scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Posted ContentDOI

Wing-pattern-specific effects of experience on mating behavior in Heliconius melpomene butterflies

17 Jul 2020-bioRxiv (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory)-
TL;DR: The results suggest that social experience can influence male mating behavior in H. melpomene and that behavioral plasticity may differ across populations in this species.
Abstract: Many animals have the ability to learn, and some taxa have shown learned mate preference. This learning may be important for speciation in some species. The butterfly Heliconius melpomene is a model system for several areas of research, including hybridization, mate selection, and speciation, partially due to its widespread diversity of wing patterns. It remains unclear whether these butterflies can learn to prefer certain mates and if social experience shapes realized mating preferences. Here we test whether previous experience with a female influences male mate preference for two different H. melpomene subspecies, H. m. malleti and H. m. rosina. We conducted no-choice behavioral assays to determine if latency to court and whether males courted (vs no courtship) differed between naive males and males with previous exposure to a young, sexually mature, virgin female. To test whether assortative courtship preference is learned in H. melpomene, males were either paired with a female who shared their phenotype or one who did not. Naive H. m. malletti males courted assortatively, while naive H. m. rosina males did not. Experienced H. m. malleti males reduced their courting relative to naive males, suggesting that social experience with a sexually mature female that does not result in copulation may be perceived as a negative experience. In contrast, experienced H. m. rosina males exhibited similar courting rates to naive H. m. rosina males. Our results suggest that social experience can influence male mating behavior in H. melpomene and that behavioral plasticity may differ across populations in this species.

Summary (2 min read)

Introduction

  • Many of the behaviors and decisions that an animal makes are affected by its observations and capacity to learn.
  • Learning in animals is often complex and is likely the result of the social dynamics and settings of a species (Coussi-Korbel and Fragaszy, 1995) .
  • Previous studies show that mimetic coloration in this species is important in choosing mates, and that these butterflies show assortative mating when choosing between their own and a different, closely related species (Heliconius cydno) (Jiggins et al., 2001) .
  • 3) If males are not able to learn, then courting was predicted to occur at random in both experienced and naïve males.

Study species and husbandry

  • Heliconius melpomene is a widespread neotropical butterfly found in Central and South America (Brower, 1994; Sheppard et al., 1985) .
  • The species is well known for its high diversity in color patterns, which play an important role in speciation (Jiggins et al., 2004) .
  • Caterpillars from the colony were given Passiflora plants ad libitum, and prior to pupation, plants containing caterpillars were removed from the breeding cages and moved to a separate 60.96 x 60.96 x 142.24 cm cage until butterfly emergence from pupa.
  • The greenhouse was lit by Sun Blaze T5 high output 120-volt fluorescent light fixtures (containing UV wavelengths), in addition to natural sunlight, and the presence of UV light in the greenhouse was confirmed using an Ocean Optics Jaz spectrometer.
  • Males were placed into sex-and phenotype-specific cages, so they were isolated from both females and other wing patterns prior to behavioral assays.

Observational Experiment Time of Day Selection

  • To determine the time of day when the butterflies were the most active, the authors observed butterflies in colony cages for three consecutive days, between 6:00 am and 8:00 pm.
  • Point counts were conducted every thirty minutes, where behaviors (flight, walk, flutter, abdomen lift, bask [defined by resting with wings held in open position], rest [defined by resting with wings held in closed position], antennae wiggle, court, and copulate) were recorded for each cage, followed by two ten-minute focal watches of one male and one female butterfly selected at random.
  • Based on observations, the authors determined that butterflies were most active between the hours of 10:00 am to 2:00 pm.

Behavioral Watches

  • All behavioral watches took place between 10:00 am and 2:00 pm, the time of peak H. melpomene activity in their greenhouse.
  • Each watch consisted of a male aged ten or twelve days old, and a female between three and five days old.
  • To test whether males courted females with matching wing patterns faster than they courted conspecific females with dissimilar wing patterns, the authors tested latency to courtship and presence of courtship of naïve, 12-day-old H.m. malleti and H.m. rosina males matched with either females of their own phenotype or females of different phenotypes.
  • Afterward, the female was removed, and the male was returned to the all-male, phenotype-specific cage.
  • The number of incidents of each type of behavior (flight, walk, flutter, abdomen lift, bask (wings open), rest (wings closed), antenna wiggle, sitting near, and court) were recorded.

