scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question

Showing papers on "Contrast (vision) published in 2006"


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, a CCD camera-based luminance mapping technology was used to provide new insight into the impact of luminance distributions on glare, and a new index, daylight glare probability (DGP), was developed and is presented in this paper.

783 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jul 2006
TL;DR: An automatic, real-time video and image abstraction framework that abstracts imagery by modifying the contrast of visually important features, namely luminance and color opponency is presented and finds that participants are faster at naming abstracted faces of known persons compared to photographs.
Abstract: We present an automatic, real-time video and image abstraction framework that abstracts imagery by modifying the contrast of visually important features, namely luminance and color opponency. We reduce contrast in low-contrast regions using an approximation to anisotropic diffusion, and artificially increase contrast in higher contrast regions with difference-of-Gaussian edges. The abstraction step is extensible and allows for artistic or data-driven control. Abstracted images can optionally be stylized using soft color quantization to create cartoon-like effects with good temporal coherence. Our framework design is highly parallel, allowing for a GPU-based, real-time implementation. We evaluate the effectiveness of our abstraction framework with a user-study and find that participants are faster at naming abstracted faces of known persons compared to photographs. Participants are also better at remembering abstracted images of arbitrary scenes in a memory task.

576 citations


Proceedings ArticleDOI
29 Nov 2006
TL;DR: An approximate solution to this problem is proposed that uses median-cut to find the areas considered of high luminance and subsequently applies a density estimation to generate an Expand-map in order to extend the range in the high Luminance areas using an inverse Photographic Tone Reproduction operator.
Abstract: In recent years many Tone Mapping Operators (TMOs) have been presented in order to display High Dynamic Range Images (HDRI) on typical display devices. TMOs compress the luminance range while trying to maintain contrast. The dual of tone mapping, inverse tone mapping, expands a Low Dynamic Range Image (LDRI) into a HDRI. HDRIs contain a broader range of physical values that can be perceived by the human visual system. The majority of today's media is stored in low dynamic range. Inverse Tone Mapping Operators (iTMOs) could thus potentially revive all of this content for use in high dynamic range display and image-based lighting. We propose an approximate solution to this problem that uses median-cut to find the areas considered of high luminance and subsequently apply a density estimation to generate an Expand-map in order to extend the range in the high luminance areas using an inverse Photographic Tone Reproduction operator.

254 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This work performs binocular summation and monocular, binocular, and dichoptic masking experiments for horizontal 1 cycle per degree test and masking gratings to reject three previously published proposals and favors the two-stage model.
Abstract: A fundamental problem for any visual system with binocular overlap is the combination of information from the two eyes. Electrophysiology shows that binocular integration of luminance contrast occurs early in visual cortex, but a specific systems architecture has not been established for human vision. Here, we address this by performing binocular summation and monocular, binocular, and dichoptic masking experiments for horizontal 1 cycle per degree test and masking gratings. These data reject three previously published proposals, each of which predict too little binocular summation and insufficient dichoptic facilitation. However, a simple development of one of the rejected models (the twin summation model) and a completely new model (the two-stage model) provide very good fits to the data. Two features common to both models are gently accelerating (almost linear) contrast transduction prior to binocular summation and suppressive ocular interactions that contribute to contrast gain control. With all model parameters fixed, both models correctly predict (1) systematic variation in psychometric slopes, (2) dichoptic contrast matching, and (3) high levels of binocular summation for various levels of binocular pedestal contrast. A review of evidence from elsewhere leads us to favor the two-stage model.

241 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It was found that training substantially improved visual acuity and contrast-sensitivity functions in the amblyopic eyes of all the observers in Groups I and II, although no significant performance improvement was observed in Group III.

234 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results indicate that dynamic visual cues play a dominant causal role in attracting attention, while some static visual cuesplay a weaker causal role, while other static cues are not causal at all, and may instead reflect top-down causes.

229 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, the Hartmann-Shack wavefront analyzer was used to investigate the relation between contrast sensitivity function and ocular higher-order wavefront aberrations in normal human eyes.

