scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question

Showing papers by "Diego A. Pizzagalli published in 2005"


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Objective assessments of participants' propensity to modulate behavior as a function of reward might provide a powerful tool for improving the phenotypic definition of depression and thus offer a reliable behavioral screening approach for neuroscience studies of depression.

577 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: These findings not only confirm that frontal EEG asymmetry modulates the propensity to engage in appetitively motivated behavior, but also provide anatomical details about the underlying brain systems.
Abstract: The influence of approach and avoidance tendencies on affect, reasoning, and behavior has attrac- ted substantial interest from researchers across various areas of psychology. Currently, frontal electroencephalo- graphic (EEG) asymmetry in favor of left prefrontal re- gions is assumed to reflect the propensity to respond with approach-related tendencies. To test this hypothesis, we recorded resting EEG in 18 subjects, who separately performed a verbal memory task under three incentive conditions (neutral, reward, and punishment). Using a source-localization technique, we found that higher task- independent alpha2 (10.5-12 Hz) activity within left dorsolateral prefrontal and medial orbitofrontal regions was associated with stronger bias to respond to reward- related cues. Left prefrontal resting activity accounted for 54.8% of the variance in reward bias. These findings not only confirm that frontal EEG asymmetry modulates the propensity to engage in appetitively motivated behavior, but also provide anatomical details about the underlying brain systems.

323 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Dec 2005-Emotion
TL;DR: Worrying was associated with relatively greater left frontal activity compared with anticipation and Trait public speaking anxiety was positively correlated withleft frontal activity during worrying.
Abstract: To study the psychophysiological correlates of worrying, the authors recorded heart rate, respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), skin conductance level, and alpha electroencephalographic asymmetry in healthy males during baseline, relaxation, worry induction, and anticipation of an impromptu speech task. Compared with baseline, relaxation, and anticipation, worrying was associated with greater heart rate and lower RSA. Worrying was further characterized by higher skin conductance levels compared with baseline but lower levels than during anticipation. Finally, worrying was associated with relatively greater left frontal activity compared with anticipation. Trait public speaking anxiety was positively correlated with left frontal activity during worrying. These results support the notion that worrying is a unique emotional state that is different from fearful anticipation.

159 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Panksepp et al. as discussed by the authors use selected neurochemical systems to discuss processes as complex as affiliation with the proviso that they only approximate the complexity of the underlying causal issues.
Abstract: the way to future human trials (Panksepp & Harro 2004). But what are the best animal models? With the amount of evolutionary diversification that exists, one must select model systems carefully (Panksepp et al. 1992; 2002), and D&M-S wisely prioritized primate data over rodent data. Dogs are also an excellent species (Panksepp et al. 1978). Certainly lab rats are not optimal for understanding separation-distress arising from severing specific social bonds. These excellent \" test-tube \" creatures thrive when housed alone in sterile environments , perhaps because their separation-distress systems are ves-tigial (Panksepp 2003). Selective opioid regulation of social distress is well documented in many species (Panksepp 1998), but is dubious in rats (Winslow & Insel 1991a). Perhaps because of this, they are excellent species for studying the affiliative energies of play and low-dose opioid facilitation of social interactions (Panksepp & Bishop 1981; Panksepp et al. 1985). As D&M-S recognize, the use of selected neurochemical systems to discuss processes as complex as affiliation needs to be advanced with the proviso that they only approximate the complexity of the underlying causal issues. If we try to extrapolate general neu-rochemical principles to excessively fine-circuit and synaptic levels , we may be encouraging a radical reductionism that is wrong (Bennett & Hacker 2003). Obviously, social attachments and affiliations are fully \" embodied \" within brain, body, and environment. Important fusion points between levels of analysis must not be construed as explanations. But, as this target article exemplifies, meaningful visions of the larger picture cannot deny nor should they shy away from investigations at finer layers of explanation. Continued attempts to stitch neuroscience details from animal studies into coherent, testable hypotheses at more molar, human levels are vital intellectual initiatives, as long as we recognize that the emergent neuropsychology of human beings lies at the root of our social dilemmas, not merely the biochemical mechanisms that underlie the electrical properties of subsets of neurons in limited brain regions. Ultimately, affiliation is an emergent property of being a mammal. Attachment does not simply exist in the brain, but in brains' interrelations with bodies and environments. Analysis of neuro-chemistries of brain/mind states that correlate in some way with affiliation is a most reasonable empirical way to proceed, especially if we carefully strip away erroneous philosophical assumptions , as well as potentially irrelevant fine details. If we do that well, translations between levels can be advanced at a …

