scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Topic

Social change

About: Social change is a research topic. Over the lifetime, 61197 publications have been published within this topic receiving 1797013 citations.


Papers
More filters
Book
01 Jan 1990
TL;DR: New social movements as mentioned in this paper are general theories of social movements -functionalism and Marxism sociological responses to the rise of new social movements varieties of ideology within the ecology movement movements and parties.
Abstract: New social movements - major themes general theories of social movements - functionalism and Marxism sociological responses to the rise of new social movements varieties of ideology within the ecology movement movements and parties - problems of organization and mobilization social closure and political participation.

387 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An intervention model is proposed for how schools should enhance the social and emotional learning of students in order to promote resilience and suggestions are offered for how to support implementation of this intervention model at scale.
Abstract: Social-emotional competence is a critical factor to target with universal preventive interventions that are conducted in schools because the construct (a) associates with social, behavioral, and academic outcomes that are important for healthy development; (b) predicts important life outcomes in adulthood; (c) can be improved with feasible and cost-effective interventions; and (d) plays a critical role in the behavior change process. This article reviews this research and what is known about effective intervention approaches. Based on that, an intervention model is proposed for how schools should enhance the social and emotional learning of students in order to promote resilience. Suggestions are also offered for how to support implementation of this intervention model at scale.

384 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
Jerome Kagan1
TL;DR: For example, this paper found that 15% of Caucasian children in the second year of life are consistently shy and emotionally subdued in unfamiliar situations, whereas another 15% are consistently sociable and affectively spontaneous.
Abstract: About 15% of Caucasian children in the second year of life are consistently shy and emotionally subdued in unfamiliar situations, whereas another 15% are consistently sociable and affectively spontaneous. A majority of the children in these two groups retain these profiles through their eighth year. In addition, the two groups differ in physiological qualities that imply differential thresholds in limbic sites, especially the amygdala and the hypothalamus, suggesting that the two temperamental groups are analogous to closely related strains of mammals. However, the behavioral profiles of the children are influenced in a major way by environmental conditions existing during the early years of life. The word temperament is used by most, but not all, behavioral scientists to refer to those psychological qualities that display considerable variation among infants and, in addition, have a relatively, but not indefinitely, stable biological basis in the organism's gcnotype, even though the inherited physiological processes mediate different phenotypic displays as the child grows. It is reasonable to suggest that some of the temperamental differences among children are analogous to the biobehavioral differences among closely related strains of dogs, cats, or monkeys (Adamec & Stark-Adamec, 1986; Clarke, Mason, & Moberg, 1988). The temperamental qualities that are most obvious to contemporary American parents, and that are investigated most often by psychologists, include irritability, smiling, motor activity, and adaptability to new situations. These qualities are popular, in part, because they have implications for the ease with which parents can socialize their infant. It is not clear at the moment how many temperamental qualities will be discovered; it certainly will be more than 6, but hopefully less than 60. We will have to wait for history's answer. Inhibited and Uninhibited Chi ld ren Steven Reznick, Nancy Snidman, and I, together with Cynthia Garcia-Coll, Wendy Coster, Michcle Gersten, and many others in our laboratory, have been studying two categories from the larger set of temperamental qualities (Garcia-Coll, Kagan, & Reznick, 1984; Kagan, Reznick, Clarke, Snidman, & Garcia-Coll, 1984; Kagan, Reznick & Snidman, 1987, 1988; Kagan, Reznick, Snidman, Gibbons, & Johnson, 1988; Reznick et al., 1986). The original behavioral referent for each of the qualities was the response profile of 20to 30-month-old children when they were in unfamiliar situations. Some children consistently become quiet, vigilant, and restrained while they assess the situation and their resources before acting. Others act with spontaneity, as though the distinctions between familiar and novel situations were of minimal psychological consequence. The situations that best reveal these two qualities in young children are encounters with unfamiliar children or adults, perhaps because other people are the most frequent basis for categorizing most settings as unfamiliar. Of course, it is rare to find a large number of children who are consistently shy and affcctively restrained or outgoing and spontaneous regardless of the social context. There is, however, a small group of children (my colleagues and I estimate it to be about 10% to 15%) who usually bring one or the other of these behavioral styles to new situations. We call the shy children inhibited and the sociable children uninhibited. Our current studies of inhibited and uninhibited children trace their beginnings to an early collaboration with Howard Moss, which was summarized in 1962 in the book entitled Birth to Maturity (Kagan & Moss, 1962). A large group of families was participating in the Fels Institute's longitudinal project, which began in the early 1930s. The children in these families were observed from birth to adolescence in their homes, the Institute's nursery school, and their own school settings, and they were tested and interviewed regularly. Moss rated each child on a set of variables for consecutive, chronological epochs, using as evidence the extensive corpus of information available on each subject. I was in another room interviewing these same subjects, who were then in their 20s, and administering a relevant battery of tests, but I was unaware of the early information Moss was reading. It is of interest that the most important discovery of the Fels study was that the only psychological quality preserved from the first three years of life through adulthood was the characteristic we now call behavioral inhibition, although we called it passivity in 1962. Although Moss and I suggested that this predisposition might be a partial function of biological variables, the Zeitgeist during the early 1960s was not prepared to award much formative power to temperamental factors. Unfortunately, our faith in a temperamental interpretation of these data was not suf668 April 1989 • American Psychologist Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0003-066X/89/$00.75 Vol. 44, No. 4, 668-674 ficiently strong, and neither of us pursued this phenomenon. In a later collaboration, Richard Kearsley, Philip Zelazo, and I enrolled Chinese-American and Caucasian infants from similar social class backgrounds in a longitudinal study of the effect of day care across the period from 3 to 29 months of age. We noted in our 1978 monograph, called In fancy (Kagan, Kearsley, & Zeiazo, 1978), that although the effect of day care on the children was minimal, the Chinese infants, whether attending our day care center or raised only at home, were, relative to the Caucasians, more subdued, shy, and fearful when they met unfamiliar adults or children, and they cried more intensely when their mothers left them for a brief separation. In addition, the Chinese children consistently showed more stable heart rates than the Caucasians during the laboratory episodes. This association implied a biological basis for the inhibition among the Chinese children. The unexpected association between shy, timid behavior and a minimally variable heart rate provoked me to pursue this phenomenon more directly. Cynthia Garcia-Coll and Nancy Snidman, in independent dissertation research, selected from large samples of young Caucasian children (aged 21 months for Cohort 1 and 31 months for Cohort 2) those who were either consistently shy and fearful (behaviorally inhibited) or sociable and fearless (uninhibited) when they encountered unfamiliar people or objects in unfamiliar laboratory rooms. They had to screen over 400 children in order to find 54 consistently inhibited and 53 consistently uninhibited children, about 15% of the children screened, with equal numbers of boys and girls in each group. These children have been seen on three additional occasions; at the last assessment at 71/2 years of age, there were 41 children in each of the two cohorts---a loss of about 20% of the original sample. In each of the assessments, the children were observed in different situations. Usually the assessments inc ludeda testing session with a female examiner and, on a different day, a play situation with an unfamiliar child of the same age and sex. At 51/2 years of age the aggregate index included observations of the child's behavior in his or her school setting (Gersten, 1986). Details of the procedures can be found in previously published articles (see Garcia-Coll et al., 1984; Kagan et al., 1988, Reznick et Editor's note. This article was originally presented as a Distinguished Scientific Contributions award address at the meeting of the American Psychological Association in Atlanta in August 1988. Award-based manuscripts appearing in the American Psychologist are scholarly articles based in part on earlier award addresses presented at the APA convention. In keeping with the policy of recognizing these distinguished contributors to the field, these submi.~sioas are given special consideration in the editorial selection process. /a~thor's note. The research for this article was SUPl~rted by the John D. and Catherine 1\". MacArthur Foundation. I thank J. Steven Reznick, Nancy Suidman, Jane Gibbons, and Maureen O. Johnson for their contributions. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jerome g~a~ , Delgtrtment of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland St., Cambridge, MA 02138. al., 1986; and Snidman, 1984). We computed aggregate indexes of inhibition at each age, based on the child's tendency to be quiet, shy, and emotionally subdued in each of the different contexts. The indexes of inhibition at 71/2 years were based on behavior in two laboratory situations. The first was a play situation involving 7 to 10 unfamiliar children of the same age and sex. The two critical variables were number of spontaneous comments to the other children or supervising adults and proportion of time spent standing or playing apart from any other child in the room during the free-play intervals. The second assessment context was an individual testing session with an unfamiliar female examiner who did not know the child's prior status. The two critical variables were latency to the sixth spontaneous comment to the examiner and the total number of spontaneous comments over the 90-minute session. The aggregate index of inhibition represented the average standard scores for the indexes from the two assessment situations. The intercoder reliabilities for these behavioral variables coded from videotapes were above 0.90. Preservation of Behavior There was moderate but significant preservation of the inhibited and uninhibited behavioral styles from the first assessments, at either 21 or 31 months, through 71/2 years of age. The correlation between the original index of inhibition (21 months for Cohort 1 and 31 months for Cohort 2) and the aggregate index at 71/2 years was .67 (p < .001) for Cohort 1 and .39 (p < .01) for Cohort 2. About three fourths of the children in each cohort retained their expecte

