scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question

Showing papers on "Contrast (vision) published in 1987"


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the authors examined the relationship between contrast sensitivity and real-world targets (faces, road signs, objects) using a clinic-based sample of adults aged 20-77 years.
Abstract: A major assumption underlying the use of contrast sensitivity testing is that it predicts whether a patient has difficulty seeing objects encountered in everyday life. However, there has been no large-scale attempt to examine whether this putative relationship actually exists. We have examined this assumption using a clinic based sample of adults aged 20-77 years. Contrast thresholds were measured for both: (1) gratings of 0.5-22.8 cycles/degree; and (2) real-world targets (faces, road signs, objects). Multiple regression techniques indicated that the best predictors of thresholds for real-world targets were age and middle to low spatial frequencies. Models incorporating these variables accounted for 25-40% of the variance. Although acuity significantly correlated with thresholds for real-world targets, the inclusion of acuity as a predictor variable did not improve the model. These data provide direct evidence that spatial contrast sensitivity can effectively predict how well patients see targets typical of everyday life.

360 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: People with low vision usually require large characters to read, so high contrast is particularly important for them, so a contrast-sensitivity function (CSF) for reading is constructed and related to the shape of sine-wave grating CSFs.

333 citations


Book ChapterDOI
TL;DR: A real-time visual processing theory is used to unify the explanation of monocular and binocular brightness data and describes adaptive processes which overcome limitations of the visual uptake process to synthesize informative visual representations of the external world.
Abstract: A real-time visual processing theory is used to unify the explanation of monocular and binocular brightness data. This theory describes adaptive processes which overcome limitations of the visual uptake process to synthesize informative visual representations of the external world. The brightness data include versions of the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet effect and its exceptions, Bergstrom's demonstrations comparing the brightnesses of smoothly modulated and step-like luminance profiles, Hamada's demonstrations of nonclassical differences between the perception of luminance decrements and increments, Fechner's paradox, binocular brightness averaging, binocular brightness summation, binocular rivalry, and fading of stabilized images and ganzfelds. Familiar concepts such as spatial frequency analysis, Mach bands, and edge contrast are relevant but insufficient to explain the totality of these data. Two parallel contour-sensitive processes interact to generate the theory's brightness, color, and form explanations. A boundary-contour process is sensitive to the orientation and amount of contrast but not to the direction of contrast in scenic edges. It generates contours that form the boundaries of monocular perceptual domains. The spatial patterning of these contours is sensitive to the global configuration of scenic elements. A feature-contour process is insensitive to the orientation of contrast, but is sensitive to both the amount of contrast and to the direction of contrast in scenic edges. It triggers a diffusive filling-in reaction of featural quality within perceptual domains whose boundaries are dynamically defined by boundary contours. The boundary-contour system is hypothesized to include the hypercolumns in visual striate cortex. The feature-contour system is hypothesized to include the blobs in visual striate cortex. These preprocessed monocular activity patterns enter consciousness in the theory via a process of resonant binocular matching that is capable of selectively lifting whole monocular patterns into a binocular representation of form-and-color-in-depth. This binocular process is hypothesized to occur in area V4 of the visual prestriate cortex.

294 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is implied that the neural transfer function is much flatter than previously thought and that private line connections from foveal photoreceptors to higher visual centers are common.

200 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Temporal contrast sensitivity for counterphase flicker was determined for specifically disabled and normal readers to investigate whether the two groups differ in the functioning of their transient systems, supporting the hypothesis of a transient-system deficit in the visual systems of disabled readers.
Abstract: Temporal contrast sensitivity for counterphase flicker was determined for specifically disabled and normal readers to investigate whether the two groups differ in the functioning of their transient systems. In experiment 1, temporal contrast sensitivity was measured over a range of temporal frequencies with a spatial frequency of 2 cycles deg-1. Disabled readers were less sensitive than the control subjects at all temporal frequencies. In experiment 2, temporal contrast sensitivity was measured at a temporal frequency of 20 Hz over a range of spatial frequencies. Disabled readers were less sensitive than the controls at all spatial frequencies, with the differences between the groups increasing as spatial frequency increased. Both these findings are interpreted as supporting the hypothesis of a transient-system deficit in the visual systems of disabled readers.

180 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results show the older group to have significantly lower contrast sensitivity at medium and high spatial frequencies, and suggest that this is due primarily to retinal and neural changes with age, with optical factors having a slight effect at the highest spatial frequency only.

