scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.

J. R. Stroop1
01 Jan 1992-Journal of Experimental Psychology: General (Psychological Review Company)-Vol. 18, Iss: 6, pp 643-662
About: This article is published in Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.The article was published on 1992-01-01 and is currently open access. It has received 15981 citations till now.

Summary (3 min read)

INTRODUCTION

  • Interference or inhibition (the terms seem to have been used almost indiscriminately) has been given a large place in experimental literature.
  • Bergstrom (1894, p. 441) concluded that "the interference effect of an association bears a constant relation to the practice effect, and is, in fact, equivalent to it.".
  • The increase in the time for reacting to colors caused by the presence of conflicting word stimuli is taken as the measure of the interference of word stimuli upon naming colors.

EXPERIMENTAL

  • The materials employed in these experiments are quite different from any that have been used to study interference.
  • In former studies the subjects were given practice in responding to a set of stimuli until associative bonds were formed between the stimuli and the desired responses, then a change was made in the experimental 'set up' which demanded a different set of responses to the same set of stimuli.
  • In the present study pairs of conflicting stimuli, both being inherent aspects of the same symbols, are presented simultaneously (a name of one color printed in the ink of another color-a word stimulus and a color stimulus).
  • These stimuli are varied in such a manner as to maintain the potency of their interference effect.
  • Detailed descriptions of the materials used in each of the three experiments are included in the reports of the respective experiments.

The Effect of Interfering Color Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colors Serially Materials:

  • When this experiment was contemplated, the first task was to arrange suitable tests.
  • Also, because of the difficulty of printing words in yellow that would approximate the stimulus intensity of the other colors used, yellow was discarded.
  • The colors were arranged so as to avoid any regularity of occurrence and so that each color would appear twice in each column and in each row, and that no color would immediately succeed iuelf in either column or row.
  • The words were also arranged so that the name of each color would appear twice in each line.
  • The test was printed from fourteen point Franklin lower case type.

Subjects and Procedure:

  • Seventy college undergraduates (14 males and 56 females) were used as subjects.
  • Every subject read two whole sheets (the two forms) of each test at one sitting.
  • All subjects were in the experimental room a few minutes before beginning work to allow the eyes to adjust to light conditions.
  • Contrary to instructions 14 subjects left a total of 24 errors uncorrected on the RCNd test, 4 was the maximum for any subject, and 4 other subjects left I error each on the RCNb test.
  • The subject was seated near a window so as to have good daylight illumination from the left side.

The Effect of Interfering Word Stimuli upon Naming Colors Serially Materials:

  • For this experiment the colors of the words in the RCNd test, described in Experiment I, were printed in the same order but in the form of solid squares from 24 point type instead of words.
  • This sort of problem will be referred to as the "Naming color test" (NC).
  • The RCNd test was employed also but in a very different manner from that in Experiment 1.
  • Thus color of the print was to be the controlling stimulus and not the name of the color spelled by the word.
  • This is to be known as the "Naming color of word test where the color of the print and the word are different".

Results:

  • The means of the times for the NC and NCWd tests for the whole group and for each sex are presented in Table III along with the difference, the probable error of the difference, the reliability of the difference, and the difference divided by the mean time for the naming color test.
  • The comparison of the results for the whole group on the NC and NCWd test given in the bottom line of the table indicates the strength of the inteference of the habit of calling words upon the activity of naming colors.
  • The coefficient of variability remains the same to the third decimal place {aim = .171).
  • The means for the sex groups on the NCWd test show a difference of 3.6 seconds which is only 1.16 times its probable error; but the means on the NC test have a difference of 8.2 seconds which is 5.17 times its probable error.
  • The graphical representation of the results in the practice series gives the learning curve presented in Fig. 2 .

Materials:

  • This change also made it possible to print the NC test in shades which more nearly match those in the NCWd test.
  • The order of colors was determined under one restriction other than those given in section 2.
  • The test schedule was followed in regular daily order with two exceptions.
  • These two were finally dropped from the group and their data rejected.

The Effect of Practice on the NCWd Test upon the NC Test

  • This is only 23.7 percent of the gain on the NCWd test which means that less than one fourth of the total gain on the NCWd test is due to increase in speed in naming colors.
  • The improvement is greater for the males, which is accounted for by the fact that there is more difference between naming colors and reading names of colors for the males than for the females.

The Effects in the RCNd Test of Practice on the

  • The right section of Table VI shows that the practice on the NCWd and NC tests resulted in heavy loss in speed on the RCNd test.
  • A comparison of the right and left sections of the table shows that the loss on the RCNd test, when measured in absolute units, is practically equal to the gain on the NCWd test; when measured in relative units it is much greater.
  • The data from this experiment present interesting findings on the effect of practice upon individual differences.
  • A sex difference in naming colors has been found by all who have studied color naming and has been generally attributed to the greater facility of women in verbal reactions than of men.
  • As a test of the permanency of the interference of conflicting word stimuli to naming colors eight days practice (200 reactions per day) were given in naming the colors of the print of words (each word naming a color unlike the inkcolor of its print).

