scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Journal ArticleDOI

Women's Luxury Products as Signals to Other Women

21 Dec 2020-Vol. 4, pp 227-238
TL;DR: Wang and Griskevicius as discussed by the authors reported that women flaunt luxury products to signal their partners' devotion, thereby guarding their relationships from rivals, and found that perceived partner contribution to possessions was higher for designer products.
Abstract: We present two preregistered replications of the paper by Wang and Griskevicius (2014), which reported that women flaunt luxury products to signal their partners' devotion, thereby guarding their relationships from rivals. In Study 1, which was a conceptual replication with real luxury brands, we did not observe an effect of luxury products on partner devotion but found that women assumed that male partners contribute financial resources to women's luxury possessions. In Study 2, which was a direct replication with designer products, we observed a small-sized effect in the opposite direction, such that perceived partner devotion increased when women used nondesigner products. Similar to Study 1, perceived partner contribution to possessions was higher for designer products. (Less)

Summary (2 min read)

Introduction

  • This article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance with Now Publishers terms and conditions for self-archiving.
  • In Study 2, which was a direct replication with designer products, the authors observed a small-sized effect in the opposite direction, such that perceived partner devotion increased when women used nondesigner products.
  • Similar to Study 1, perceived partner contribution to possessions was higher for designer products.

1. Background

  • The motivational underpinnings of luxury consumption have long been a research area in marketing, and much attention has been directed towards understanding the role of luxury possessions in romantic relationships.
  • Yet, this motive did not explain why women spend on luxury, given that men are not attracted by expensive handbags or designer jewelry.
  • The question of why women spend on luxury was later addressed by Wang and Griskevicius (2014), which showed that the main motivation behind women’s luxury possessions was “mate guarding”, as women used luxury products to signal other women that they had a devoted partner, thereby protecting the mate and the relationship.
  • This study has not only been cited widely (over 300 citations in Google Scholar as of February 2020), but also received substantial coverage in media outlets such as Daily Mail, CBS News, The Atlantic, and Science Daily.
  • The main tenets of this research have not been replicated in the literature.

2.1. Hypotheses

  • In Study 1, their objective was to conceptually replicate and extend the findings presented in Wang and Griskevicius (2014).
  • Luxuriousness of a woman’s possessions will lead other women to perceive her as having a more devoted partner, also known as H1.
  • 1 Study hypotheses and methods were preregistered prior to data collection to ensure that data collection and analyses were conducted as planned.
  • Preregistration, as well as the survey, dataset, and analyses outputs for both experiments are publicly available at the Open Science Framework (see https://osf.io/czvu6/).
  • Benevolent sexism gives rise to beliefs such as women should be cherished by men or women’s financial needs should be satisfied by men; thus, women who score high in this trait might be more likely to link a woman’s luxury products to her partner’s devotion and his resources.

2.2. Sample and Design

  • The authors collected data from 250 participants (original experiment: N = 69) using the online participant pool Prolific (Palan & Schitter, 2018).
  • Participants were randomly assigned to the conditions (nnonluxury = 123, nluxury = 127).
  • He is her date and current relationship partner.

2.3. Procedure and Measures

  • First, all participants responded to six items that gauged benevolent sexism, which were adopted from the short version of the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (e.g., “Many women have a quality of purity that few men possess”, “Women should be cherished and protected by men”; Glick & Whitehead, 2010).
  • Louis Vuitton® and Tiffany & Co.®, which are among the most desirable luxury brands in the U.S. (Statista, 2018), represented luxury brands; H&M® and ZARA®, which are among the top general apparel brands (Brandirectory, 2019), represented nonluxury brands.
  • Participants read the following description (luxury condition in parentheses): “Imagine you are at a gala party, where you see another woman.
  • The survey ended with demographics questions and debriefing about the aim of the study.

2.4. Results

  • Hypothesis 1 examined whether women perceived other women with luxury possessions to have a more devoted partner.
  • Hypothesis 2 examined the likelihood that women assumed that the target woman’s partner paid for her luxury belongings.
  • The corresponding correlation matrix for these variables is presented in Table 2.
  • Conditional mediation analyses showed that, in the luxury possessions condition, the indirect effect of benevolent sexism on devotion through perceived partner contribution was not significant (B = 0.05, 95%CI [-0.01, 0.11], p = .102).

3.2. Procedure and Measures

  • As in Study 1, participants first read the description of a woman who was at a gala party with her date and had designer possessions: “Imagine you are at a gala party, where you see another woman.
  • He is her date and current relationship partner.
  • She is carrying a luxury designer (an unbranded) handbag.
  • You also notice that she has expensive and impressive (inexpensive and unimpressive) jewelry.”.
  • Next, participant responded to the manipulation check item (“I think this woman is interested in designer products”), followed by the two items measuring devotion (r = 0.86, p < .001; α = 0.92) and one item measuring male partner’s financial contribution to woman’s possessions, which were identical to the items in Study 1.

3.3. Results

  • Hypothesis 1, which stated that women with luxury possession will be perceived as having a more devoted partner, was not supported in Study 1.
  • The authors further conducted internal meta-analyses for the two presented replications.

4. Discussion and Conclusions

  • Wang and Griskevicius (2014) posited that one of the major motives for women to consume luxury is mate guarding, such that women use luxury possessions to signal other women that their partners are devoted to the relationship.
  • Another possibility is that women with luxury possessions were implicitly perceived to have materialistic traits, and the participants did not believe that the partner was devoted to a highly materialistic person.
  • Experimentally manipulating whom the women is with (e.g., friend, mother, husband, etc.) could uncover whether this contribution is linked to the romantic relationship or it is just a demand effect.

Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Citations
More filters
Journal Article
TL;DR: In this article, the authors examined the possibility that conspicuous displays of consumption and benevolence might serve as ''costly signals'' of desirable mate qualities, and found that romantic motives seem to produce highly strategic and sex-specific self-presentations best understood within a costly signaling framework.
Abstract: Conspicuous displays of consumption and benevolence might serve as \"costly signals\" of desirable mate qualities. If so, they should vary strategically with manipulations of mating-related motives. The authors examined this possibility in 4 experiments. Inducing mating goals in men increased their willingness to spend on conspicuous luxuries but not on basic necessities. In women, mating goals boosted public--but not private--helping. Although mating motivation did not generally inspire helping in men, it did induce more helpfulness in contexts in which they could display heroism or dominance. Conversely, although mating motivation did not lead women to conspicuously consume, it did lead women to spend more publicly on helpful causes. Overall, romantic motives seem to produce highly strategic and sex-specific self-presentations best understood within a costly signaling framework.

483 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The authors evaluated the replicability and generalizability of ten influential research on sensory marketing by conducting a high-powered and pre-registered replication in online settings in non-WEIRD consumers.
Abstract: We attempted to evaluate the replicability and generalizability of ten influential research on sensory marketing by conducting a high-powered and pre-registered replication in online settings in non-WEIRD consumers. The results revealed that only 20% of findings could be successfully replicated, and their effect sizes are as half as the original ones. Two successful studies had relatively larger sample sizes, used sound symbolism, and employed within-participants manipulation of senses. No studies involving visual factors, between-participant manipulation of senses, or interactions between factors could be replicated. Our findings reveal an initial estimate of the replicability and generalizability of sensory marketing.

1 citations

DOI
TL;DR: This article argued that the marketing canon has failed to confer social reality to the acts of its key protagonists, marketers, and suggested that more emphasis on marketer agency is suggested, this in the context of nominally focused anthropological enquiry.
Abstract: ABSTRACT This article argues that the marketing canon, as presently configured, has failed to confer social reality to the acts of its key protagonists – marketers. As an adjunct to both collective and contextually diverse perspectives on marketing, more emphasis on marketer agency is suggested, this in the context of nominally focused anthropological enquiry. It is further argued that the status afforded to consumer behaviour be similarly conferred for marketer behaviour, the latter under-represented within marketing research. Drawing on ontological nominalism, speech act theory and Searle’s social constructionism, this article addresses implications for intersubjective meaning within our community and offers provisional thoughts for how this might be structured and improved. It ends with a call to action for both the rehabilitation and expansion of purposeful marketer behaviour study.
References
More filters
Journal Article
TL;DR: This paper found that consumers who do not exit the luxury goods market are still interested in logo-laden products and that conspicuous consumption endures in recessions, even during economic downturns.
Abstract: The 2008 recession reportedly led to the demise of conspicuous consumption with wealthy consumers abandoning luxury goods that prominently display their brands for more subdued designs. Utilizing data collected before and in the midst of the recession from designer handbag manufacturers, Louis Vuitton and Gucci, we find products introduced during the recession actually display the brand far more prominently than those products withdrawn. Data from Hermes and luxury ads in Vogue magazine also indicate manufacturers did not tone things down. Our results suggest conspicuous consumption endures in recessions; consumers who do not exit the luxury goods market are still interested in logo-laden products.

83 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This paper showed that exposure to a sexily dressed woman increases single men's likelihood of noticing status products in a visual display, and they further support the link between conspicuous consumption and male mating strategies.

78 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors investigated the use of influence tactics in intimate relationships and the dependence of choice of various tactics on the type of conflict, gender of agents, relationship characteristics and personality variables, finding that the likelihood of using reason was higher in probability conflicts than in value and distributional conflicts.

68 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors examined possible effects of jealousy evocation and jealous expression in romantic relationships and found that while jealousy itself is not causally related to relationship stability, a person's reactions to their jealous partner may be.

67 citations

Posted Content
TL;DR: This article found that opposites tend to attract when it comes to certain spending tendencies, and that tightwads and spendthrifts tend to marry each other, consistent with the notion that people are attracted to mates who possess characteristics dissimilar to those they deplore in themselves (Klohnen and Mendelsohn 1998).
Abstract: Although much research finds that “birds of a feather flock together,” the present research suggests that opposites tend to attract when it comes to certain spending tendencies. That is, “tightwads,” who generally spend less than they would ideally like to spend, and “spendthrifts,” who generally spend more than they would ideally like to spend, tend to marry each other, consistent with the notion that people are attracted to mates who possess characteristics dissimilar to those they deplore in themselves (Klohnen and Mendelsohn 1998). In spite of this complementary attraction, tightwad/spendthrift differences within a marriage predict conflict over finances, which in turn predict diminished marital well-being. These relationships persist when controlling for important financial outcomes (household-level savings and credit card debt). These findings underscore the importance of studying the relationships between money, consumption, and happiness at an interpersonal level.

65 citations

Frequently Asked Questions (2)
Q1. What are the contributions mentioned in the paper "Replication note: women's luxury products as signals to other women" ?

The authors present two preregistered replications of the paper by Wang and Griskevicius ( 2014 ), which reported that women flaunt luxury products to signal their partners ’ devotion, thereby guarding their relationships from rivals. 

To eliminate this possibility, the authors conducted Study 2, a direct replication with designer ( vs. nondesigner ) products. One possibility is desirability bias. Another possibility is that women with luxury possessions were implicitly perceived to have materialistic traits, and the participants did not believe that the partner was devoted to a highly materialistic person. Future studies should further scrutinize the boundary conditions of the relationship between luxury products and partner devotion.