scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Social Bonds: Integrating testosterone and peptide responses for classifying social behavioral contexts

01 Oct 2011-Psychoneuroendocrinology (Elsevier Publishing)-Vol. 36, Iss: 9, pp 1265-1275
TL;DR: The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Social Bonds (S/P Theory), which integrates T and peptides to provide a model, set of predictions, and classification system for social behavioral contexts related to social bonds, is presented.
About: This article is published in Psychoneuroendocrinology.The article was published on 2011-10-01 and is currently open access. It has received 251 citations till now. The article focuses on the topics: Aggression & Poison control.

Summary (4 min read)

1. Introduction

  • The close ties between hormones and pair bonds have been studied in a variety of species, with most research focusing on the peptides oxytocin (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP).
  • The authors goal is to bring T and peptides together to better understand their joint evolutionary significance in pair bonds and other social bonds via the Steroid/Peptide Theory of Social Bonds (S/P Theory).
  • Using partner cuddling as a test case, the authors next detail the utility of the S/P.
  • Theory for classifying “tricky” behavioral contexts whose “kind” is not immediately apparent or can be misleading.
  • A major goal of this paper is to discuss how neuroendocrine responses provide a proximate means for addressing evolutionary questions about pair bonds and other social bonds.

2.1. Evolutionary significance of pair bonds

  • Pair bonds may be evolutionarily adaptive, enhancing biparental care and parent—offspring bonds when they promote parent or offspring fitness in some way (Snowdon, 2001; Fernandez-Duque et al., 2009).
  • Pair bonds are typically understood to exist between one female and one male.
  • The two-point assumption is undertheorized, however, as the presence of a pair bond between two individuals does not necessarily preclude additional pair bonds (see Fig. 1).
  • There are a host of species in which multiple females appear to bond with one male.

2.2. Proximate mechanisms for pair bonds

  • Hormones often are the proximate mechanisms by which social traits critical to life-history trade-offs are expressed and can be helpful in gaining insights into ultimate function.
  • Moreover, researchers have shown that hormone responsivity, specifically, is notably important in studying the comparative evolution of mating systems (McGlothlin et al., 2010).
  • Pair and parent-offspring bonds may overlap evolutionarily in aspects of function and mechanism, but also clearly diverge as pair bonds additionally facilitate sexual contact and reproductive potential: then the existence of two systems underlying/contributing to pair bonds is supported.
  • As far as the authors know, only humans engage in sexual activity with the intent to reproduce, but most human sexuality is not oriented around reproduction and instead around sexual pleasure, power, relationship maintenance, intimacy, displays of gender, or resource acquisition.

3. The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Social Bonds

  • The authors put forth an overarching conceptual umbrella they call the Steroid/Peptide Theory of Social Bonds (S/P Theory).
  • Theory has two major aspects and one minor feature, which the authors introduce briefly here but are best examined in the forthcoming sections when noted.
  • The first major aspect of the S/P Theory is the S/P Model (Fig. 3), which details how a specifically defined set of behavioral contexts affect T, OT, and AVP, and how these in turn facilitate or inhibit social bonds.
  • In contrast, the S/P Model is useful for delineating how two sets of behavioral contexts can affect T and peptides, to either facilitate or inhibit social bonds.
  • The Challenge Hypothesis posits a trade-off between low T and parenting (including pair bonds) versus high T and challenges (including mating/sexuality).

3.1.1. The Offspring Defense Paradox

  • The Challenge Hypothesis positions all parental behaviors in a low T category (Wingfield et al., 1990), but offspring defense has been linked to high T (Teichroeb and Sicotte, 2008), leading to what the authors call the “Offspring Defense Paradox” (i.e., that parental behaviors are linked to low T, but infant defense increases T).
  • Theory, the authors hypothesize that low T is linked only to parental contexts that are perceived/ experienced as nurturant, and high T is implicated in those parental contexts that invoke the need for defensive or protective responses.
  • In the S/P Framework (Fig. 4), the authors classify behaviors on the basis of their apparent or evolutionary motivation, and any behavioral context linked to competition, including defending resources (and offspring certainly represent resources), is linked to high T.