Statistical Analyses

  • All statistical analyses were performed in JMP v. 14 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, U.S.A.).
  • The authors assessed whether latency to court was influenced by male experience or female wing pattern (similar or different from the male's) using a GLM with male experience and female wing pattern as factors, as well as an interaction term.
  • To test whether female behavior during a male's first experience with a female had an effect on the observed courtship behavior in later interactions with females, the authors analyzed all behavioral data collected on day 10 watches (N=51 watches with behavioral data) and examined whether any of these behaviors were predictive of male courting on day 12.
  • To do this the authors ran a principal components analysis on all the female behaviors and then ran logistic regression models on the first three principal components.

Ethical Note

  • All H. melpomene butterflies were kept under laboratory conditions as defined by U.S. Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service permit P526P-17-00343.
  • Before being used in behavioral watches all butterflies were maintained in cages in a climatecontrolled setting in conditions similar to those of their native habitat, and cages were inspected daily for ample food and appropriate conditions.

Discussion

  • The authors results show that male H. melpomene butterflies change their mating behavior in response to a social experience.
  • The negative effect of the pre-mating social exposure, and the wing-pattern-specific response to this pre-mating social exposure, were unexpected.
  • These males are then tested repeatedly, and past experience is often not accounted for when male preference is assessed, assuming that past experience does not inform present courting decisions.
  • It also highlights the importance of checking for both positive and negative valence when testing the presence of learning.

Conclusion

  • Here the authors show that male H. melpomene butterflies use past social experience to inform current mating behavior.
  • This response is lineage (wing pattern) specific, and coincides with lineagespecific differences in male assortative preference.
  • The authors findings strongly suggest that there are lineage-specific selective forces acting on cognitive function in Heliconius butterflies.
  • Lineage-specific effect of experience on male courtship.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

1
Wing-pattern-specific effects of experience on mating behavior in Heliconius
1
melpomene butterflies
2
Peyton A. Rather*, Abigail E. Herzog*, David A. Ernst, Erica L. Westerman
3
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR 72701
4
*
equal contributors
5
6
7
Abstract
8
Many animals have the ability to learn, and some taxa have shown learned mate preference. This
9
learning may be important for speciation in some species. The butterfly Heliconius melpomene is
10
a model system for several areas of research, including hybridization, mate selection, and
11
speciation, partially due to its widespread diversity of wing patterns. It remains unclear whether
12
these butterflies can learn to prefer certain mates and if social experience shapes realized mating
13
preferences. Here we test whether previous experience with a female influences male mate
14
preference for two different H. melpomene subspecies, H. m. malleti and H. m. rosina. We
15
conducted no-choice behavioral assays to determine if latency to court and whether males
16
courted (vs no courtship) differed between naïve males and males with previous exposure to a
17
young, sexually mature, virgin female. To test whether assortative courtship preference is
18
learned in H. melpomene, males were either paired with a female who shared their phenotype or
19
one who did not. Naïve H. m. malletti males courted assortatively, while naïve H.m. rosina males
20
did not. Experienced H. m. malleti males reduced their courting relative to naïve males,
21
suggesting that social experience with a sexually mature female that does not result in copulation
22
may be perceived as a negative experience. In contrast, experienced H. m. rosina males exhibited
23
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 17, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.205435doi: bioRxiv preprint

2
similar courting rates to naïve H. m. rosina males. Our results suggest that social experience can
24
influence male mating behavior in H. melpomene and that behavioral plasticity may differ across
25
populations in this species.
26
27
Keywords
28
behavioral plasticity, mate choice, Lepidoptera, assortative mating, social learning, male choice
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 17, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.205435doi: bioRxiv preprint