171 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Succcessful monovision subjects suppressed blur at higher contrast levels than did unsuccessful subjects, and these results suggest a possible clinical test for quantifying adaptation tomonovision.
Abstract: Presbyopic contact lens patients with monocular corrections (monovision) see clearly at all distances by virtue of an interocular suppression of anisometropic blur that occurs regionally between corresponding retinal areas. This suppression fails to occur with small high-contrast targets viewed under low luminance conditions. The effect of target size and contrast upon interocular suppression of blur was quantified by reducing contrast of a bright test spot, viewed binocularly while wearing various plus lenses monocularly, until the out-of-focus image was suppressed. The strength of interocular suppression was equivalent when the plus lens was before either eye. However, after subjects wore a plus lens over their nonsighting eye for one day, interocular suppression of blur became enhanced when the nonsighting eye was blurred, and it became reduced when the sighting eye was blurred. Successful monovision subjects suppressed blur at higher contrast levels than did unsuccessful subjects. These results suggest a possible clinical test for quantifying adaptation to monovision.

135 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results suggest that alterations occur already at the retinal level where dopamine receptors have been found, and the reported changes of the VEP are not caused by the visual cortex alone.

123 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Aug 1987-Nature
TL;DR: This work finds that when either illusory contours or random-dot patterns are moved in the vicinity of a colour-border, the colour border will also seem to move in the same direction even though it is physically stationary.
Abstract: There is a wealth of anatomical and psychological evidence which suggests that when people look at an object in the visual world, its various attributes such as colour, 'form', motion and depth are analysed by separate channels in the visual system. If so, how are these attributes put back together again to create a unified picture of the object? And if the object moves rapidly, how is perfect perceptual synchrony maintained between different features on its surface, if it is indeed true that they are being processed separately? Our evidence suggests that the visual system extracts certain conspicuous image features based on luminance contrast, and that the signals derived from these are then attributed to other features on the object, a process that we call 'capture'. Specifically, we find that when either illusory contours or random-dot patterns are moved in the vicinity of a colour-border, the colour border will also seem to move in the same direction even though it is physically stationary.

118 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Apr 1987-Brain
TL;DR: It is concluded that orientation selectivity implicates visual cortical cells in Parkinson's disease and that a preferential loss of contrast sensitivity to horizontal gratings might be due to a functional abnormality in the striate cortex that relatively spares the extrastriate cortex.
Abstract: Visual contrast sensitivity was measured using 2 cycle/deg sinewave gratings of different orientations in 10 patients with Parkinson's disease and in 15 age-matched controls. Loss of visual contrast sensitivity was found in 6 patients, all of whom had normal visual acuity. Visual loss depended on grating orientation; in all cases the maximum sensitivity loss was for the horizontal. Sensitivity loss was most marked at a temporal frequency of 4 to 8 Hz. Visual fields gave no hint of the orientation selectivity. We conclude that orientation selectivity implicates visual cortical cells in Parkinson's disease. We tentatively suggest that a preferential loss of contrast sensitivity to horizontal gratings might be due to a functional abnormality in the striate cortex that relatively spares the extrastriate cortex. The dependence of visual loss on temporal frequency combined with the sparing of visual acuity might possibly be understood if Parkinson's disease preferentially affects the visual pathway leading from the retina to cortex via the magnocellular layer of the lateral geniculate nucleus. There is an intriguing similarity between the pattern of visual loss in Parkinson's disease and in multiple sclerosis.