145 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Increased retinal illuminance due to the bright zone of males is used to enhance contrast sensitivity and speed motion detector responses, and local neural properties vary across the visual world in a way not expected if HS cells serve purely as matched filters to measure yaw-induced visual motion.
Abstract: Eyes of the hoverfly Eristalis tenax are sexually dimorphic such that males have a fronto-dorsal region of large facets. In contrast to other large flies in which large facets are associated with a decreased interommatidial angle to form a dorsal 'acute zone' of increased spatial resolution, we show that a dorsal region of large facets in males appears to form a 'bright zone' of increased light capture without substantially increased spatial resolution. Theoretically, more light allows for increased performance in tasks such as motion detection. To determine the effect of the bright zone on motion detection, local properties of wide field motion detecting neurons were investigated using localized sinusoidal gratings. The pattern of local preferred directions of one class of these cells, the HS cells, in Eristalis is similar to that reported for the blowfly Calliphora. The bright zone seems to contribute to local contrast sensitivity; high contrast sensitivity exists in portions of the receptive field served by large diameter facet lenses of males and is not observed in females. Finally, temporal frequency tuning is also significantly faster in this frontal portion of the world, particularly in males, where it overcompensates for the higher spatial-frequency tuning and shifts the predicted local velocity optimum to higher speeds. These results indicate that increased retinal illuminance due to the bright zone of males is used to enhance contrast sensitivity and speed motion detector responses. Additionally, local neural properties vary across the visual world in a way not expected if HS cells serve purely as matched filters to measure yaw-induced visual motion.

130 citations


Journal Article
TL;DR: In normal human eyes, comalike aberration of the eye significantly influences contrast sensitivity function.

123 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Although Z40 was significantly lower in the eyes with the aspherical IOL, no statistically significant differences were found between asphericals and spherical IOLs in LCVA, HCVA, and contrast sensitivity.
Abstract: Purpose To intraindividually compare visual performance in terms of photopic high-contrast visual acuity (HCVA), mesopic HCVA, mesopic low-contrast visual acuity (LCVA), and contrast sensitivity (CS) in patients after implantation of either an aspherical or a spherical intraocular lens (IOL). Setting Department of Ophthalmology, Johann Wolfgang Goethe-University, Frankfurt am Main, Germany. Methods Forty eyes of 20 patients were randomized to implantation of an aspherical IOL (Tecnis Z9000, AMO) in 1 eye and a spherical IOL (Sensar AR40e, AMO) in the other eye. Three to 4 months postoperatively, photopic HCVA (270 cd/m 2 ) was measured with the observer-independent Frankfurt-Freiburg Contrast and Acuity Test System (FF-CATS) and high-mesopic HCVA and LCVA (8 cd/m 2 ) were measured with Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study charts. CS was assessed with the FF-CATS under photopic (167 cd/m 2 ), high-mesopic (1.67 cd/m 2 ), and low-mesopic (0.167 cd/m 2 ) luminance conditions with and without glare. For each individual eye, higher-order wavefront aberrations were reconstructed for a physiological mesopic pupil diameter. Intraindividual differences (Δ i ) in visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and higher-order aberrations (HOAs) were calculated, and the influence of age and Δ i HOA on Δ i contrast sensitivity (logCS) under high-mesopic conditions was investigated using multiple regression analysis. Results There were no statistically significant differences between the Tecnis IOL and the Sensar IOL in visual acuity measurements or contrast sensitivity measurements. For physiological mesopic pupil diameter, primary spherical aberration (Z 4 0 ) was significantly lower in the Tecnis group ( P 4 0 , the Δ i values were distributed around zero. Multiple regression analysis showed only a partial influence of Δ i Z 4 0 on Δ i logCS (adjusted R 2 = 0.49) but did not show any influence of age, coma-like aberration, or residual HOA. Conclusions Although Z 4 0 was significantly lower in the eyes with the aspherical IOL, no statistically significant differences were found between aspherical and spherical IOLs in LCVA, HCVA, and contrast sensitivity. Statistical analysis of intraindividual contrast sensitivity differences showed that in most patients, this Z 4 0 difference was too low to have an effect on contrast sensitivity.