3.1.2. The Aggression Paradox

  • As the Offspring Defense Paradox (i.e., that all parental behaviors are linked to low T, but offspring defense increases T) exposes a conceptual gap in the social role for T, aggression exposes a similar conceptual gap in the social role of peptides.
  • Peptides are most generally tied to intimacy within social bonds (Carter, 1998), yet they also contribute to certain types of aggression.
  • In other words, some types of aggression can increase peptides and thus facilitate social bonds, and other types of aggression can increase T and inhibit social bonds.
  • The authors speculate that peptides are more salient than T for social bonds, and that this could be reflected via a number of mechanisms.
  • Permissive effects of T on AVP (Carter, 2007) also might be at play such that stimuli that increase both T and AVP lead to an androgenic upregulation of AVP (and thus higher AVP).

3.1.3. The Intimacy Paradox

  • The Challenge Hypothesis holds that sexual activity should be tied to high T and pair bonds to low T (Wingfield et al., 1990), and does not address links between sexuality and pair bonds directly.
  • Sexual activity actually facilitates pair bonds and very often occurs within a pair bond context (Carter, 1998; Snowdon et al., 2006; van Anders et al., 2007a).
  • The authors speculate that sexual intimacy is a more salient stimulus for T than nurturant intimacy.
  • In other words, there have been stronger selective pressures for a T response to sexual intimacy because of its importance in reproduction.

3.2. The Steroid/Peptide Framework of Social Bonds

  • The S/P Framework lays out a more general conceptual hypothesis for the role that T and OT play in social bonds (Fig. 4).
  • Though the authors focus on OT, they expect that other peptides and estradiol will fulfill similar roles (Lynch and Wilczynski, 2006; Heinrichs et al., 2009).
  • The S/P Matrix (Table 1) makes a set of predictions specifically about the interactions between T, OT, and AVP for behavioral contexts related both to social bonds and their obverse (e.g., missing or dysfunctional bonds).
  • There are some “tricky” behaviors that are either difficult to categorize or easy to miscategorize based on cultural (mis)-conceptions, and the S/P Framework can be used to make hypotheses about the evolutionary role of these behaviors based on the hormonal response they elicit.
  • Below (in Section 3.3), the authors discuss cuddling as a test case for this use of the S/P Framework.

3.2.1. Testosterone and social goals

  • High T is related to competition, that is, social behavioral contexts that involve acquiring or keeping resources.
  • Resources are broadly defined and include traditionally conceptualized resources like territory, status, etc., but also sexual/mating opportunities and offspring.
  • Actual examples of competitive behavioral contexts could include sexual activity, offspring defense, mate/partner guarding including jealousy, acts designed to acquire status or territory (e.g., fights and status-oriented maneuvering), etc.
  • This in many ways overlaps with Wingfield et al.’s (1990) concept of challenge, and recent reviews provide support for links between these behavioral contexts and high T (Hirschenhauser and Oliveira, 2006; van Anders and Watson, 2006b).
  • As such, low T is linked to nurturant behavioral contexts—those that involve loving warm contact with others.

3.2.2. Oxytocin and bonding goals

  • High OT is tied to social behavioral contexts that involve social bonds, and this can relate to the anticipation of these contexts, the need/desire for these contexts, or their existence (e.g., initiation, development, maintenance).
  • Perpetrating rape or offenses that hurt others without the concomitant experience of empathy for victims might also lead to or reflect dysregulation (that itself could sometimes have resulted from early abuse/neglect: Pedersen, 2004).
  • Low OT is instead linked to dysregulated social bonds, which differs conceptually from “poor” social bonds.

3.3. Cuddling: a test case of the S/P framework for “tricky” behavioral contexts

  • As the authors note above and in Fig. 3, sexual and nurturant intimacy are linked to high OT.
  • Theory, the authors were able to develop a hypothesis that cuddling involves sexual intimacy, leading to a better classification of partner cuddling, and supporting the S/P Theory.