3
Introduction
47
48
Many of the behaviors and decisions that an animal makes are affected by its observations and
49
capacity to learn. Learning can be defined as a set of processes that allows an animal to acquire,
50
store, and use information gathered from the environment (Galef and Laland, 2005). Learning in
51
animals is often complex and is likely the result of the social dynamics and settings of a species
52
(Coussi-Korbel and Fragaszy, 1995). There is a substantial amount of evidence that animals have
53
the ability to socially learn (Dukas, 1998). Some of the many behaviors that might be the result
54
of social learning include food choices, predator avoidance, and mate preferences. For example,
55
many species of fish have been observed to learn how to find food, how to recognize predators,
56
and how to assess mate quality (Brown and Laland, 2003). This breadth of learning ability,
57
however, is not limited to vertebrates (Dukas, 2008; Verzijden et al., 2012).
58
59
It is now understood that learning affects many essential activities of invertebrates, including
60
predator avoidance and social interactions (Dukas, 2008, 2010). Particularly, many insects and
61
spiders have shown the ability to learn mate preference. Studies on the wolf spider Schizocosa
62
uetzi have shown that female social experience in their penultimate juvenile period can affect
63
their mate choices as adults (Hebets, 2003). Female Teleogryllus oceanicus crickets modify
64
their mate preferences after hearing attractive male songs (Bailey and Zuk, 2009), and female
65
Bicyclus anynana butterflies learn preferences for enhanced male ornaments (Westerman et al.,
66
2012). Male B. anynana also learn preferences for wing pattern elements in females (Westerman
67
et al., 2014). Furthermore, work with Drosophila melanogaster fruit flies have shown that
68
learning to be selective leads to a higher lifetime mating success than males who court
69
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 17, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.205435doi: bioRxiv preprint

4
indiscriminately (Dukas et al., 2006). Therefore, when it comes to mate preference and sexual
70
behavior in insects it is often beneficial to learn.
71
72
Learning can potentially increase rates of assortative mating, which can lead to speciation
73
through processes such as when young animals imprint on parents (Dukas, 2013). One such
74
example of this is how cross
fostering experiments in two subspecies of zebra finch
75
demonstrated that assortative mating is due to imprinting. Birds in this study paired with mates
76
that resembled their foster parents instead of their own phenotype (Irwin and Price, 1999). It has
77
also been shown that mate preference can be learned in mature animals, such as male guppies
78
and Syrian hamsters. These animals have demonstrated learning to discriminate against
79
heterospecific mates after courtship interactions (Verzijden et al., 2012). This type of learning
80
would help maintain speciation. With these studies in mind, we might expect that Heliconius
81
butterflies, or other animals with high levels of speciation, might learn to court assortatively.
82
83
Heliconius butterflies have a long lifespan compared to other species of butterflies, which allows
84
them to potentially mate multiple times (Gilbert, 1972). Therefore, the ability to learn in
85
response to mating experiences could be advantageous. Studies have shown that male mate
86
preferences evolve early in the speciation process in Heliconius within both intraspecific hybrid
87
mating zones and conspecific polymorphic populations (Merrill et al., 2011a). These male mate
88
preferences are based on wing color pattern cues, which are under natural selection to correspond
89
to local mimetic environments (Gray and McKinnon, 2007; Kronforst et al., 2006). Heliconius is
90
well known for its diversity in color patterns, and divergence in these color morphs is associated
91
with speciation and adaptive radiation (Heliconius-Genome-Consortium* et al., 2012).
92
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 17, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.205435doi: bioRxiv preprint

