scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Tracking eye movements proves informative for the study of gaze direction detection in autism

01 Jul 2009-Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders (Elsevier)-Vol. 3, Iss: 3, pp 723-733
TL;DR: This article found that participants with autism were significantly less accurate at interpreting eye direction and detecting a target item (array sizes 4 and 6 items) than typically developing participants of comparable nonverbal ability.
About: This article is published in Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders.The article was published on 2009-07-01 and is currently open access. It has received 27 citations till now. The article focuses on the topics: Eye tracking & Task analysis.

Summary (1 min read)

Introduction

  • Considerable research effort has been dedicated to exploring how well children with autistic spectrum disorders infer eye gaze direction from the face of an actor.
  • The current research emphasises the value of tracking eye movements for revealing components of task performance in domains known to be of difficulty.
  • Furthermore, even when this ability does develop, it remains severely delayed relative to participants of equivalent mental age (Leekam, Hunnisett, & Moore, 1998).
  • The development of gaze direction detection and gaze perception are likely to follow atypical (and independent) pathways (Webster & Potter, 2008).

Participants

  • All participants had been diagnosed by clinicians and satisfied the diagnostic criteria for autism according to the DSM-IV (APA, 1994).
  • All participants completed a calibration phase at the beginning of the study and all were reported to have normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
  • For typically developing participants teachers completed the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 2001), scoring each individual on emotional symptoms, conduct, hyperactivity, peer relationships and pro-social behaviour.
  • This measure of nonverbal ability is one of the most frequently used matching measures (Mottron, 2004) and provides a quick assessment across a wide age range.

Materials

  • Colour digital photographs were taken of a woman directing her gaze towards one of several targets.
  • In the six-item array, targets were placed on either side at 10˚, 20˚ and 30˚ visual angle from the model’s midline.
  • The system was transported to the testing location of each individual, who sat approximately 50cm from the screen during testing.
  • The eye tracker was calibrated for each participant using a 9 point calibration of each eye.
  • The Tobii system recorded gaze behaviour to pre-specified areas of interest (AOI).

Autism and Developmental Disorders.

  • Scan Path Differences and Similarities During Emotion Perception in those With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorders.
  • Eye Direction Detection Improves with Development in Autism, also known as Brief Report.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is argued that VCs and environments are a valuable assistive, educational and therapeutic tool for HFA by enabling an ecologically valid experience of social presence, and by providing an experimental platform that can be systematically and fully controlled.
Abstract: High-functioning autism (HFA) is a neurodevelopmental disorder, which is characterized by life-long socio-communicative impairments on the one hand and preserved verbal and general learning and memory abilities on the other. One of the areas where particular difficulties are observable is the understanding of non-verbal communication cues.Thus, investigating the underlying psychological processes and neural mechanisms of non-verbal communication in HFA allows a better understanding of this disorder, and potentially enables the development of more efficient forms of psychotherapy and trainings. However, the research on non-verbal information processing in HFA faces several methodological challenges. The use of virtual characters (VCs) helps to overcome such challenges by enabling an ecologically valid experience of social presence, and by providing an experimental platform that can be systematically and fully controlled. To make this field of research accessible to a broader audience, we elaborate in the first part of the review the validity of using VCs in non-verbal behavior research on HFA, and we review current relevant paradigms and findings from social-cognitive neuroscience. In the second part, we argue for the use of VCs as either agents or avatars in the context of “transformed social interactions.” This allows for the implementation of real-time social interaction in virtual experimental settings, which represents a more sensitive measure of socio-communicative impairments in HFA. Finally, we argue that VCs and environments are a valuable assistive, educational and therapeutic tool for HFA.

92 citations


Cites background from "Tracking eye movements proves infor..."