4. Gender/sex and the S/P Theory of Social Bonds

  • Though there are increasing exceptions, research on nonhuman species with peptides and T has largely been gendered, with OT measured in females, and AVP in males; in contrast, peptide administration studies in humans are more often conducted with men.
  • There is evidence for overlapping function (Carter, 2007), as OT and AVP facilitate protective aggression (Bosch, 2011), and both peptides seem to function in males and females (Gouin et al., 2010; Snowdon et al., 2010).
  • The authors find that sexual stimuli are more likely to increase T in women than men (van Anders et al., 2007a; Goldey and van Anders, 2011, submitted for publication).
  • Thus, there may be gender/sex sensitivities in the specificity or type of hormonal response.
  • As an example, certain activities may be high T competitive for men but not women because men have been taught to value those activities as competitive, while women have not (e.g., handshake grip strength); or women have not been encouraged or allowed to engage in those activities, while men have been.

5. Conclusion

  • The authors have put forth the Steroid/Peptide Theory of Social Bonds (S/P Theory), highlighting its relevance for understanding evolutionary pathways to pair bonds and other social bonds, as well as its importance in classifying and conceptualizing behavioral contexts.
  • Theory provides a way to conduct theoretically rooted experimental work with humans (or other species) that does not rely on more invasive approaches, including administration, that is, by testing hormonal responses to social behavioral contexts.
  • In addition, the S/P Theory reflects gaps in the literature in that research has largely focused on OT rather than AVP or prolactin (PRL), and T rather than other steroids.
  • By partitioning intimacy and aggression, the S/P Theory also resolves several paradoxes apparent in the literature on social bonds and hormones: the Offspring Defense Paradox, Aggression Paradox, and Intimacy Paradox.
  • Role of the funding source – K.L.G. was supported by a Graduate Research Fellowship from the National Science Foundation (grant no. DGE 0718128).

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Figures (6)
Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: 5 sets of empirical findings are described that fundamentally undermine the gender binary, spanning multiple disciplines, that refute sexual dimorphism of the human brain and psychological findings that highlight the similarities between men and women.
Abstract: The view that humans comprise only two types of beings, women and men, a framework that is sometimes referred to as the "gender binary," played a profound role in shaping the history of psychological science. In recent years, serious challenges to the gender binary have arisen from both academic research and social activism. This review describes 5 sets of empirical findings, spanning multiple disciplines, that fundamentally undermine the gender binary. These sources of evidence include neuroscience findings that refute sexual dimorphism of the human brain; behavioral neuroendocrinology findings that challenge the notion of genetically fixed, nonoverlapping, sexually dimorphic hormonal systems; psychological findings that highlight the similarities between men and women; psychological research on transgender and nonbinary individuals' identities and experiences; and developmental research suggesting that the tendency to view gender/sex as a meaningful, binary category is culturally determined and malleable. Costs associated with reliance on the gender binary and recommendations for future research, as well as clinical practice, are outlined. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).

436 citations


Cites background from "The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Socia..."

  • ...…increases related to competition (involving acquisition of resources, broadly defined), providing a theoretical basis that is grounded in both sociocultural and evolutionary understandings as well as other endocrine systems (via the steroid/peptide theory of social bonds; van Anders et al., 2011)....

    [...]

  • ...…it provides a framework for dynamically incorporating evolutionary and social context into research with hormones and humans (van Anders, 2013; van Anders et al., 2011), for example, by exploring how gendered social experiences influence what are typically seen as bodily markers of sex, like…...