5
93
Here we take advantage of the social butterfly species Heliconius melpomene, whose widespread
94
diversity of color patterns makes it an ideal model for studies on speciation and mating patterns
95
(Jiggins et al., 2004). In this species, mimetic color patterns play a key role in species
96
recognition, and mate preferences based on these patterns evolve alongside changes in wing
97
pattern (Jiggins et al., 2004). Previous studies show that mimetic coloration in this species is
98
important in choosing mates, and that these butterflies show assortative mating when choosing
99
between their own and a different, closely related species (Heliconius cydno) (Jiggins et al.,
100
2001). Furthermore, males often do discriminate between conspecific females with different
101
wing patterns, and do not copy the mate preferences of conspecific males who have different
102
wing patterns (Jiggins et al., 2004). However, it remains unclear whether individual H.
103
melpomene males use past social experience with sexually receptive (or non-receptive) females
104
to inform current mating decisions. The ability to learn mate preferences for intraspecies
105
variation in wing pattern may be important for the initiation of assortative mating, reproductive
106
isolation, and the speciation process.
107
108
Here we test whether experience impacts future male mate preference and courting behavior in
109
two races of H. melpomene using three distinct H. melpomene color morph phenotypes (Figure
110
1). We had three alternative hypotheses: 1) If males learn, then we predicted that experienced
111
males would be more likely to court and have a shorter latency to court relative to naïve males.
112
This type of learning is seen in B. anynana, where males exposed to dorsal hindwing spot
113
number variation learn preferences for this trait (Westerman et al., 2014). 2) If however male
114
exposure to a female is somehow a negative experience, then we predicted that experienced
115
.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International licenseavailable under a
was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made
The copyright holder for this preprint (whichthis version posted July 17, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.205435doi: bioRxiv preprint

References
More filters
Book
01 Jan 1998
TL;DR: This study examines how the environment shapes the way an animal processes information and makes decisions, and how constraints imposed on nervous systems affect an animal's activities.
Abstract: This study examines how the environment shapes the way an animal processes information and makes decisions, and how constraints imposed on nervous systems affect an animal's activities. To help answer these questions, this text integrates evolutionary ecology and cognitive science, demonstrating how studies of perception, memory and learning can deepen understanding of animal behaviour and ecology. The book considers such issues as: the evolution of learning and its influence on behaviour; the effects of cognitive mechanisms on the evolution of signalling behaviour; how neurobiological and evolutionary processes have shaped navigational activities; functional and mechanical explanations for altered behaviours in response to changing environments; how foragers make decisions and how these decisions are influenced by the risk of predation; and how cognitive mechanisms affect partner choice. The study should encourage biologists to consider how animal cognition affects behaviour, and should also interest comparative psychologists and cognitive scientists.