1 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: In this article, the authors discuss the role of neuroscience in shaping new conceptualizations of emotion and the putative role of theta oscillation within frontocingulate pathways in depression, a syndrome in which emotioncognition relations are dysfunctional.
Abstract: By emphasizing nonlinear dynamics between appraisal and emotions, Lewis’s model provides a valuable platform for integrating psychological and neural perspectives on the emotion-cognition interface. In this commentary, I discuss the role of neuroscience in shaping new conceptualizations of emotion and the putative role of theta oscillation within frontocingulate pathways in depression, a syndrome in which emotioncognition relations are dysfunctional. In the target article, Lewis provides a wide-ranging and timely theoretical formulation of emotion-cognition relations. By emphasizing (a) bidirectional interactions between appraisal and emotion; (b) lower-order psychological and neural constituents underlying the emergence of emotion–appraisal processes; and (c) large-scale functional coupling through oscillatory neurophysiological mechanisms, Lewis offers a multilevel account of appraisal-emotion interactions, fostering a better integration of emotion theory and neurobiology. In this commentary, I elaborate on two important points raised in the target article. First, I emphasize how a brain-based approach to emotion and appraisal can uniquely inform and constrain theoretical models of these complex constructs. Second, I comment on Lewis’s assertion that “phase synchrony in the theta range may underpin the functional integration of systems mediating appraisal–emotion processes” (sect. 5.4). To this end, I review recent event-related potential (ERP) findings of action monitoring (Luu et al. 2004) and electroencephalographic (EEG) findings highlighting disrupted functional connectivity within frontocingulate pathways in depression (Pizzagalli et al. 2003a). With respect to brain-based approaches to emotion and appraisal, Lewis discusses definitional problems that have hindered the development of comprehensive theories of emotion. Here, I would like to emphasize two points. First, as Lewis argues, definitions of “appraisal” and “emotion” often overlap substantially, causing formidable conundrums to theoretical approaches based on the assumption that these two constructs have distinct functions and are governed by simple, linear, and unidirectional causal processes (e.g., appraisal as a temporal and causal antecedent of emotion; Roseman & Smith 2001). Second, and more important, the definitional overlap between emotion and appraisal mirrors substantial anatomical and functional overlap among brain regions subserving affective and cognitive processes (see Davidson 2003b, for an extended discussion). That is, many brain regions subserving appraisal processes also participate in emotional functions, and vice versa. This evidence forcefully contradicts assertions that affect and cognition are subserved by separate and independent neural circuits, and speaks against the notion that affect and appraisal are subcortically and cortically mediated, respectively (e.g., Panksepp 2003). As suggested by Lewis and others (e.g., Davidson 2003b; Pizzagalli et al. 2003b), emotion is not a monolithic process but comprises different subcomponents encompassing a distributed network of cortical and subcortical systems. Acknowledging empirical data consistent with this assertion (Phan et al. 2002) has important theoretical consequences, because, as appropriately stated by Lewis, “brain function prohibits any real independence between appraisal and emotion” (sect. 5). In sum, although Lewis’s overview of neural substrates underlying appraisal and emotional processes is neither comprehensive nor new, a reconceptualization of these substrates in terms of dynamic systems is indeed useful for stressing that the brain’s anatomy places important constraints upon psychological theories of emotion and its relations to cognition. Emerging brain-based approaches to the study of depression have similarly underscored not only the synergy between emotional and appraisal processes, but also the utility of a neurobiological framework to parsing the clinical heterogeneity of the disorder (Davidson et al. 2002; Pizzagalli et al. 2004). My second set of comments pertains to the hypothesis that phase synchrony in the theta range may play a critical role in the functional integration of appraisal–emotion processes. Specifically, Lewis predicts that theta synchronization across the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate (ACC), orbitofrontal (OFC), and prefrontal (PFC) cortices may “underpin the functional integration of systems mediating appraisal–emotion processes” (sect. 5.4). In humans, empirical evidence for this hypothesis is very limited, but recent findings provide promising support. First, a recent ERP study has shown that the error-related negativity (ERN) – an ERP peak occurring 50–100 msec after the commission of an error – was largely explained by transient phase-locking of midline theta activity to the error responses within distinct frontocingulate regions (Luu et al. 2004). This finding replicated and extended a prior report that error monitoring and evaluative feedback engaged dorsal and rostral ACC sources oscillating within the theta range (Luu et al. 2003). As Luu et al. (2003) proposed, these findings indicate that action regulation mediated by the ACC is associated with entrainment of frontocingulate pathways, consistent with the general framework of Lewis’s model. A second, albeit more indirect, line of evidence suggesting that large-scale corticolimbic synchronization is crucially involved in the emergence of emotion-appraisal processes can be derived from recent findings in major depression, a clinical condition in which coordination of these states is dysfunctional (Mineka et al. 2003). In a recent study, Pizzagalli et al. (2003a) found that baseline theta activity within ACC and PFC/OFC regions was functionally coupled for control, but not depressed, subjects. In healthy controls, this functional connectivity within frontocingulate pathways is in line with anatomical data suggesting that the ACC has reciprocal connections with the dorsolateral PFC and OFC (Barbas 1992; Petrides & Pandya 1999). Disrupted functional connectivity within frontocingulate networks in depression is intriguing, particularly in light of evidence reviewed in the target article and elsewhere (Bush et al. 2000) indicating that the ACC is critically implicated in monitoring conflicting response deCommentary/Lewis: Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling 214 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2005) 28:2 mands, detecting errors, and evaluating the emotional significance of events, and may thus be a site of convergence and integration between affective and cognitive processes. The fact that functional connectivity within frontocingulate pathways emerged for the theta band (6.5–8 Hz) is consistent with the hypothesis that theta may serve a gating function for the information processing flow in corticolimbic limbic regions (Vinogradova 1995; Luu et al. 2003; 2004), thereby providing the necessary neurophysiological substrates for the emergence of adaptive emotionappraisal processes, as Lewis discusses. In sum, using a theoretical framework inspired by emerging neurobiological concepts and findings, Lewis proposes a reconceptualization of emotion-cognition relations that emphasizes nonlinear interactions between their psychological and neural constituents, ultimately giving rise to a unitary phenomenon. Large-scale corticolimbic theta synchronization is proposed as a putative neurophysiological substrate giving rise to a coordinated integration of emotion and cognition. Because the strength of any theoretical account lies mainly in its predictive validity, empirical work is now needed to test hypotheses derivable from this model, including its extension to psychopathology.

1 citations