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the authors propose a testable, empirically grounded framework for understanding diverse partnered sexualities, separate from solitary sexualities and discuss a sexual diversity lens as a way to study the particularities and generalities of diverse sexualities without privileging either.
Abstract: Sexual orientation typically describes people's sexual attractions or desires based on their sex relative to that of a target. Despite its utility, it has been critiqued in part because it fails to account for non-biological gender-related factors, partnered sexualities unrelated to gender or sex, or potential divergences between love and lust. In this article, I propose Sexual Configurations Theory (SCT) as a testable, empirically grounded framework for understanding diverse partnered sexualities, separate from solitary sexualities. I focus on and provide models of two parameters of partnered sexuality--gender/sex and partner number. SCT also delineates individual gender/sex. I discuss a sexual diversity lens as a way to study the particularities and generalities of diverse sexualities without privileging either. I also discuss how sexual identities, orientations, and statuses that are typically seen as misaligned or aligned are more meaningfully conceptualized as branched or co-incident. I map out some existing identities using SCT and detail its applied implications for health and counseling work. I highlight its importance for sexuality in terms of measurement and social neuroendocrinology, and the ways it may be useful for self-knowledge and feminist and queer empowerment and alliance building. I also make a case that SCT changes existing understandings and conceptualizations of sexuality in constructive and generative ways informed by both biology and culture, and that it is a potential starting point for sexual diversity studies and research.

371 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is argued that disputes about the nature and prevalence of teaching across human societies and nonhuman animals are based on a number of deep-rooted theoretical differences between fields, as well as on important differences in how teaching is defined.
Abstract: The human species is more reliant on cultural adaptation than any other species, but it is unclear how observational learning can give rise to the faithful transmission of cultural adaptations. One possibility is that teaching facilitates accurate social transmission by narrowing the range of inferences that learners make. However, there is wide disagreement about how to define teaching, and how to interpret the empirical evidence for teaching across cultures and species. In this article I argue that disputes about the nature and prevalence of teaching across human societies and nonhuman animals are based on a number of deep-rooted theoretical differences between fields, as well as on important differences in how teaching is defined. To reconcile these disparate bodies of research, I review the three major approaches to the study of teaching – mentalistic, culture-based, and functionalist – and outline the research questions about teaching that each addresses. I then argue for a new, integrated framework that differentiates between teaching types according to the specific adaptive problems that each type solves, and apply this framework to restructure current empirical evidence on teaching in humans and nonhuman animals. This integrative framework generates novel insights, with broad implications for the study of the evolution of teaching, including the roles of cognitive constraints and cooperative dilemmas in how and when teaching evolves. Finally, I propose an explanation for why some types of teaching are uniquely human, and discuss new directions for research motivated by this framework.

303 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: How the increasing use of meta-analysis helped to clarify sex difference findings if not the causal explanations for these effects is considered, as well as developing research trends that address the interactive processes by which nature and nurture work together in producing sex differences and similarities.
Abstract: Nature-nurture debates continue to be highly contentious in the psychology of gender despite the common recognition that both types of causal explanations are important. In this article, we provide a historical analysis of the vicissitudes of nature and nurture explanations of sex differences and similarities during the quarter century since the founding of the Association for Psychological Science. We consider how the increasing use of meta-analysis helped to clarify sex difference findings if not the causal explanations for these effects. To illustrate these developments, this article describes socialization and preferences for mates as two important areas of gender research. We also highlight developing research trends that address the interactive processes by which nature and nurture work together in producing sex differences and similarities. Such theorizing holds the promise of better science as well as a more coherent account of the psychology of women and men that should prove to be more influential with the broader public.

269 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A more comprehensive look at the distinct networks identified by social neuroscience and the importance of a motivational state, in addition to approach and avoidance, associated with quiescence and homeostatic regulation is advocated.
Abstract: Extensive animal and recent human research have helped inform neuroendocrinological models of social cognition, motivation and behavior. In this review, we first summarize important findings regarding oxytocin, arginine vasopressin and testosterone in the domains of affiliation, social cognition, aggression and stress/anxiety. We then suggest ways in which human research can continue to profit from animal research, particularly by exploring the interactive nature of neuromodulatory effects at neurochemical, organismic and contextual levels. We further propose methods inspired by the animal literature for the ecologically valid assessment of affiliative behavior in humans. We conclude with suggestions for how human research could advance by directly assessing specific social cognitive and motivational mechanisms as intermediate variables. We advocate a more comprehensive look at the distinct networks identified by social neuroscience and the importance of a motivational state, in addition to approach and avoidance, associated with quiescence and homeostatic regulation.