293 citations


"Wing-pattern-specific effects of ex..." refers background in this paper

  • ...There is a substantial amount of evidence that animals have the ability to socially learn (Dukas, 1998)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: By hybridizing races of Heliconius melpomene and races of H. erato, it is shown that, as expected from the two-step theory, the races differ at a number of genetic loci, usually unlinked or loosely linked, including at least one mutant of major effect in each case.
Abstract: A protected and warningly coloured butterfly can become a muellerian mimic of another species in two steps: (i) a major mutation converts the pattern of the less protected species to an approximate resemblance of the better protected (one-way convergence); (ii) after the spread of this mutant, the species, which now resemble each other sufficiently to be mistaken one for the other by predators, undergo mutual convergence, using whatever major or minor genetic variation is available to them. Although sometimes one or other step might occur alone, in general early theorists were mistaken in attributing muellerian mimicry to only one of these processes. By hybridizing races of Heliconius melpomene and races of H. erato (a pair of parallel mimetic species from the neotropics, held in mutual muellerian mimicry across wide inter-racial variations in colour pattern) we have shown that, as expected from the two-step theory, the races differ at a number (two to nine) of genetic loci, usually unlinked or loosely linked, including at least one mutant of major effect in each case. We describe the genetic constitution of eight races of H. melpomene (for 11 loci affecting colour pattern) and of eight races of H. erato (for up to 15 loci), and have started to identify the linkage groups. Map distances for those loci that are linked range from around 0.3 to zero in males, with no recombination in females. Muellerian mimicry is expected to produce total uniformity of pattern: universal exceptions to this are the existence of distinct mimicry rings flying within the same habitat, geographical variation within nearly all the more widespread species (divergence in the face of normalizing selection), and, in a few species, polymorphism or sexual dimorphism. Sympatric mimicry rings will, according to the two-step model of evolution, persist indefinitely if their patterns are so distinct that under no circumstances do predators mistake one for the other. Gradual mutual convergence is then impossible, although members of a weakly protected mimicry ring that can produce a mutation giving sufficient initial resemblance to a better protected ring can still be captured by it. Batesian mimics promote this by lowering the protection of the ring that they belong to, but their models can escape only in this way as normalizing selection prevents their gradual evolution away from the batesian mimic. If the rings are too distinct in pattern even this capture of species becomes impossible as no single mutant is able to bridge the gap between the two patterns, and the necessary two mutations will be extremely unlikely to occur together. The five principal sympatric mimicry rings of the mature neotropical rain forests are very distinct in their appearance. The capture of a species by another ring can produce geographical variation both in the species captured and in the capturing ring, whose pattern is somewhat altered by mutual convergence with the captured species in the second step of the evolution of the muellerian resemblance. We suggest that the striking differences between the races within H. melpomene, H. erato and other Heliconius species resulted from these effects of inter-ring capture. Distributional evidence suggests that this chiefly occurred in refuges formed by the contraction of the neotropical rain forests during the cool dry periods in the Quaternary; these, by differential extinction of elements of the flora and fauna of different refuges, could have produced long-term changes in the relative abundances of the mimicry rings, and hence (as the protection given to a ring is proportional to its abundance) somewhat different capture events in each refuge. Several existing species confirm that this mode of evolution occurs, by retaining a distinctive pattern in the absence of any other remotely similar species, but becoming mimetic in areas where they encounter a pattern somewhat like their own. The isolated populations of Heliconius hermathena show this particularly clearly; the effect can be discerned also in H. melpomene and H. erato. Although polymorphism in muellerian mimics is largely unexplained, in two species of Heliconius it may result from the existence of two or more similar but slightly differing 9sub-rings9 among their comimics in the family Ithomiidae, which show both spatial and temporal heterogeneity in their local distribution, which apparently is able to maintain a polymorphic equilibrium in the more uniformly distributed Heliconius. We have tentatively reconstructed the ancestral patterns of H. melpomene and erato by two independent methods: first, as dominant genes are much more likely to be incorporated than recessive ones during changes of pattern, the phenotype produced by the recessive alleles at all the known loci will be close to the ancestral pattern; secondly, species that are becoming mimics evolve more than those that are not, so that non-mimetic relatives of melpomene and erato will have a pattern close to ancestral. Both methods suggest, for both species, that the ancestor was a black butterfly with yellow (or possibly white) bars, and it may be that melpomene and erato have been comimics for a very long time. Previous climatic cycles in the Quaternary have apparently caused full speciation within two mutually mimetic evolving lineages, producing pairs of parallel mimetic species within the genus, of which melpomene and erato constitute one pair.

285 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is demonstrated that the mating preferences of female wolf spiders can be acquired through exposure as subadults to unrelated, sexually active adult males, and a previously uncharacterized mechanism influencing the origin of female mating preferences and ultimately the evolution of male traits is suggested.
Abstract: Current sexual selection theory proposes several potential mechanisms driving the evolution of female mating preferences, few of which involve social interactions. Although vertebrate examples of socially influenced mating preferences do exist, the invertebrate examples are virtually nonexistent. Here I demonstrate that the mating preferences of female wolf spiders can be acquired through exposure as subadults to unrelated, sexually active adult males. I first conducted exposure trials during which subadult females of the wolf spider Schizocosa uetzi were allowed to interact with mature males of an experimentally manipulated phenotype (either black or brown forelegs). After maturation, these previously exposed females were paired with a male of either a familiar or unfamiliar manipulated phenotype for mate-choice trials. Subadult females that were exposed to directed courtship by mature males of a particular morphological phenotype were subsequently more likely to mate with a male of a familiar phenotype as adults. Furthermore, females that were exposed as subadults were more likely, as adults, to cannibalize a courting male with an unfamiliar phenotype. Unexposed females did not distinguish between phenotypes in either mate choice or cannibalism frequency. These results suggest a previously uncharacterized mechanism influencing the origin of female mating preferences and ultimately the evolution of male traits: subadult experience. This study also stresses the potential importance of learning and memory on adult mate choice in an arthropod.

263 citations


"Wing-pattern-specific effects of ex..." refers background in this paper

  • ...Studies on the wolf spider Schizocosa uetzi have shown that female social experience in their penultimate juvenile period can affect their mate choices as adults (Hebets, 2003)....

    [...]