104 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A simple and effective implementation of the proposed self-adaptive contrast enhancement algorithm based on plateau histogram equalization for infrared images, including its threshold value calculation, is described by using pipeline and parallel computation architecture.

Journal ArticleDOI
21 Dec 2006-Neuron
TL;DR: The results indicate that in cerebral cortex the modulation of synchronicity of responses is used as a mechanism complementary to rate changes to enhance the saliency of neuronal responses.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this paper, a tomographic technique is proposed for reconstruction under specified conditions of the three-dimensional distribution of complex refractive index in a sample from a single projection image per view angle, where the images display both absorption contrast and propagation-induced phase contrast.
Abstract: A tomographic technique is proposed for reconstruction under specified conditions of the three-dimensional distribution of complex refractive index in a sample from a single projection image per view angle, where the images display both absorption contrast and propagation-induced phase contrast. The algorithm achieves high numerical stability as a consequence of the complementary nature of the absorption and phase contrast transfer functions. The method is pertinent to biomedical imaging and nondestructive testing of samples exhibiting weak absorption contrast. © 2006 American Institute of Physics. DOI: 10.1063/1.2226794 X-ray computer tomography CT is a well-established technique for three-dimensional 3D imaging of internal structure of samples. 1,2 Conventional CT imaging is based on the differential attenuation of transmitted x rays by constituents of the sample. This contrast mechanism is effective for distinguishing between elemental components with significant differences in atomic number or density, e.g., between flesh and bones in the case of medical CT. However, the difference in x-ray attenuation by different types of soft tissues e.g., healthy and malignant ones is typically rather weak, which results in poor image contrast, hampering diagnostics. It has been suggested that x-ray phase contrast can be utilized for improvement of the contrast in transmission images of noncrystalline samples consisting predominantly of light chemical elements. 3‐5 Subsequently, phase-contrast x-ray CT PCT has been implemented in several forms, including x-ray interferometry, 6,7 , analyzer-based 8,9 and propagation-based phase contrast, 10‐12 and others. The sub

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In eyes with 20/17 and better P HC acuity, P HC logMAR acuity is insensitive to variations in retinal image quality compared with M LC logMARAcuity, and the ability of the metrics of retinal images quality to predict log MAR acuity improved as luminance and/or contrast is lowered.
Abstract: Purpose. The purpose of this study is to determine the ability of single-value metrics of retinal image quality of the eye to predict visual performance as measured by high (HC) and low (LC) -contrast acuity at photopic (P) and mesopic (M) light levels in eyes with 20/17 and better visual acuity. Methods. Forty-nine normal subjects in good health ranging in age from 21.8 to 62.6 with 20/17 or better monocular high-contrast logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR) acuity served as subjects. Wavefront error through the 10th Zernike radial order over a 7-mm pupil was measured on each test eye using a custom-built Shack/Hartmann wavefront sensor. For each eye, 31 different single-value retinal image quality metrics were calculated. Visual acuity was measured using HC (95%) and LC (11%) logMAR at photopic (270 cd/m 2 ) and mesopic (0.75 cd/m 2 ) light levels. To determine the ability of each metric of retinal image quality to predict each type of logMAR acuity (P HC, P LC, M HC, and M LC), each acuity measure was regressed against each optical quality metric. Results. The ability of the metrics of retinal image quality to predict logMAR acuity improved as luminance and/or contrast is lowered. The best retinal image quality metric (logPFSc) accounted for 2.6%, 15.1%, 27.6%, and 40.0% of the variance in P HC, P LC, M HC, and M LC logMAR acuity, respectively. Conclusions. In eyes with 20/17 and better P HC acuity, P HC logMAR acuity is insensitive to variations in retinal image quality compared with M LC logMAR acuity. Retinal image quality becomes increasingly predictive of logMAR acuity as contrast and/or luminance is decreased. Everyday life requires individuals to function over a large range of contrast and luminance levels. Clinically, the impact of retinal image quality as a function of luminance and contrast is readily measurable in a time-efficient manner with M LC logMAR acuity charts. (Optom Vis Sci 2006;83:635–640)