259 citations

References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This model indicates that there may be widely different hormonal responses to male-male and male-female interactions and presumably equally plastic neural mechanisms for the transduction of these signals into endocrine secretions.
Abstract: A combination of field and laboratory investigations has revealed that the temporal patterns of testosterone (T) levels in blood can vary markedly among populations and individuals, and even within individuals from one year to the next. Although T is known to regulate reproductive behavior (both sexual and aggressive) and thus could be expected to correlate with mating systems, it is clear that the absolute levels of T in blood are not always indicative of reproductive state. Rather, the pattern and amplitude of change in T levels are far more useful in making predictions about the hormonal basis of mating systems and breeding strategies. In these contexts we present a model that compares the amplitude of change in T level with the degree of parental care shown by individual males. On the basis of data collected from male birds breeding in natural or captive conditions, polygynous males appear less responsive to social environmental cues than are monogamous males. This model indicates that there may be wi...

2,098 citations


"The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Socia..." refers background or methods in this paper

  • ...The Challenge Hypothesis positions all parental behaviors in a low T category (Wingfield et al., 1990), but offspring defense has been linked to high T (Teichroeb and Sicotte, 2008), leading to what we call the ‘Offspring Defense Paradox’ (i.e., that parental behaviors are linked to low T, but infant defense increases T)....

    [...]

  • ...The Offspring Defense Paradox The Challenge Hypothesis positions all parental behaviors in a low T category (Wingfield et al., 1990), but offspring defense has been linked to high T (Teichroeb and Sicotte, 2008), leading to what we call the ‘Offspring Defense Paradox’ (i....

    [...]

  • ...The Challenge Hypothesis positions all parental behaviors in a low T category (Wingfield et al., 1990), but offspring defense has been linked to high T (Teichroeb and Sicotte, 2008), leading to what we call the ‘Offspring Defense Paradox’ (i.e., that parental behaviors are linked to low T, but…...

    [...]

  • ...Our model builds on the Challenge Hypothesis, but adds to it by clarifying that low T is linked to nurturance in any context, rather than parental behaviors in general....

    [...]

  • ..., 2009), and because it is so strongly implicated in trade-offs relevant to pair bonding (Wingfield et al., 1990; Ketterson et al., 2005; Bales et al., 2006; van Anders and Watson, 2006b)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A review of existing behavioral and neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love reveals a recurrent association between high levels of activity in the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis and the subsequent expression of social behaviors and attachments.

1,170 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This review focuses on recent knowledge of the behavioral, endocrine, genetic, and neural effects of OT and AVP in humans and provides a synthesis of recent advances made in the effort to implicate the oxytocinergic system in the treatment of psychopathological states.

730 citations


"The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Socia..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Though we focus on OT, we expect that other peptides and estradiol will fulfill similar roles (Lynch and Wilczynski, 2006; Heinrichs et al., 2009)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Plasma oxytocin levels at early pregnancy and the postpartum period were related to a clearly defined set of maternal bonding behaviors, including gaze, vocalizations, positive affect, and affectionate touch; to attachment-related thoughts; and to frequent checking of the infant.
Abstract: Although research on the neurobiological foundation of social affiliation has implicated the neuropeptide oxytocin in processes of maternal bonding in mammals, there is little evidence to support such links in humans. Plasma oxytocin and cortisol of 62 pregnant women were sampled during the first trimester, last trimester, and first postpartum month. Oxytocin was assayed using enzyme immunoassay, and free cortisol was calculated. After the infants were born, their interactions with their mothers were observed, and the mothers were interviewed regarding their infant-related thoughts and behaviors. Oxytocin was stable across time, and oxytocin levels at early pregnancy and the postpartum period were related to a clearly defined set of maternal bonding behaviors, including gaze, vocalizations, positive affect, and affectionate touch; to attachment-related thoughts; and to frequent checking of the infant. Across pregnancy and the postpartum period, oxytocin may play a role in the emergence of behaviors and mental representations typical of bonding in the human mother.