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Journal
of
Experimental Psychology
VOL. XVIII,
No. 6
DECEMBER,
1935
STUDIES
OF
INTERFERENCE
IN
SERIAL
VERBAL REACTIONS
BY
J.
RIDLEY STROOP»
George
Peabody
College
INTRODUCTION
Interference
or
inhibition
(the
terms seem
to
have been
used almost indiscriminately)
has
been given
a
large place
in
experimental literature.
The
investigation
was
begun
by
the physiologists prior
to 1890
(Bowditch
and
Warren,
J. W.,
1890)
and has
been continued
to the
present, principally
by
psychologists (Lester, 1932).
Of the
numerous studies that
have been published during this period only
a
limited number
of
the
most relevant reports demand
our
attention here.
Miinsterberg (1892) studied
the
inhibiting effects
of
changes
in
common daily habits such
as
opening
the
door
of
his
room, dipping
his pen in ink, and
taking
his
watch
out
of
his
pocket.
He
concluded that
a
given association
can
function automatically even though some effect
of a
previous
contrary association remains.
Mviller
and
Schumann (1894) discovered that more time
*
The writer
wishes
to
acknowledge
the
kind assistance received
in
the preparation
of this thesis.
He is
indebted
to Dr.
Joseph Peterson
for
encouragement, helpful
suggestions,
and
criticism
of the
manuscript;
to
Major
H. W.
Fenker,
a
graduate
student
in
psychology,
for
helpful suggestions relative
to
preparation
of the
manu-
script;
to
Drs.
J.
Peterson,
S. C.
Garrison,
M. R.
Schneck,
J. E.
Caster, O. A. Simley,
W.
F. Smith,
and
to
Miss
M. Nichol
for
aid
in
securing subjects; to
some
three hundred
college students
who
served
as
subjects;
and to
William Fitzgerald
of The
Peabody
Press
for
substantial assistance
in the
printing
of
the test materials.
643

644 / RIDLEY STROOP
was necessary to relearn a series of nonsense syllables if the
stimulus syllables had been associated with other syllables in
the meantime. From their results they deduced the law of
associative inhibition which is quoted by Kline
(1921,
p. 270)
as follows: "If a is already connected with b, then it is diffi-
cult to connect it with k, b gets in the way." Nonsense syl-
lables were also used by Shepard and Fogelsonger (1913) in
a series of experiments in association and inhibition. Only
three subjects were used in any experiment and the changes
introduced to produce the inhibition were so great in many
cases as to present novel situations. This latter fact was
shown by the introspections. The results showed an in-
crease in time for the response which corresponded roughly
to the increase in the complexity of the situation. The only
conclusion was stated thus: "We have found then that in
acquiring associations there is involved an inhibitory process
which is not a mere result of divided paths but has some deeper
basis yet unknown" (p. 311).
Kline (1921) used 'meaningful' material (states and
capitals, counties and county seats, and books and authors)
in a study of interference effects of associations. He found
that if the first associative bond had a recall power of 10 per-
cent or less it facilitated the second association, if it had a
recall power of 15 percent to 40 percent the inhibitory power
was small, if it had a recall power of 45 percent to 70 percent
the inhibiting strength approached a maximum, if the recall
power was 70 percent to 100 percent the inhibition was of
medium strength and in some cases might disappear or even
facilitate the learning of a new association.
In card sorting Bergstrom (1893 and 1894), Brown (1914),
Bair (1902), and Culler (1912) found that changing the
arrangement of compartments into which cards were being
sorted produced interference effects. Bergstrom (1894, p.
441) concluded that "the interference effect of an association
bears a constant relation to the practice effect, and is, in fact,
equivalent to it." Both Bair and Culler found that the inter-
ference of the opposing habits disappeared if the habits were
practiced alternately.