  • ...In concordance with the literature, participants with HFA spent less time on the eye region compared to typically developing individuals (e.g., Pelphrey et al., 2002; Rutherford and Towns, 2008; Riby and Doherty, 2009; Nakano et al., 2010; Falkmer et al., 2011)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Eye tracking was used to explore how individuals with WS and autism attended to, and subsequently interpreted, an actor’s eye gaze cue within a social scene, and the atypicalities found are likely to be entwined with the deficits shown in interpreting social cognitive cues from the images.
Abstract: Background: From a young age the typical development of social functioning relies upon the allocation of attention to socially relevant information, which in turn allows experience at processing such information and thus enhances social cognition. As such, research has attempted to identify the developmental processes that are derailed in some neuro-developmental disorders that impact upon social functioning. Williams syndrome (WS) and autism are disorders of development that are characterized by atypical yet divergent social phenotypes and atypicalities of attention to people. Methods: We used eye tracking to explore how individuals with WS and autism attended to, and subsequently interpreted, an actor’s eye gaze cue within a social scene. Images were presented for 3 seconds, initially with an instruction simply to look at the picture. The images were then shown again, with the participant asked to identify the object being looked at. Allocation of eye gaze in each condition was analyzed by analysis of variance and accuracy of identification was compared with t tests. Results: Participants with WS allocated more gaze time to face and eyes than their matched controls, both with and without being asked to identify the item being looked at; while participants with autism spent less time on face and eyes in both conditions. When cued to follow gaze, participants with WS increased gaze to the correct targets; those with autism looked more at the face and eyes but did not increase gaze to the correct targets, while continuing to look much more than their controls at implausible targets. Both groups identified fewer objects than their controls. Conclusions: The atypicalities found are likely to be entwined with the deficits shown in interpreting social cognitive cues from the images. WS and autism are characterized by atypicalities of social attention that impact upon socio-cognitive expertise, but, importantly, the type of atypicality is syndrome specific.

81 citations


Cites background from "Tracking eye movements proves infor..."

  • ...Research involving individuals on the autism spectrum has used eye tracking to explore emotion recognition ability [31-33], the effect of face-familiarity on face perception [34] and eye direction detection within a basic gaze-cueing paradigm [35]....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results show that the mechanisms coding gaze are less flexible in autism and offer a potential new explanation for these children's difficulties discriminating subtle deviations in gaze direction.

70 citations


Cites background from "Tracking eye movements proves infor..."

  • ...Similarly, school-age children, but not adolescents, with autism showed difficulties detecting the target of another's gaze when targets were separated by 51 and 101 (Webster and Potter, 2008; see also Riby & Doherty, 2009)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Findings suggest older adults do not use eye-gaze cues to engage in joint attention, and have specific social difficulties decoding critical information from the eye region.
Abstract: BACKGROUND Eye-gaze following is a fundamental social skill, facilitating communication. The present series of studies explored adult age-related differences in this key social-cognitive ability. METHOD In Study 1 younger and older adult participants completed a cueing task in which eye-gaze cues were predictive or non-predictive of target location. Another eye-gaze cueing task, assessing the influence of congruent and incongruent eye-gaze cues relative to trials which provided no cue to target location, was administered in Study 2. Finally, in Study 3 the eye-gaze cue was replaced by an arrow. RESULTS In Study 1 older adults showed less evidence of gaze following than younger participants when required to strategically follow predictive eye-gaze cues and when making automatic shifts of attention to non-predictive eye-gaze cues. Findings from Study 2 suggested that, unlike younger adults, older participants showed no facilitation effect and thus did not follow congruent eye-gaze cues. They also had significantly weaker attentional costs than their younger counterparts. These age-related differences were not found in the non-social arrow cueing task. DISCUSSION Taken together these findings suggest older adults do not use eye-gaze cues to engage in joint attention, and have specific social difficulties decoding critical information from the eye region.

33 citations


Cites background from "Tracking eye movements proves infor..."

  • ...…is the main way of engaging in joint attention with others and therefore this ability is important for facilitating social interactions and communication with others (see Langton, Watt, & Bruce, 2000; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Riby & Doherty, 2009; Sigman & Ruskin, 1999; Stone & Yoder, 2001)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, an eye-tracking method was used to verify the restorative effect of natural objects using eye tracking methods and a survey regarding visual aesthetics, complexity, and the perceived restorativeness scale.
Abstract: Attention Restoration Theory argues that natural objects such as trees and flowers have psychological restoration effects. However, relevant studies have been mostly based on survey methods, and few of them suggest guidelines for restoration environments. This study, therefore, aims to verify the restorative effect of natural objects using eye-tracking methods and a survey regarding visual aesthetics, complexity, and the Perceived Restorativeness Scale, as well 25 various images divided into 4 types: natural scene and close view, natural scene and distant view, built scene and close view, and built scene and distant view. The analysis showed that natural scenes had a stronger positive restorative effect compared to built scenes regardless of differences in the distance. In terms of the overall landscape composition, visual characteristics such as visual aesthetics and complexity had a statistically significant relationship with restorative effect. Additionally, an eye-tracking method was found to be a valid and useful tool for studying the restorative environments by significant differences in the scan path length depending on the four types of landscape images. This study ultimately provides an overview regarding restorative design guidelines not only by using natural elements but also by considering landscape composition in terms of complexity, openness, and so on.