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Visual function in patients with yellow tinted IOLs is virtually the same as that in patientsWith non-tinted Iolo groups, and there was no significant difference in contrast visual acuity loss as a result of glare.
Abstract: Aim: To compare the visual function in patients with short wavelength blue light blocking yellow tinted intraocular lenses (IOLs) with that in patients with non-tinted IOLs. Methods: 74 patients scheduled for bilateral cataract surgery underwent implantation of either yellow IOLs (HOYA YA60BB) or non-tinted IOLs (VA60BB) in both eyes. Contrast visual acuity with and without a glare source was measured under photopic (100 cd/m2) and mesopic (slightly higher luminance than typically used—5 cd/m2) conditions at 2 weeks and 3 months after surgery using the contrast sensitivity accurate tester. Visual acuity and the incidence of patients who noted cyanopsia were also examined. Results: No significant differences between the yellow tinted and non-tinted IOL groups were observed in mean visual acuity or in photopic or higher luminance mesopic contrast visual acuity with and without glare source at either 2 weeks or 3 months after surgery. Furthermore, there was no significant difference in contrast visual acuity loss as a result of glare. The incidence of patients who noticed cyanopsia was significantly less in the yellow tinted IOL group than in the non-tinted IOL group at 2 weeks after surgery (p = 0.0234), but no patients reported cyanopsia at 3 months. Conclusion: Visual function in patients with yellow tinted IOLs is virtually the same as that in patients with non-tinted IOLs.

Patent
13 Jun 2006
TL;DR: In this article, a low-resolution bitmap is magnified to form a magnified image and edge detection is performed to find high contrast edges to produce a plurality of foreground and background decisions determining whether a portion of an image patch is a background or a foreground.
Abstract: A technique for generating high-resolution bitmaps from low-resolution bitmaps. A low-resolution bitmap is magnified to form a magnified image. Edge detection is performed on the magnified image to find high contrast edges. A plurality of image patches of the magnified image are generated. These images patches are analyzed by performing connected components analysis on each of them using the high contrast edges to produce a plurality of foreground and background decisions determining whether a portion of an image patch is a background or a foreground region. Then the contrast of one or more pixels in each of the plurality of image patches is enhanced based on the foreground and background decisions. Finally, the system and method of the invention combines the luminance of the enhanced output pixels with the color values generated by the magnification algorithm. This produces a high-resolution bitmap from the contrast-enhanced pixels.

Patent
26 Jan 2006
TL;DR: In this paper, a method and apparatus for adjusting the contrast of an image, in which each pixel in the image has an initial brightness level that is within a range of brightness levels between a minimum brightness level and a maximum brightness level, is presented.
Abstract: A method and apparatus for adjusting the contrast of an image, in which each pixel in the image has an initial brightness level that is within a range of brightness levels between a minimum brightness level and a maximum brightness level. For each brightness level within the range, the number of pixels that have the same initial brightness level is counted. The pixels are divided into at least two types by identifying the or each set of pixels having initial brightness levels in which each of a plurality of adjacent ones of said brightness levels has more pixels than a threshold number. The pixels of the plurality of adjacent ones of said brightness levels are treated as a first type. The remaining pixels are treated as a second type. For the or each set of pixels of the first type, contrast enhancement is carried out separately on those pixels.