INTERFERENCE JN SERIAL VERBAL
REACTIONS-
645
Culler (1912), in the paper already referred to, reported
two other experiments. In one experiment the subjects
associated each of a series of numbers with striking a parti-
cular key on the typewriter with a particular finger; then the
keys were changed so that four of the numbers had to be
written with fingers other than those formerly used to write
them. In the other experiment the subjects were trained to
react with the right hand to 'red' and with the left hand to
'blue.'
Then the stimuli were interchanged. In the former
experiment an interference was found which decreased rapidly
with practice. In the latter experiment the interference was
overbalanced by the practice effect.
Hunter and Yarbrough (1917), Pearce (1917), and Hunter
(1922) in three closely related studies of habit interference
in the white rat in a T-shaped discrimination box found that
a previous habit interfered with the formation of an 'opposite'
habit.
Several studies have been published which were not pri-
marily studies of interference, but which employed materials
that were similar in nature to those employed in this research,
and which are concerned with why it takes more time to
name colors than to read color names. Several of these
studies have been reviewed recently by Telford (1930) and
by Ligon (1932). Only the vital point of these studies will
be mentioned here.
The difference in time for naming colors and reading color
names has been variously explained. Cattell (1886) and
Lund (1927) have attributed the difference to 'practice.'
Woodworth and Wells
(1911,
p. 52) have suggested that,
"The real mechanism here may very well be the mutual
interference of the five names, all of which, from immediately
preceding use, are 'on the tip of the tongue,' all are equally
ready and likely to get in one another's way." Brown (1915,
p.
51) concluded "that the difference in speed between color
naming and word reading does not depend upon practice"
but that (p. 34) "the association process in naming simple
objects like colors is radically different from the association
process in reading printed words."

646 /. RIDLEY STROOP ]
Garrett and Lemmon (1924, p. 438) have accounted for
their findings in these words, "Hence it seems reasonable to
say that interferences which arise in naming colors are due
not so much to an equal readiness of the color names as to an
equal readiness of the color recognitive processes. Another
factor present in interference is very probably the present
strength of the associations between colors and their names,
already determined by past use." Peterson (1918 and 1925)
has attributed the difference to the fact that, "One particular
response habit has become associated with each word while
in the case of colors themselves a variety of response ten-
dencies have developed." (1925, p. 281.) As pointed out by
Telford (1930), the results published by Peterson (1925,
p.
281) and also those published by Lund (1927, p. 425)
confirm Peterson's interpretation.
Ligon (1932) has published results of a 'genetic study'
of naming colors and reading color names in which he used
638 subjects from school grades 1 to 9 inclusive. In the
light of his results he found all former explanations untenable
(He included no examination of or reference to Peterson's
data and interpretation.) and proceeded to set up a new
hypothesis based upon a three factor theory, a common factor
which he never definitely describes and special factors of
word reading and color naming. He points out that the com-
mon factor is learned but the special factors are organic. He
promises further evidence from studies now in progress.
The present problem grew out of experimental work in
color naming and word reading conducted in Jesup Psy-
chological Laboratory at George Peabody College For
Teachers. The time for reading names of colors had been
compared with the time for naming colors themselves. This
suggested a comparison of the interfering effect of color
stimuli upon reading names of colors (the two types of
stimuli being presented simultaneously) with the interfering
effect of word stimuli upon naming colors themselves. In
other words, if the word 'red' is printed in blue ink how will
the interference of the ink-color 'blue' upon reading the
printed word 'red' compare with the interference of the

INTERFERENCE
IN
SERIAL VERBAL REACTIONS
647
printed word
'red'
upon calling
the
name
of the
ink-color
'blue?'
The
increase
in
time
for
reacting
to
words caused
by
the
presence
of
conflicting color stimuli
is
taken
as the
measure
of the
interference
of
color stimuli upon reading
words.
The
increase
in the
time
for
reacting
to
colors
caused
by the
presence
of
conflicting word stimuli
is
taken
as
the measure
of the
interference
of
word stimuli upon naming
colors.
A
second problem grew
out of the
results
of the
first.
The problem
was,
What effect would practice
in
reacting
to
the color stimuli
in the
presence
of
conflicting word stimuli
have upon
the
reaction times
in the two
situations described
in
the
first problem?
EXPERIMENTAL
The materials employed
in
these experiments
are
quite
different from
any
that have been used
to
study interference.
1
In former studies
the
subjects were given practice
in
respond-
ing
to a set of
stimuli until associative bonds were formed
between
the
stimuli
and the
desired responses, then
a
change
was made
in the
experimental
'set up'
which demanded
a
different
set of
responses
to the
same
set of
stimuli.
In the
present study pairs
of
conflicting stimuli, both being inherent
aspects
of the
same symbols,
are
presented simultaneously
(a
name
of one
color printed
in the ink of
another color—a
word stimulus
and a
color stimulus). These stimuli
are
varied
in
such
a
manner
as to
maintain
the
potency
of
their
interference effect. Detailed descriptions
of the
materials
used
in
each
of the
three experiments
are
included
in the re-
ports
of the
respective experiments.
EXPERIMENT
I
The Effect
of
Interfering Color Stimuli Upon Reading Names
of Colors Serially
Materials:
When this experiment
was
contemplated,
the
first task
was to
arrange suitable
tests.
The
colors used
on the
Woodworth-Wells color-sheet were considered
but two
changes were deemed advisable.
As the
word test
to be
used
in
comparison with
the
1
Descoeudres (1914)
and
also Goodenough
and
Brian (1929) presented color
and
form simultaneously
in
studying their relative values
as
stimuli.