114 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The patient had the symptoms of cerebral achromatopsia, and his achromatic vision supports the view that the many different visual cortical areas recently demonstrated in the brains of monkeys, and presumed to exist in man, have a perceptual specialisation that matches their physiological differences.
Abstract: A patient is described whose most striking visual disorder was a grossly impaired ability to discriminate between different colours (hues) that were matched for brightness. In contrast his ability to discriminate between different neutral greys presented in the same fashion was much less abnormal, even though the greys were perceptually difficult. Although visual acuity was reduced and visual fields were constricted, and the patient's memory was moderately impaired, these associated symptoms could not themselves be the cause of his unusual colour vision. The patient had the symptoms of cerebral achromatopsia, and the relative preservation of his form vision (when his reduced acuity is taken into account) and his achromatic vision supports the view that the many different visual cortical areas recently demonstrated in the brains of monkeys, and presumed to exist in man, have a perceptual specialisation that matches their physiological differences.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results of the study indicate that deficits in visual function are common after resolution of optic neuritis.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The data and modelling supported the idea that subjects exploited probability summation at threshold while using a peak detection criterion above threshold, and single- and multiple-channel models of temporal processing are discussed.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A model of interval discrimination is developed, including an opponent stage between hypothetical spatial coincidence detectors, and the relative contrast of the two bars in an interval discrimination could be randomly perturbed over trials without substantially reducing acuity.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Investigating adult age differences in four measures of visual function indicated that contrast sensitivity is a useful measure for detecting age‐related changes in visual function and that a common mechanism may underlie age differences on various visual tests.
Abstract: We investigated adult age differences in four measures of visual function: distance acuity, near acuity, stereopsis, and contrast sensitivity. Twenty-four young adults (mean age 19.5 years) and 24 older adults (mean age 68.4 years) participated. Age differences were present in each of the four measures. A stepwise discriminant analysis performed on the four measures revealed that, when the correlations among the measures were taken into account, only contrast sensitivity significantly discriminated young and older adults' performance. The strength of the correlations among the four measures was greater for older adults than for young adults. The results indicated that contrast sensitivity is a useful measure for detecting age-related changes in visual function and that a common mechanism may underlie age differences on various visual tests.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Cell activity can be related to cone excitation in a trichromatic colour space with the help of the model, to give an indication of suprathreshold coding of colour and lightness.
Abstract: Coloured surfaces in the normal environment may be brighter or dimmer than the mean adaptation level. Changes in the firing rate of cells of the parvocellular layers of macaque lateral geniculate nucleus were studied with such stimuli; chromatic mixtures briefly replaced a white adaptation field. This paradigm is therefore one of successive contrast. Families of intensity-response curves for different wavelengths were measured. When taking sections at different luminance ratios through these families of curves, strongly opponent cells displayed spectrally selective responses at low luminance ratios, while weakly opponent cells had higher chromatic thresholds and responded well to stimuli at higher luminance ratios, brighter than the adaptation field. Strength of cone opponency, defined as the weight of the inhibitory cone mechanism relative to the excitatory one, was thus related to the range of intensity in which cells appeared to operate most effectively. S-cone inputs, as tested with lights lying along tritanopic confusion lines, could either be excitatory or inhibitory. Families of curves for different wavelengths can be simulated mathematically for a given cell by a simple model by using known cone absorption spectra. Hyperbolic response functions relate cone absorption to the output signals of the three cone mechanisms, which are assumed to interact linearly. Parameters from the simulation provided estimates of strength of cone opponency and cone sensitivity which were shown to be continuously distributed. Cell activity can be related to cone excitation in a trichromatic colour space with the help of the model, to give an indication of suprathreshold coding of colour and lightness.

Patent
14 Aug 1987
TL;DR: In this paper, a colored original to be reproduced is scanned point-by-point in each of the three primary colors and the resulting imaging signals are processed to generate a luminance signal and a pair of chrominance signals.
Abstract: A colored original to be reproduced is scanned point-by-point in each of the three primary colors. The resulting imaging signals are processed to generate a luminance signal and a pair of chrominance signals. The luminance signal is branched into a high-pass filter channel and a low-pass filter channel. The high-pass portion of the luminance signal is amplified in accordance with a sublinear characteristic function which causes signals of small amplitude to be amplified to a greater degree than signals of large amplitude. The low-pass portion of the luminance signal is modified per a non-linear characteristic gradation function. The thus-modified high-pass and low-pass portions of the luminance signal are added to yield an enhanced luminance signal. The enhanced luminance signal is divided by the original luminance signal and each of the chrominance signals is multiplied by the resulting quotient. This automatially compensates for the changes in color saturation which accompany changes in brightness. After the multiplication, the chrominance signals are amplified in accordance with a sublinear characteristic function which causes signals of small amplitude to be amplified to a greater degree than signals of large amplitude. This characteristic function further has a form such that no additional ampification takes place beyond a predetermined threshold limit.