699 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A working model of affiliation under stress suggests that oxytocin may be a biomarker of social distress that accompanies gaps or problems with social relationships and that may provide an impetus for affiliation.
Abstract: In addition to fight-or-flight, humans demonstrate tending and befriending responses to stress—responses underpinned by the hormone oxytocin, by opioids, and by dopaminergic pathways. A working model of affiliation under stress suggests that oxytocin may be a biomarker of social distress that accompanies gaps or problems with social relationships and that may provide an impetus for affiliation. Oxytocin is implicated in the seeking of affiliative contact in response to stress, and, in conjunction with opioids, it also modulates stress responses. Specifically, in conjunction with positive affiliative contacts, oxytocin attenuates psychological and biological stress responses, but in conjunction with hostile and unsupportive contacts, oxytocin may exacerbate psychological and biological stress responses. Although significant paradoxes remain to be resolved, a mechanism that may underlie oxytocin's relation to the health benefits of social support may be in view.

608 citations


"The Steroid/Peptide Theory of Socia..." refers background in this paper

  • ..., 2010), and has been theorized to be an additional hormonal pathway that responds to stress and promotes the need to affiliate (Taylor, 2006)....

    [...]

  • ...3 We are indebted to Charles Snowdon for this suggestion. et al., 2010), and has been theorized to be an additional hormonal pathway that responds to stress and promotes the need to affiliate (Taylor, 2006)....

    [...]

Frequently Asked Questions (11)
Q1. What are examples of nurturant behavioral contexts?

Actual examples of nurturant behavioral contexts could include grooming, feeding, pair bond existence, huddling, and other close warm contact. 

Given that parent-offspring bonds are likely to be evolutionarily older, pair bonds may be predicated upon a neuroendocrine system that evolved to support parent-offspring bonds but in general promotes nurturance (Fisher, 1992; Carter, 1998; Fernandez-Duque et al., 2009). 

The authors used the S/P Theory to develop a hypothesis about the tricky behavioral context of cuddling, a hypothesis which the authors tested in a follow-up study. 

Like others, the authors define pair bonds as long-lasting affiliations involving intimacy, sexualcontact, preferential proximity, and emotional attachment with relative exclusivity (Hawkes, 2004). 

Actual examples of social bonding could include sexual intimacy, nurturant intimacy, loneliness (i.e., the need for social bonding), social conflict resolution (e.g., arguments with loved ones that arise out a of need to strengthen a bond), etc. Consistent with the above, OT facilitates social cognitions and empathy in humans (Bos et al., 2011). 

They are generally defined by social and sexual “monogamy,” and though extra-pair sexual contacts occur, pair bonds still limit sexual access to others. 

Their example with cuddling highlights the importance of incorporating T into research on intimacies, even though intimacy is typically studied only in conjunction with peptides, and T is only studied in conjunction with competition. 

This reasoning should apply only to partner cuddling, and the authors predict that parent-child cuddling should decrease T as a low T nurturant behavior (unless it is experienced as infant defense, which should accordingly increase T; a testable viable alternative hypothesis). 

Though findings link high OT with pair bonds and partner closeness, other research demonstrates a complementary role for OT, that is, as tied to the need or desire for social bonds. 

Low T is related to nurturance, i.e. social behavioral contexts that involve loving warm contact with others (e.g. partners/mates, offspring, friends, etc.) (and this may possibly transpire via conversation of T to estradiol, and estrogenic facilitation of peptides). 

T is a favorable candidate for testing these distinct evolutionary functions, since it is positively linked to sexuality but negatively linked to nurturance (van Anders and Watson, 2006b; Ziegler et al., 2009), and because it is so strongly implicated in tradeoffs relevant to pair bonding (Wingfield et al., 1990; Ketterson et al., 2005; Bales et al., 2006; van Anders and Watson, 2006b).