scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Topic

Non-rapid eye movement sleep

About: Non-rapid eye movement sleep is a research topic. Over the lifetime, 8661 publications have been published within this topic receiving 389465 citations. The topic is also known as: NREM.


Papers
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Significant differences in nocturnal sleep patterns from matched controls initially after infarction included greater wakefulness, low REM sleep per cent, long REM latency, fewer REM periods, more awakenings, more stage shifts and decreased sleep efficiency.

153 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Feb 2008-Sleep
TL;DR: NREM sleep obtained during a daytime nap benefits declarative memory performance, with these benefits being intimately tied to how well subjects acquire the tasks and the way in which the information is acquired.
Abstract: IT IS BECOMING INCREASINGLY CLEAR THAT NREM SLEEP, ESPECIALLY SLOW WAVE SLEEP (SWS), IS IMPORTANT FOR THE PROCESSING OF HIPPOCAMPUS-dependent declarative memories,1–6 with SWS hypothesized to provide the optimal electrophysiological and biochemical state for this type of processing.7,8 When sleep occurs in the form of a short daytime nap, it is very common to obtain only NREM sleep, without entering REM sleep, which would usually occur at least 90 minutes into the sleep period. The few studies examining the effect of daytime naps on memory have made use of this knowledge, demonstrating that daytime naps containing only NREM sleep (including SWS) facilitate verbal declarative memory (semantically related paired associates),9 with one study showing that paired associates improvement is contingent on whether subjects obtained SWS during the nap.10 These findings represent a first step forward in our understanding of how daytime naps benefit declarative memory processing. However, there are many questions still to be explored. To this end, the present study examines the benefits of a daytime NREM nap on a spectrum of declarative memory tasks, and begins to assess the importance of factors related to task acquisition and their potential to modulate sleep-related memory processing. To more broadly assess the declarative memory benefits of NREM sleep obtained during a daytime nap, subjects were trained on 3 well-known declarative (hippocampus-dependent) memory tasks. The first task was an unrelated paired associates task, a more difficult counterpart to the commonly used related paired associates task.3,4 The task comprises pairs of common words that lack an inherent semantic relationship (e.g., shirt–paper). Two nonverbal declarative memory tasks that do not rely strongly on previously learned concepts were also evaluated: the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test (ROCFT; a measure of visuospatial declarative memory) and a maze learning task adapted from the task used by Brenda Milner on a large sample of hippocampal lesion patients including HM.11 Both of these tasks are void of semantically charged landmarks, objects, or verbal material that would have been previously learned by subjects. To date, no studies have used this particular maze learning task, and only 2 have examined the effect of sleep on memory using the ROCFT. In epileptic patients it was shown that performance on the ROCFT correlated positively with low frequency EEG spectral power (<1.25 Hz) overnight,12 and in schizophrenic patients the amount of SWS correlated positively with overnight ROCFT performance.13 In the present study we also explored the extent to which different methods of information encoding modulate the effect of sleep on memory. The impetus for exploring encoding factors was based on two studies by Smith, et al.14,15 who found that only when rats successfully acquired an operant conditioning or passive avoidance task was there an increase in subsequent paradoxical sleep. These findings suggested for the first time that the extent of task acquisition may be an important modulator of the effect of sleep on memory processing. Support for this general finding comes from a recent PET study demonstrating that the strength of acquisition of a serial reaction time task is correlated with increased brain activation (regional cerebral blood flow) during post-acquisition REM sleep.16 Similarly, it was shown that stronger acquisition of a motor adaptation task not only correlates with an increase in slow wave activity (SWA) during subsequent sleep, but this increase in SWA is correlated with enhanced performance following sleep.17 Given these findings, it becomes clear that the individual's success in acquiring a task may be an important factor in understanding how sleep facilitates memory formation. In addition to the assessment of individual differences in acquisition, the level of task acquisition can be experimentally manipulated to assess the preferential effect of sleep for information that is more strongly acquired. A recent study by Schmidt et al.18 has shown that not only does spindle density increase significantly during a daytime nap following the encoding of a difficult (but not an easy) paired associates task, but that this increase in spindle density correlates with improvement in paired associates recall. To assess the importance of task acquisition in modulating sleep-related memory processing, we created 2 encoding conditions within the paired associates task. To date, almost all sleep-dependent consolidation studies have employed a “study-test” paradigm,19 whereby subjects learn a list of word pairs, and then perform cued recall tests until a specified performance criterion is met (e.g., 60% correct or one perfect recall trial2,3,20). However, it is still unclear whether immediate testing leads to enhanced paired associates encoding, which in turn allows sleep to more strongly facilitate memory processing, or whether sleep imparts the same performance benefits to subjects that simply learn the word pairs without immediate testing. Interestingly, in a recent study that did not examine sleep/wake differences it was shown that when subjects were tested immediately after learning declarative information (a text passage), recall after one week was superior to recall of subjects that underwent multiple study sessions without being tested.19 To add to our understanding of the nature of task acquisition and its potential to modulate the effects of sleep on memory, subjects in the present study were immediately tested on a subset of the word pairs during the training session (referred to as “tested” word pairs), while the remaining word pairs were studied without immediate test (referred to as “untested” word pairs). In the present study, performance on all 3 declarative tasks was assessed following a 3.5-h training-retest interval that included a daytime NREM nap or no nap. To test the extent to which task acquisition factors modulate sleep-related memory processing, subjects were not only exposed to 2 modes of paired associates encoding, but for each task subjects were also divided post hoc into high and low performers based on training performance (i.e., those performing in the top and bottom half of the sample based on a median split). This allowed for an analysis of the effect of the subjects' ability to acquire each of the 3 tasks as well as the effect of 2 different modes of paired associates acquisition (tested vs. untested) on sleep-related memory processing.