Book ChapterDOI
TL;DR: A theory is presented of how global visual interactions between depth, length, lightness, and form percepts can occur and suggests how quantized activity patterns which reflect these visual properties can coherently fill-in, or complete, visually ambiguous regions starting with visually informative data features.
Abstract: A theory is presented of how global visual interactions between depth, length, lightness, and form percepts can occur. The theory suggests how quantized activity patterns which reflect these visual properties can coherently fill-in, or complete, visually ambiguous regions starting with visually informative data features. Phenomena such as the Cornsweet and Craik-O'Brien effects, phantoms and subjective contours, binocular brightness summation, the equidistance tendency, Emmert's law, allelotropia, multiple spatial frequency scaling and edge detection, figure-ground completion, coexistence of depth and binocular rivalry, reflectance rivalry, Fechner's paradox, decrease of threshold contrast with increased number of cycles in a grating pattern, hysteresis, adaptation level tuning, Weber law modulation, shift of sensitivity with background luminance, and the finite capacity of visual short term memory are discussed in terms of a small set of concepts and mechanisms. Limitations of alternative visual theories which depend upon Fourier analysis, Laplacians, zero-crossings, and cooperative depth planes are described. Relationships between monocular and binocular processing of the same visual patterns are noted, and a shift in emphasis from edge and disparity computations toward the characterization of resonant activity-scaling correlations across multiple spatial scales is recommended. This recommendation follows from the theory's distinction between the concept of a structural spatial scale, which is determined by local receptive field properties, and a functional spatial scale, which is defined by the interaction between global properties of a visual scene and the network as a whole. Functional spatial scales, but not structural spatial scales, embody the quantization of network activity that reflects a scene's global visual representation. A functional scale is generated by a filling-in resonant exchange, or FIRE, which can be ignited by an exchange of feedback signals among the binocular cells where monocular patterns are binocularly matched.

Journal ArticleDOI
Montgomery Gp1, Vaz Na1
TL;DR: The results presented here demonstrate the importance of standardizing procedures for measuring contrast ratios of these new liquid crystal films and explain the differences between the illumination geometries in the two measurements.
Abstract: Contrast ratio is an important measure of the performance of an electrooptic display. From measurements of film brightness and transmittance, we determined contrast ratios of thin polymer films containing microdroplets of liquid crystalline material. Contrast ratios based on brightness were typically ~3 for all samples studied, whereas contrast ratios based on transmittance varied from ~20 to 200 depending on the sample. We explain these differences by analyzing the relations between the illumination geometries in the two measurements and the voltage-dependent multiple scattering of light which controls the electrooptic performance of the films. Effects of the spectral content of the light source and the response of the human eye on contrast ratio were also determined. The results presented here demonstrate the importance of standardizing procedures for measuring contrast ratios of these new liquid crystal films.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Vernier thresholds were affected by both grating contrast and spatial frequency, and most of the data are consistent with the predictions of Wilson's [(1986) Vision Res. 26, 453-469] model.


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The model is sufficiently general to account for binocular performance at the differential luminance threshold and in suprathreshold contrast matching, and it also offers a novel explanation for interocular transfer of adaptation.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The contrast sensitivity function of the rat was assessed by investigating the relationship between the amplitude of visually evoked cortical potentials (VECP) and the spatial frequency and contrast of grating stimuli.

Patent
14 Aug 1987
TL;DR: In this paper, an improved method for correction of color saturation, in electronic image processing, automatically compensates for brightness changes resulting from contrast processing, which is used with a two-dimensional original image is scanned along rows and columns for three primary colors.
Abstract: An improved method for correction of color saturation, in electronic image processing, automatically compensates for brightness changes resulting from contrast processing. The method is used with electronic image processing, in which a two-dimensional original image is electro-optically scanned along rows and columns for three primary colors, the resulting image signals are transformed into a luminance signal Y and two color-difference or chrominance signals C1, C2, and the luminance signal is modified by electronic contrast processing. The invention improves the basic image processing method by measuring the luminance signals Y 1 and Y 2 which occur, respectively, before and after contrast processing, and multiplying their quotient Y 2 /Y 1 by each of the two chrominance signals C1, C2. This compensates automatically for the increase in color saturation which accompanies a brightness reduction and for the decrease in color saturation which accompanies a brightness increase.

01 Jan 1987
TL;DR: In this paper, a retinal illuminance response for higher spatial frequencies can be computed from the optical transfer function of the eye, and these computed responses are subtracted from pattern electroretinograms to reveal a pattern-specific response with a marked bandpass characteristic.
Abstract: Pattern electroretinograms have been presumed to arise from a combination of luminance and pattern detection activities. Since the responses at low spatial frequencies are linearly related to contrast and contain negligible pattern specific components, it is proposed that a retinal illuminance response for higher spatial frequencies can be computed from the optical transfer function of the eye. These computed responses are subtracted from pattern electroretinograms to reveal a pattern-specific response with a marked bandpass characteristic. The peak spatial frequency of the bandpass curve declines with increasing peripheral angle. For central vision, the peak amplitude of the pattern-specific response is larger than the retinal illuminance response, but, in the peripheral retina, the two responses are found to be almost equal. The possible origins of these signals are discussed, and it is concluded that the technique provides a method of obtaining separated illuminance and pattern responses from retinal regions having different properties of spatial selectivity.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A comparison between patients with strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia showed that, when matched for visual acuity, the contrast sensitivity functions were similar for both the nonamblyopic and amblyopic eyes.