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results suggest that it is important to recognize both the unity and diversity ofExecutive functions and that latent variable analysis is a useful approach to studying the organization and roles of executive functions.

12,182 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is proposed that cognitive control stems from the active maintenance of patterns of activity in the prefrontal cortex that represent goals and the means to achieve them, which provide bias signals to other brain structures whose net effect is to guide the flow of activity along neural pathways that establish the proper mappings between inputs, internal states, and outputs needed to perform a given task.
Abstract: ▪ Abstract The prefrontal cortex has long been suspected to play an important role in cognitive control, in the ability to orchestrate thought and action in accordance with internal goals. Its neural basis, however, has remained a mystery. Here, we propose that cognitive control stems from the active maintenance of patterns of activity in the prefrontal cortex that represent goals and the means to achieve them. They provide bias signals to other brain structures whose net effect is to guide the flow of activity along neural pathways that establish the proper mappings between inputs, internal states, and outputs needed to perform a given task. We review neurophysiological, neurobiological, neuroimaging, and computational studies that support this theory and discuss its implications as well as further issues to be addressed

10,943 citations


Cites background from "Studies of interference in serial v..."

  • ...But flexibility carries a cost: Although our elaborate sensory and motor systems provide detailed information about the external world and make available a large repertoire of actions, this introduces greater potential for interference and confusion....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors (1977) in a series of experiments and demonstrated the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search.
Abstract: Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors (1977) in a series of experiments. The studies (a) demonstrate the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search; (b) trace the course of the

7,032 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Two computational modeling studies are reported, serving to articulate the conflict monitoring hypothesis and examine its implications, including a feedback loop connecting conflict monitoring to cognitive control, and a number of important behavioral phenomena.
Abstract: A neglected question regarding cognitive control is how control processes might detect situations calling for their involvement. The authors propose here that the demand for control may be evaluated in part by monitoring for conflicts in information processing. This hypothesis is supported by data concerning the anterior cingulate cortex, a brain area involved in cognitive control, which also appears to respond to the occurrence of conflict. The present article reports two computational modeling studies, serving to articulate the conflict monitoring hypothesis and examine its implications. The first study tests the sufficiency of the hypothesis to account for brain activation data, applying a measure of conflict to existing models of tasks shown to engage the anterior cingulate. The second study implements a feedback loop connecting conflict monitoring to cognitive control, using this to simulate a number of important behavioral phenomena.

6,385 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is concluded that recent theories placing the explanatory weight on parallel processing of the irrelevant and the relevant dimensions are likely to be more sucessful than are earlier theories attempting to locate a single bottleneck in attention.
Abstract: The literature on interference in the Stroop Color-Word Task, covering over 50 years and some 400 studies, is organized and reviewed. In so doing, a set of 18 reliable empirical finding is isolated that must be captured by any successful theory of the Stroop effect. Existing theoretical positions are summarized and evaluated in view of this critical evidence and the 2 major candidate theories ―relative speed of processing and automaticity of reading― are found to be wanting. It is concluded that recent theories placing the explanatory weight on parallel processing of the irrelevant and the relevant dimensions are likely to be more sucessful than are earlier theories attempting to locate a single bottleneck in attention

5,172 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1886-Mind

471 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: There is good reason to believe that the mechanism of these clonic movements is the same as that of the ordinary knee-jerk, and it might naturally be expected that both phenomena would be similarly influenced by peripheric nerve stimulation.
Abstract: THE study of the modifications of the knee-jerk brought about by the simultaneous activity of other portions of the nervous system is important both on account of the evidence as to the real nature of the knee-jerk which may thus be obtained and because an investigation of these phenomena is likely to throw light upon the mysterious processes of the central nervous system. That the exaggerated tendon reflexes observed in certain diseases of the central nervous system can be inhibited by the stimulation of the skin or of the peripheral nerves has been noticed by various observers', but the reflexes thus inhibited were in nearly all the cases reported the clonic movements produced under morbid conditions by stretching the tendons of the muscles concerned. Erb, however, mentions a single case of myelitis from compression in which the tendon reflex as well as the trembling of the legs were stopped at once by pinching the skin of the abdomen. There is good reason to believe that the mechanism of these clonic movements is the same as that of the ordinary knee-jerk, and it might therefore naturally be expected that both phenomena would be similarly influenced by peripheric nerve stimulation, but that the normal knee-jerk of a healthy individual can be thus inhibited has not yet, as far as we are aware, been a matter of recorded observation. On the other hand the reinforcement of the nornmal knee-jerk by various forms of activity of the nervous system has during the last few years been made a subject of careful study by many independent observers. Jendrissik' first called attention to the fact "that if, when the patellar tendon is struck, the patient clinches the hand, or

65 citations

Journal ArticleDOI

61 citations