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, Cytochrome Oxidase I and II sequences from multiple, parallel races of Heliconius erato and H. melpomene are examined, to estimate intraspecific phylogeny and gauge sequence divergence and ages of clades among races within each species.
Abstract: Mimicry has been a fundamental focus of research since the birth of evolutionary biology yet rarely has been studied from a phylogenetic perspective beyond the simple recognition that mimics are not similar due to common descent. The difficulty of finding characters to discern relationships among closely related and convergent taxa has challenged systematists for more than a century. The phenotypic diversity of wing pattens among mimetic Heliconius adds an additional twist to the problem, because single species contain more than a dozen radically different-looking geographical races even though the mimetic advantage is theoretically highest when all individuals within and between species appear the same. Mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) offers an independent way to address these issues. In this study, Cytochrome Oxidase I and II sequences from multiple, parallel races of Heliconius erato and Heliconius melpomene are examined, to estimate intraspecific phylogeny and gauge sequence divergence and ages of clades among races within each species. Although phenotypes of sympatric races exhibit remarkable concordance between the two species, the mitochondrial cladograms show that the species have not shared a common evolutionary history. H. erato exhibits a basal split between trans- and cis-Andean groups of races, whereas H. melpomene originates in the Guiana Shield. Diverse races in either species appear to have evolved within the last 200,000 yr, and convergent phenotypes have evolved independently within as well as between species. These results contradict prior theories of the evolution of mimicry based on analysis of wing-pattern genetics.

218 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is suggested that species from the melpomene -group of Heliconius have radiated to occupy mimetic niches protected by model species in the Ithomiinae and the erato -groups ofHeliconius, suggesting the maintenance of mimetic diversity would be aided by the habitat and behavioural differences revealed here.
Abstract: In the new world tropics there is an extravagant array of sympatric butterfly mimicry rings. This is puzzling under strictly coevolutionary (Mullerian) mimicry: all unpalatable species should converge as ‘co-mimics’ to the same pattern. If mimicry has usually evolved in unpalatable species by one-sided (Batesian) evolution, however, it is easy to see that mimicry rings centred on different models could remain distinct. If mimicry rings were also segregated by habitat, a diversity of mimicry rings could be stabilized. In this paper we report correlations between behaviour and mimicry of nine unpalatable Heliconius species. It is already known that co-mimics fly in similar habitats, and non-mimics fly in different habitats, although there is much overlap. Contrary to a previous report, we find little difference in flight heights of heliconiine mimicry rings; all species fly from ground level to the canopy. However, co-mimics roost at night in similar habitats and at similar heights above the ground, but in different habitats and at different heights from species in other mimicry rings. Heliconius (especially the erato taxonomic group) are renowned for roosting gregariously; and co-mimics roost gregariously with each other more often than with non-mimics. Gregarious roosting is therefore common between species, as well as within species. There are thus strong links between mimicry and behavioural ecology in Heliconius. The paradoxical correlation between nocturnal roosting and visual mimicry is presumably explained by bird predation at dusk when roosts are forming, or at dawn before they have disbanded. Direct evidence of predation is lacking, but there are high rates of disturbance by birds at these times. These results, together with knowledge of the phylogeny of Heliconius, suggest that species from the melpomene -group of Heliconius have radiated to occupy mimetic niches protected by model species in the Ithomiinae and the erato -group of Heliconius. A variety of sympatric mimicry rings is apparently maintained because key models fail to converge, while more rapidly-evolving unpalatable mimics evolve towards the colour patterns of the models. The maintenance of mimetic diversity would be aided by the habitat and behavioural differences between mimicry rings revealed here, provided that different predators are found in different habitats. This explanation for the maintenance of multiple mimicry rings is more plausible for Heliconius mimicry than alternatives based on visual mating constraints, thermal ecology, or camouflage.

211 citations


"Wing-pattern-specific effects of ex..." refers background in this paper

  • ...H. melpomene butterflies are relatively long-lived (up to 6 months in nature) (Gilbert, 1972), highly social (they roost in groups at night) (Mallet and Gilbert, 1995), and learn food sources and color cues (Toure et al., 2020)....

    [...]