (N = 70) using a spatial compatibility task called Sidedness task. The task requires participants to judge the colour of a circle superimposed on a task‐irrelevant body part picture. Responses are facilitated when the spatial side of the responding hand corresponds to the spatial code generated by the hand stimulus's position with respect to a body of reference. Results showed that the observation of the task‐irrelevant body parts oriented participants’ attention and facilitated responses that were spatial compatible with the spatial position such body parts have within a configural representation of the body structure (i.e., Body Structural Representation) in all the five experimental conditions. Notably, the body part stimuli were mentally attached to the body according to the most comfortable and less awkward postures, following the anatomo‐physiological constraints. Moreover, the pattern of the results was not influenced by manipulating the participants’ response postures, suggesting that the automatic and implicit coding of the body part stimuli does not rely on proprioceptive information about one's body (i.e., Body Schema). We propose that the human body's morphometry knowledge is enriched by biomechanical and anatomo‐physiological information about the real body movement possibilities. Moreover, we discuss the importance of the automatic orienting of attention based on the sidedness within the context of imitational learning.

3 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results showed highly similar pattern of mislocalisation for the knuckles of one's own hand and those of another person's hand, suggesting that people share an abstract conceptual knowledge about the hand structure.
Abstract: Hands play a fundamental role in everyday behaviour. Nevertheless, healthy adults show striking misrepresentations of their hands which have been documented by a wide range of studies addressing various aspects of body representation. For example, when asked to indicate the location within the hand of the knuckles, people place them substantially farther forward than they actually are. Previous research, however, has focused exclusively on the knuckles at the base of each finger, not considering the other knuckles in the fingers. This study, therefore, aimed to investigate conceptual knowledge of the structure of the whole hand, by investigating judgements of the location of all 14 knuckle joints in the hand. Participants localised each of the 14 knuckles of their own hand (Experiment 1) or of the experimenter's hand (Experiment 2) on a hand silhouette. We measured whether there are systematic localisation biases. The results showed highly similar pattern of mislocalisation for the knuckles of one's own hand and those of another person's hand, suggesting that people share an abstract conceptual knowledge about the hand structure. In line with previous reports, we showed that the metacarpophalangeal joints at the base of the fingers are judged as substantially father forward in the hand than they actually are. Moreover, for the first time we showed a gradient of this bias, with progressive reduction of distal bias from more proximal to more distal joints. In sum, people think their finger segments are roughly the same, and that their fingers are shorter than they are.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An inventory of 20 items with a set of instructions and response- and computational-conventions is proposed and the results obtained from a young adult population numbering some 1100 individuals are reported.

33,268 citations


"Structural representations of finge..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...All were right-handed, as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971; M 90, range 47–100)....

    [...]

01 Jan 1971
TL;DR: In this paper, an inventory of 20 items with a set of instructions and response-and computational-conventions is proposed and the results obtained from a young adult population numbering some 1100 individuals are reported.
Abstract: The need for a simply applied quantitative assessment of handedness is discussed and some previous forms reviewed. An inventory of 20 items with a set of instructions and response- and computational-conventions is proposed and the results obtained from a young adult population numbering some 1100 individuals are reported. The separate items are examined from the point of view of sex, cultural and socio-economic factors which might appertain to them and also of their inter-relationship to each other and to the measure com- puted from them all. Criteria derived from these considerations are then applied to eliminate 10 of the original 20 items and the results recomputed to provide frequency-distribution and cumulative frequency functions and a revised item-analysis. The difference of incidence of handedness between the sexes is discussed.

3,559 citations


"Structural representations of finge..." refers methods in this paper

  • ...All were right-handed, as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971; M 90, range 47–100)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is argued that to best comprehend many data sets, plotting judiciously selected sample statistics with associated confidence intervals can usefully supplement, or even replace, standard hypothesis-testing procedures.
Abstract: We argue that to best comprehend many data sets, plotting judiciously selected sample statistics with associated confidence intervals can usefully supplement, or even replace, standard hypothesis-testing procedures. We note that most social science statistics textbooks limit discussion of confidence intervals to their use in between-subject designs. Our central purpose in this article is to describe how to compute an analogous confidence interval that can be used in within-subject designs. This confidence interval rests on the reasoning that because between-subject variance typically plays no role in statistical analyses of within-subject designs, it can legitimately be ignored; hence, an appropriate confidence interval can be based on the standard within-subject error term-that is, on the variability due to the subject × condition interaction. Computation of such a confidence interval is simple and is embodied in Equation 2 on p. 482 of this article. This confidence interval has two useful properties. First, it is based on the same error term as is the corresponding analysis of variance, and hence leads to comparable conclusions. Second, it is related by a known factor (√2) to a confidence interval of the difference between sample means; accordingly, it can be used to infer the faith one can put in some pattern of sample means as a reflection of the underlying pattern of population means. These two properties correspond to analogous properties of the more widely used between-subject confidence interval.

2,432 citations


"Structural representations of finge..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Error bars indicate 95% within participants’ confidence intervals (Loftus & Masson, 1994)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, a conceptual analysis of the body schema contrasting it with the body image(s) is provided, as well as assess whether (i) body schema can be specifically impaired, while other types of body representation are preserved.

483 citations


"Structural representations of finge..." refers background in this paper

  • ...…body representations (BSRs) are mediated by mechanisms different from sensorimotor representations, such as the body schema (Anema et al., 2008; de Vignemont, 2010; Longo, Azañón, & Karina Dolgilevica and Matthew R. Longo, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London; X…...

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A wide range of neuropsychological, neuroimaging and neurophysiological data is reviewed to explore the dissociation between these different aspects of higher somatosensory function.

399 citations