384 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The right to learn is undoubtably the most fundamental civil rights for which the world has struggled and fought for 5,000 years, and whatever we may think of the curtailment of other civil rights, we should fight to the last ditch to keep open this right as discussed by the authors.
Abstract: Of all the civil rights for which the world has struggled and fought for 5,000 years, the right to learn is undoubt. edly the most fundamental.... The freedom to learn .. . has been bought by bitter sacrifice. And whatever we may think of the curtailment of other civil rights, we should fight to the last ditch to keep open the right to learn, the right to have examined in our schools not only what we believe, but what we do not believe; not only what our leaders say, but what the leaders of other groups and nations, and the leaders of other centuries have said. We must insist upon this to give our children the fairness of a start which will equip them with such an array of facts and such an attitude toward truth that they can have a real chance to judge what the world is and what its greater minds have thought it might be. (pp. 230-231)

383 citations


Network Information
Related Topics (5)
Politics
263.7K papers, 5.3M citations
89% related
Democracy
108.6K papers, 2.3M citations
87% related
Social relation
29.1K papers, 1.7M citations
85% related
Globalization
81.8K papers, 1.7M citations
84% related
Government
141K papers, 1.9M citations
83% related
Performance
Metrics
No. of papers in the topic in previous years
YearPapers
2023115
2022303
20211,155
20201,678
20191,734
20181,858