Patent
24 Mar 2006
TL;DR: In this article, the authors proposed a method to automatically regulate brightness of liquid crystal displays (LCDs), which resolves the overload and easy fatigue of human eyes and poor image contrast due to high brightness of the conventional LCD.
Abstract: The present invention provides a method to automatically regulate brightness of liquid crystal displays (LCDs), which resolves the overload and easy fatigue of human eyes and poor image contrast due to high brightness of the conventional LCD. The present invention includes use of four parts, including an image brightness regulating unit, an image brightness ratio computation and output controlling unit, an ambient light detection and brightness adaptation regulating unit and an image gray level expanding unit. The image signals of an LCD are processed in such a manner to enable automatic control of optical flux of LCD TV by regulating the gray level of output images. Also, optical flux of LCD TV can be automatically controlled with the change of images and ambient light, thus alleviating overburdening of human eyes and improving the contrast and comfort of images for a better visual effect.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The purpose of this work is to analyze a displayed LDR image against its original HDR counterpart in terms of perceived contrast distortion, and to enhance the LDR depiction with perceptually driven colour adjustments to restore the original HDR contrast information.
Abstract: High Dynamic Range (HDR) images capture the full range of luminance present in real world scenes, and unlike Low Dynamic Range (LDR) images, can simultaneously contain detailed information in the deepest of shadows and the brightest of light sources. For display or aesthetic purposes, it is often necessary to perform tone mapping, which creates LDR depictions of HDR images at the cost of contrast information loss. The purpose of this work is two-fold: to analyze a displayed LDR image against its original HDR counterpart in terms of perceived contrast distortion, and to enhance the LDR depiction with perceptually driven colour adjustments to restore the original HDR contrast information. For analysis, we present a novel algorithm for the characterization of tone mapping distortion in terms of observed loss of global contrast, and loss of contour and texture details. We classify existing tone mapping operators accordingly. We measure both distortions with perceptual metrics that enable the automatic and meaningful enhancement of LDR depictions. For image enhancement, we identify artistic and photographic colour techniques from which we derive adjustments that create contrast with colour. The enhanced LDR image is an improved depiction of the original HDR image with restored contrast information.

Proceedings ArticleDOI
30 Jul 2006
TL;DR: An innovative algorithm is presented that dynamically adjusts the content of the input images before radiometric compensation is carried out, which reduces the perceived visual artifacts while simultaneously preserving a maximum of luminance and contrast.
Abstract: We present a real-time algorithm for dynamically adjusting radiometric compensation depending on the image content. This reduces the perception of clipping errors by simultaneously preserving a maximum of luminance and contrast. The algorithm is implemented entirely on the GPU and runs in real-time.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The proposed visual quality metric is based on an effective Human Visual System model and relies on the computation of three distortion factors: blockiness, edge errors and visual impairments, which take into account the typical artifacts introduced by several classes of coders.
Abstract: In this paper, a multi-factor full-reference image quality index is presented. The proposed visual quality metric is based on an effective Human Visual System model. Images are pre-processed in order to take into account luminance masking and contrast sensitivity effects. The proposed metric relies on the computation of three distortion factors: blockiness, edge errors and visual impairments, which take into account the typical artifacts introduced by several classes of coders. A pooling algorithm is used in order to obtain a single distortion index. Results show the effectiveness of the proposed approach and its consistency with subjective evaluations.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The mobility performance, as measured by PPWS, was associated with visual fields but not with visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, or dynamic vision measures.
Abstract: PURPOSE. The purpose of this study was to determine the association of static (visual acuity, visual fields, and contrast sensitivity) and dynamic (dynamic visual acuity and motion threshold) measures of vision with mobility performance on a mobility course with obstacles. METHODS. A cross-sectional population-based study of 1504 persons aged 72 to 92 years enrolled in the third round of the Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project. Standardized examinations were used to test binocular visual acuity, better eye-contrast sensitivity, visual fields, dynamic visual acuity, and motion threshold. Cognitive status was assessed by using the standardized Mini-Mental State Examination. Participants were timed when walking a straight 4-m distance and when walking through a mobility course seeded with obstacles. The percentage of preferred walking speed (PPWS) for each subject was calculated as the ratio of mobility course speed to a 4-m walking speed expressed as a percentage. RESULTS. The mean age of the participants was 78.2 years. The mean 4-m walking speed was 0.82 m/s, whereas the mean mobility course speed was 0.47 m/s. The mean PPWS was 57.1%. All vision variables except visual acuity were associated with PPWS in univariate analyses. Multivariate models found visual fields and the cognitive state to be associated with PPWS. There was no association with dynamic measures of vision. CONCLUSIONS. The mobility performance, as measured by PPWS, was associated with visual fields but not with visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, or dynamic vision measures. Deficits in cognition also play an important role in predicting mobility performance. (Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47:65‐71) DOI:10.1167/iovs.05-0582