28 citations

References
More filters
Reference EntryDOI
11 Jun 2013

113,134 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The reliability and validity of the SDQ make it a useful brief measure of the adjustment and psychopathology of children and adolescents.
Abstract: Objective To describe the psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), a brief measure of the prosocial behavior and psychopathology of 3–16-year-olds that can be completed by parents, teachers, or youths. Method A nationwide epidemiological sample of 10,438 British 5–15-year-olds obtained SDQs from 96% of parents, 70% of teachers, and 91% of 11–15-year-olds. Blind to the SDQ findings, all subjects were also assigned DSM-IV diagnoses based on a clinical review of detailed interview measures. Results The predicted five-factor structure (emotional, conduct, hyperactivity-inattention, peer, prosocial) was confirmed. Internalizing and externalizing scales were relatively "uncontaminated" by one another. Reliability was generally satisfactory, whether judged by internal consistency (mean Cronbach α: .73), cross-informant correlation (mean: 0.34), or retest stability after 4 to 6 months (mean: 0.62). SDQ scores above the 90th percentile predicted a substantially raised probability of independently diagnosed psychiatric disorders (mean odds ratio: 15.7 for parent scales, 15.2 for teacher scales, 6.2 for youth scales). Conclusion The reliability and validity of the SDQ make it a useful brief measure of the adjustment and psychopathology of children and adolescents.

5,618 citations


"Tracking eye movements proves infor..." refers background in this paper

  • ...The data have implications for the design of tasks for individuals with autism and provide insights into the usefulness of including measures of visual attention in understanding task performance....

    [...]

  • ...For typically developing participants teachers completed the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 2001), scoring each individual on emotional symptoms, conduct, hyperactivity, peer relationships and pro-social behaviour....

    [...]

Book
24 Feb 1995
TL;DR: The four steps autism and mindblindness how brains read minds the language of the eyes mindreading - back to the future was discussed in evolutionary psychology and social chess mindreading as discussed by the authors.
Abstract: Mindblindness and mindreading evolutionary psychology and social chess mindreading - nature's choice developing mindreading - the four steps autism and mindblindness how brains read minds the language of the eyes mindreading - back to the future.

4,255 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A novel method of quantifying atypical strategies of social monitoring in a setting that simulates the demands of daily experience is reported, finding that fixation times on mouths and objects but not on eyes are strong predictors of degree of social competence.
Abstract: Background: Manifestations of core social deficits in autism are more pronounced in everyday settings than in explicit experimental tasks. To bring experimental measures in line with clinical observation, we report a novel method of quantifying atypical strategies of social monitoring in a setting that simulates the demands of daily experience. Enhanced ecological validity was intended to maximize between-group effect sizes and assess the predictive utility of experimental variables relative to outcome measures of social competence. Methods: While viewing social scenes, eye-tracking technology measured visual fixations in 15 cognitively able males with autism and 15 age-, sex-, and verbal IQ– matched control subjects. We reliably coded fixations on 4 regions: mouth, eyes, body, and objects. Statistical analyses compared fixation time on regions of interest between groups and correlation of fixation time with outcome measures of social competence (ie, standardized measures of daily social adjustment and degree of autistic social symptoms). Results: Significant between-group differences were obtained for all 4 regions. The best predictor of autism was reduced eye region fixation time. Fixation on mouths and objects was significantly correlated with social functioning: increased focus on mouths predicted improved social adjustment and less autistic social impairment, whereas more time on objects predicted the opposite relationship. Conclusions: When viewing naturalistic social situations, individuals with autism demonstrate abnormal patterns of social visual pursuit consistent with reduced salience of eyes and increased salience of mouths, bodies, and objects. Fixation times on mouths and objects but not on eyes are strong predictors of degree of social competence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:809-816