Journal Article
TL;DR: It is concluded that the technique provides a method of obtaining separated illuminance and pattern responses from retinal regions having different properties of spatial selectivity.
Abstract: Pattern electroretinograms have been presumed to arise from a combination of luminance and pattern detection activities. Since the responses at low spatial frequencies are linearly related to contrast and contain negligible pattern specific components, it is proposed that a retinal illuminance response for higher spatial frequencies can be computed from the optical transfer function of the eye. These computed responses are subtracted from pattern electroretinograms to reveal a pattern-specific response with a marked bandpass characteristic. The peak spatial frequency of the bandpass curve declines with increasing peripheral angle. For central vision, the peak amplitude of the pattern-specific response is larger than the retinal illuminance response, but, in the peripheral retina, the two responses are found to be almost equal. The possible origins of these signals are discussed, and it is concluded that the technique provides a method of obtaining separated illuminance and pattern responses from retinal regions having different properties of spatial selectivity.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The signal-to-noise ratio S as a function of velocity V at eccentricities of 0°, 3°, 6°, 12°, 24°, 48° in the temporal visual field of the right eye was determined in this paper.
Abstract: The detection of coherent motion was studied in stroboscopically displayed moving random-dot patterns disturbed by incoherent noise. We determined the threshold signal-to-noise ratio S as a function of velocity V at eccentricities of 0°, 3°, 6°, 12°, 24°, 48° in the temporal visual field of the right eye. At each eccentricity the measurements of S = ƒ(V) were repeated for a range of rms contrast values from 60% (0 dB) in steps of 3 dB down to 1.9% (-30 dB). All stimuli were scaled with eccentricity to keep the ratio of pixel size to acuity constant (about 2). It is shown that the S values in our paradigm are never determined by contrast-threshold effects. They are true correlational thresholds. Bilocal movement detectors are assumed to underlie the detection of coherent motion. The bilocal correlation proves to be rather insensitive to rms contrast down to contrast levels of about 10%. Despite the eccentricity scaling, which is quite effective at high contrast levels, differences between the eccentricities become noticeable at lower contrast levels (below about 30–20%). The fovea is the least, and the far periphery the most, resistent to contrast degradation.

Journal Article
TL;DR: The results on patients show that the second harmonic of the pattern response, but not that of the uniform-field response, is reduced in cases of optic atrophy secondary to trauma or optic neuritis, which suggests generators different, at least in part, for thesecond harmonic evoked by modulation of either luminance or spatial contrast.
Abstract: The amplitude of the second harmonic of the focal electroretinogram (ERG) in response to either modulation of the luminance of the uniform-field or the spatial contrast of a patterned field (pattern-reversal ERG) was measured in a group of normal subjects as well as in patients with two different types of unilateral dysfunctions, namely optic atrophy or temporary retinal ischemia. Such patients had a reduced visual acuity in their affected eyes but normal full-field flicker (20 Hz) ERG. In normal eyes, for the same stimulation area and modulation depth, the second harmonic of the uniform-field response is smaller (mean value 62%) than that of the optimal pattern (around 1.5 cycles/degree). The results on patients show that the second harmonic of the pattern response, but not that of the uniform-field response, is reduced in cases of optic atrophy secondary to trauma or optic neuritis. This result suggests generators different, at least in part, for the second harmonic evoked by modulation of either luminance or spatial contrast. By contrast, both responses are reduced in cases of temporary retinal ischemia. These findings are discussed in light of the recent literature on the origin of the pattern ERG. The possible clinical applications of the technique are outlined.

Book ChapterDOI
01 Jan 1987
TL;DR: It is found that displacement thresholds are abnormal in ocular hypertension and glaucoma and it is suggested that this may provide a more sensitive and specific measure of loss of vision in glau coma and ocular pulmonary hypertension than conventional perimetric measures.
Abstract: Displacement thresholds may depend on processing by ganglion cells and, like contrast sensitivity and colour vision, may therefore be affected in glaucoma. However, unlike contrast sensitivity and colour vision, human sensitivity to motion is relatively retained outside the fovea so that displacement thresholds may provide a sensitive measure of ganglion cell mediated function throughout the visual field. We have investigated displacement thresholds to a 2 minute by 2 degree line generated by computer on a green phosphor video display unit. We found that displacement thresholds are abnormal in ocular hypertension and glaucoma and suggest that this may provide a more sensitive and specific measure of loss of vision in glaucoma and ocular hypertension than conventional perimetric measures.