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Abnormal contrast sensitivity and abnormal color vision can occur independently in HIV-infected individuals and can be present in the absence of severe immunosuppression.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is shown that a subset of cortical neurons in cat area 18 show binocular interactions for the contrast-envelope stimuli, and these neurons are capable of representing a variety of depths in the three-dimensional space based on the information available from contrast cues alone.
Abstract: Humans and animals use visual cues such as brightness and color boundaries to identify objects and navigate through environments. However, even when these cues are not available, we can effortlessly perform these tasks by using second-order cues such as contrast variation (envelope) of patterns on surfaces. Previously, numerous psychophysical studies examined properties of binocular depth processing based on the contrast-envelope cues and suggested the existence of a stereo system that uses these cues. However, its physiological substrate has not been identified yet. Here, we show that a subset of cortical neurons in cat area 18 show binocular interactions for the contrast-envelope stimuli. These neurons are capable of representing a variety of depths in the three-dimensional space based on the information available from contrast cues alone. Furthermore, these neurons show similar disparity-tuning curves for borders defined by both luminance and contrast cues. This cue-invariant tuning is consistent with a linear binocular convergence model for monocular luminance and contrast-envelope processing pathways.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is suggested that for typical luminance settings of current LCDs, it is possible to raise ambient illumination to minimize differences in eye adaptation, potentially reducing visual fatigue while also complying with the TG18 specifications for controlled contrast rendition.
Abstract: Ambient lighting in soft-copy reading rooms is currently kept at low values to preserve contrast rendition in the dark regions of a medical image. Low illuminance levels, however, create inadequate viewing conditions and may also cause eye strain. This eye strain may be potentially attributed to notable variations in the luminance adaptation state of the reader's eyes when moving the gaze intermittently between the brighter display and darker surrounding surfaces. This paper presents a methodology to minimize this variation and optimize the lighting conditions of reading rooms by exploiting the properties of liquid crystal displays (LCDs) with low diffuse reflection coefficients and high luminance ratio. First, a computational model was developed to determine a global luminance adaptation value, Ladp, when viewing a medical image on display. The model is based on the diameter of the pupil size, which depends on the luminance of the observed object. Second, this value was compared with the luminance reflected off surrounding surfaces, Ls, under various conditions of room illuminance, E, different values of diffuse reflection coefficients of surrounding surfaces, Rs, and calibration settings of a typical LCD. The results suggest that for typical luminance settings of current LCDs, it is possible to raise ambient illumination to minimize differences in eye adaptation, potentially reducing visual fatigue while also complying with the TG18 specifications for controlled contrast rendition. Specifically, room illumination in the 75-150 lux range and surface diffuse reflection coefficients in the practical range of 0.13-0.22 sr(-1) provide an ideal setup for typical LCDs. Future LCDs with lower diffuse reflectivity and with higher inherent luminance ratios can provide further improvement of ergonomic viewing conditions in reading rooms.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Subjects wearing GP multifocals, soft bifocal, monovision, and PAL spectacles have good binocular contrast sensitivity, satisfactory binocular low and high contrast acuity, and increased sensitivity to glare.
Abstract: Purpose. The purpose of this study is to assess the visual performance of subjects wearing gas-permeable (GP) multifocal contact lenses, soft bifocal contact lenses, GP monovision lenses and spectacles. Methods. The study included 32 subjects between the ages of 42 and 65 years wearing GP monovision, the Acuvue Bifocal (Vistakon), the Essentials GP Multifocal (Blanchard), and progressive addition lenses (PAL; spectacles group). There were eight subjects in each of these groups who were already wearing these modalities. Binocular low (18%) and high (95%) contrast acuities were recorded using the Bailey-Lovie chart; binocular contrast sensitivity from 1.5 to 18 cycles per degree (cpd) measured with the Vistech VCTS 6500 system, and monocular glare sensitivity at three luminance settings (400, 100, and 12 foot lamberts) was measured using the brightness acuity tester (BAT). Binocular near visual task performance (a modified version of letter counting method used in previous presbyopic studies) was also assessed. Results. For the contact lens-wearing groups, subjects wearing GP multifocals provided the best binocular high and low contrast acuity followed by soft bifocal wearers. There was relative parity between the binocular high and low contrast acuity with PAL and GP multifocal wearers. Monovision acuity, measured binocularly, was determined to be lower than the other three groups with this difference being most significant with high contrast acuity. Among contact lens-wearing groups, it was observed that GP multifocal lens wearers experienced the lowest amount of monocular disability glare followed by soft bifocal wearers and monovision wearers. Subjects wearing soft bifocal lenses and monovision demonstrated slightly reduced binocular contrast sensitivity at all spatial frequencies. In the contact lens groups, GP multifocal lens wearers had the highest binocular contrast sensitivity at all spatial frequencies, on parity with PAL wearers, except at the highest spatial frequency (18 cpd) at which PAL wearers had better vision. Error scores for the binocular near visual task performance between the four groups revealed subjects with GP multifocal lenses and PAL wearers to have the least errors, followed by monovision users and then soft bifocal wearers with the most errors. Conclusion. Subjects wearing GP multifocals, soft bifocals, monovision, and PAL spectacles have good binocular contrast sensitivity, satisfactory binocular low and high contrast acuity, and increased sensitivity to glare. Presbyopic subjects requiring the use of contact lenses under dim light levels could benefit from GP multifocal lenses. Contrast and glare sensitivity evaluations provide significant information regarding the visual performance of the presbyopic contact lenses and should be included in regular presbyopic contact lens fitting. (Optom Vis Sci 2006;83:611–615)