153 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is found that basal metabolic rate correlates negatively rather than positively with sleep quotas, and that neither adult nor neonatal brain mass correlates positively with REM or NREM sleep times, which contradict hypotheses that invoke energy conservation, cognition, and development as drivers of sleep variation.
Abstract: The amount of time asleep varies greatly in mammals, from 3 h in the donkey to 20 h in the armadillo. Previous comparative studies have suggested several functional explanations for interspecific variation in both the total time spent asleep and in rapid-eye movement (REM) or “quiet” (non-REM) sleep. In support of specific functional benefits of sleep, these studies reported correlations between time in specific sleep states (NREM or REM) and brain size, metabolic rate, and developmental variables. Here we show that estimates of sleep duration are significantly influenced by the laboratory conditions under which data are collected and that, when analyses are limited to data collected under more standardized procedures, traditional functional explanations for interspecific variation in sleep durations are no longer supported. Specifically, we find that basal metabolic rate correlates negatively rather than positively with sleep quotas, and that neither adult nor neonatal brain mass correlates positively with REM or NREM sleep times. These results contradict hypotheses that invoke energy conservation, cognition, and development as drivers of sleep variation. Instead, the negative correlations of both sleep states with basal metabolic rate and diet are consistent with trade-offs between sleep and foraging time. In terms of predation risk, both REM and NREM sleep quotas are reduced when animals sleep in more exposed sites, whereas species that sleep socially sleep less. Together with the fact that REM and NREM sleep quotas correlate strongly with each other, these results suggest that variation in sleep primarily reflects ecological constraints acting on total sleep time, rather than the independent responses of each sleep state to specific selection pressures. We propose that, within this ecological framework, interspecific variation in sleep duration might be compensated by variation in the physiological intensity of sleep.

152 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
Hisashi Okada1, S. Iwase, T. Mano, Y. Sugiyama, T. Watanabe 
TL;DR: The decreases in MSA during nonREM sleep stages may indicate sleep-stage dependent central suppression of MSA activity; increases in M SA during REM sleep suggest instability of the autonomic nervous system; and a common pathway may exist for MSA bursts and K-complexes.
Abstract: We microneurographically recorded muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSA) during sleep in 12 healthy volunteers while simultaneously recording EEC, EOG, ECG, respiration, and blood pressure and determined the number of pulse-synchronous MSA bursts per minute (burst rate) for non-rapid eye movement (nonREM) sleep and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. MSA decreased during nonREM sleep with progressively deeper sleep stages. During REM sleep, the burst rate of MSA increased and was associated with marked fluctuations in arterial blood pressure. During sleep stage 2, MSA bursts occurred approximately 1 second after spontaneous K-complexes. We conclude that (1) the decreases in MSA during nonREM sleep stages may indicate sleep-stage dependent central suppression of MSA activity; (2) increases in MSA during REM sleep suggest instability of the autonomic nervous system; and (3) a common pathway may exist for MSA bursts and K-complexes.

152 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is suggested that the intrinsic tendency of cortical neurons to fall into a down-state after a transient activation (i.e. bistability) prevents the emergence of stable patterns of causal interactions among cortical areas during NREM.

152 citations


Network Information
Related Topics (5)
Prefrontal cortex
24K papers, 1.9M citations
82% related
Dopaminergic
29K papers, 1.4M citations
81% related
Dopamine
45.7K papers, 2.2M citations
80% related
Hippocampal formation
30.6K papers, 1.7M citations
80% related
Hippocampus
34.9K papers, 1.9M citations
80% related
Performance
Metrics
No. of papers in the topic in previous years
YearPapers
2023229
2022453
2021353
2020283
2019315
2018221