1,893 citations

Book
01 Jan 1987
TL;DR: In this article, a sibling's perspective on the diagnosis of Rett's syndrome is presented. But the authors focus on the development and social development of individuals with autism and do not address the social aspects of the disorder.
Abstract: Partial table of contents: DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION Rett's Syndrome: A Pervasive Developmental Disorder (R Van Acker) DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR Social Development in Autism (F Volkmar et al) Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood (W Stone) NEUROBIOLOGY Neurological Aspects of Autism (N Minshew, et al) ASSESSMENT Behavioral Assessment of Individuals with Autism (M Powers) INTERVENTIONS Behavioral Interventions (J Bregman & J Gerdtz) Working with Families (L Marcus, et al) Routine Health Care (R Lacamera & A Lacamera) PUBLIC POLICY PERSPECTIVES Coping with the Diagnosis of Autism (B Siegel) Ethical Issues in Research and Treatment (A Klin & D Cohen) THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Classification and Causal Issues in Autism (I Rapin) Perspectives on Social Impairment (L Waterhouse & D Fein) INTERNATIONAL PERSPECTIVES Canada (P Szatmari) Spain (J Fuentes) PERSONAL PERSPECTIVES A Sibling's Perspective on Autism (J Konidaris) Indexes

1,783 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (6)
Q1. What are the contributions in this paper?

Here the authors combine task performance ( accuracy to correctly label a target item ) and eye movement information ( ‘ where ’ the participant fixates when completing the task ) to understand more about the components involved in completing eye direction detection tasks. The data have implications for the design of tasks for individuals with autism and provide insights into the usefulness of including measures of visual attention in understanding task performance. RUNNING HEAD: eye-tracking, gaze direction & autism Tracking eye movements proves informative for the study of gaze direction detection in autism Autism is a neuro-developmental disorder characterized by qualitative impairments of social communication, accompanied by unusual repetitive or stereotyped behaviours ( DSM IV, American Psychiatric Association, 1994 ). Clifford and Dissanayake ( 2008 ) studied home videos of infants later diagnosed with autism, identifying poor quality and timing of eye contact even during the first year of life. The development of gaze direction detection and gaze perception are likely to follow atypical ( and independent ) pathways ( Webster & Potter, 2008 ). This suggestion is supported by research showing more pronounced problems for younger children with the disorder, indicating an unusually protracted development ( Webster & Potter, 2008 ). With an abundance of research revealing deficits in the domain of social cognition and attention in autism there have been calls for a shift away from characterising overall task performance and towards the study of the processes and strategies used to perform these tasks ( Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, & Cohen 2002b ; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schutlz, & Klin, 2004 ; Boraston & Blakemore, 2007 ). Information about which aspects of the stimuli the participant fixates upon can provide insights into the strategies they are using. The current study extends the combined use of eye-tracking and behavioural performance to the study of gaze direction detection, a domain that is regularly implicated in explorations of autism. In typical development the evidence therefore suggests that explicit judgement of eye-direction is a skill that arises by approximately 3-years of age and continues to gradually improve in precision during early childhood. Furthermore, even when this ability does develop, it remains severely delayed relative to participants of equivalent mental age ( Leekam, Hunnisett, & Moore, 1998 ). Evidence supporting this suggestion comes from apparent dissociations between the ability to identify where someone is looking ( Leekam et al., 1997 ) and a lack of spontaneous gaze monitoring ( e. g. Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman, 1986 ) which prevents triadic interactions. Boraston and Blakemore ( 2007 ) suggest that eye tracking is particularly useful when it is combined with tests of cognitive performance because it provides information in addition to a participant ’ s overall test score. 

Five attended the special education unit of a mainstream secondary school and 10 attended schools for pupils with additional educational needs. 

The Tobii 1750 eye-tracking screen was interfaced and controlled by a Dell Latitude D820 computer for presenting stimuli and recording responses (using Tobii Studio software). 

Each individual with autism was matched to a typically developing child (TD) on the basis of nonverbal ability (11 males and 4 females, mean chronological age 6 years 10 months, ranging 5 years 1 month to 8 years 3 months). 

To comply with their inclusion criteria, all participants scored within the autistic range, with CARS scores ranging between 33-50. 

However when the task gets harder (6 items for typically developing participants) participants look at the target for less time when they respond incorrectly.