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The blue light-filtering AcrySof Natural (SN60) IOL has postoperative visual performance comparable with that of the Acrysof single-piece (SA60)IOL, and there were no statistically significant differences in colour perception performance between the two groups.
Abstract: Purpose: To compare postoperative best distance visual acuity (VA), contrast sensitivity and colour perception with the blue light-filtering AcrySof Natural (SN60AT) and AcrySof single-piece (SA60AT) intraocular lenses (IOLs). Methods: This was a prospective, randomized, comparative, interventional study comparing postoperative performance between the SN60 and SA60 IOLs. There were nine patients (nine eyes) in the SN60 group and 10 patients (10 eyes) in the SA60 group. All patients were operated using phacoemulsification. Postoperative VA (Snellen chart), contrast sensitivity (Pelli–Robson contrast sensitivity chart) and colour perception (Farnsworth–Munsell D-15 panel test) were measured at 1, 3 and 6 months postoperatively. Results: Postoperative best corrected VA after 6 months was 20/20 or better in 89% of SN60 eyes and 100% of SA60 eyes. Postoperative contrast sensitivity scores improved significantly in both groups under both photopic and mesopic conditions. There were no statistically significant differences in contrast sensitivity scores between the SN60 and SA60 groups at any of the postoperative evaluation time-points. Postoperative colour perception improved significantly in both the SN60 and SA60 groups, and there were no statistically significant differences in colour perception performance between the two groups. Conclusion: The blue light-filtering AcrySof Natural (SN60) IOL has postoperative visual performance comparable with that of the AcrySof single-piece (SA60) IOL.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: A psychophysical experiment was performed in which images of natural scenes under two successive daylights were presented on a computer-controlled high-resolution color monitor and receptor-based rather than colorimetric properties of the images provided a good fit to the effects of scene and of illuminant change on color constancy.
Abstract: To what extent do observers' judgments of surface color with natural scenes depend on global image statistics? To address this question, a psychophysical experiment was performed in which images of natural scenes under two successive daylights were presented on a computer-controlled high-resolution color monitor. Observers reported whether there was a change in reflectance of a test surface in the scene. The scenes were obtained with a hyperspectral imaging system and included variously trees, shrubs, grasses, ferns, flowers, rocks, and buildings. Discrimination performance, quantified on a scale of 0 to 1 with a color-constancy index, varied from 0.69 to 0.97 over 21 scenes and two illuminant changes, from a correlated color temperature of 25,000 K to 6700 K and from 4000 K to 6700 K. The best account of these effects was provided by receptor-based rather than colorimetric properties of the images. Thus, in a linear regression, 43% of the variance in constancy index was explained by the log of the mean relative deviation in spatial cone-excitation ratios evaluated globally across the two images of a scene. A further 20% was explained by including the mean chroma of the first image and its difference from that of the second image and a further 7% by the mean difference in hue. Together, all four global color properties accounted for 70% of the variance and provided a good fit to the effects of scene and of illuminant change on color constancy, and, additionally, of changing test-surface position. By contrast, a spatial-frequency analysis of the images showed that the gradient of the luminance amplitude spectrum accounted for only 5% of the variance.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The degree of residual binocularity and interocular suppression predicts monocular acuity and may be a significant etiological mechanism of vision loss.
Abstract: Objectives To examine deficits in monocular and binocular vision in adults with amblyopia and to test the following 2 hypotheses: (1) Regardless of clinical subtype, the degree of impairment in binocular integration predicts the pattern of monocular acuity deficits. (2) Subjects who lack binocular integration exhibit the most severe interocular suppression. Methods Seven subjects with anisometropia, 6 subjects with strabismus, and 7 control subjects were tested. Monocular tests included Snellen acuity, grating acuity, Vernier acuity, and contrast sensitivity. Binocular tests included Titmus stereo test, binocular motion integration, and dichoptic contrast masking. Results As expected, both groups showed deficits in monocular acuity, with subjects with strabismus showing greater deficits in Vernier acuity. Both amblyopic groups were then characterized according to the degree of residual stereoacuity and binocular motion integration ability, and 67% of subjects with strabismus compared with 29% of subjects with anisometropia were classified as having “nonbinocular” vision according to our criterion. For this nonbinocular group, Vernier acuity is most impaired. In addition, the nonbinocular group showed the most dichoptic contrast masking of the amblyopic eye and the least dichoptic contrast masking of the fellow eye. Conclusion The degree of residual binocularity and interocular suppression predicts monocular acuity and may be a significant etiological mechanism of vision loss.

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An unexpected 92% of clients with SPMD to have visual impairments were found, as well as impaired visual attention, fixation and following, and consequences for caregiving and for modifications of the environment were discussed.
Abstract: Background: The prevalence of visual impairments in people with severe and profound multiple disabilities (SPMD) is the subject of considerable debate and is difficult to assess. Methods: In a typical Dutch care organization, all clients with SPMD (n = 76) participated in the study and specific instruments adapted to these clients (requiring a minimum of cooperation) were used to measure visual acuity, the visual field, binocular vision, contrast sensitivity, refractive errors and visual functioning behaviour. Results: We found an unexpected 92% of clients with SPMD to have visual impairments. Previously, only 30% were known to have visual problems. None of the persons observed had normal visual acuity. Subnormal visual acuity was the best result. The severity of the visual impairment was related to the severity of the intellectual disability. In addition to the problem of acuity, impairments in the visual field, impaired contrast sensibility and impaired binocular functioning were found, as well as impaired visual attention, fixation and following. In 22% of the clients observed, refractive errors were found and glasses were advised. Conclusions: Consequences for caregiving and for modifications of the environment were discussed. © 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.