scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question

Showing papers by "Gregory McCarthy published in 2001"


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Despite having average HDR amplitudes similar to those of younger subjects, older subjects had higher noise levels in activated voxels, resulting in lower signal-to-noise ratios and distribution analyses of voxel statistics revealed that Older subjects had proportionally fewer small-effect-size voxELs, due to their increased voxalwise noise.

292 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Neural changes associated with face and eye processing were investigated developmentally using ERPs (N170), in 128 subjects (4–15 year olds and adults), and N170 was present in the youngest children with similar patterns of face sensitivity seen in adults.
Abstract: Faces and eyes are critical social stimuli which adults process with ease, but how this expertise develops is not yet understood. Neural changes associated with face and eye processing were investigated developmentally using ERPs (N170), in 128 subjects (4-15 year olds and adults). Stimuli included upright faces to assess configural processing, eyes and inverted faces to assess feature-based processing. N170 was present in the youngest children with similar patterns of face sensitivity seen in adults. Development of N170 to upright faces continued until adulthood, suggesting slow maturation of configural processing. In contrast, N170 was shorter latency and much larger to eyes than faces in children and was mature by 11 years, suggesting the early presence of an eye detector, with a rapid maturational course.

276 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: It is concluded that characteristics of the HDR, notably its amplitude, latency, and refractory properties, differ across visual cortical areas.

96 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: There was an exponential relation between number of trials and spatial extent, such that additional trials identified, on average, a constant proportion of the remaining voxels, and the variability of the estimated hemodynamic response decreased with signal averaging.
Abstract: We examined effects of trial averaging upon spatial extent, spatial topography, and temporal properties of fMRI activation. Two subjects participated in an event-related visual stimulation design. There was an exponential relation between number of trials and spatial extent, such that additional trials identified, on average, a constant proportion of the remaining voxels. At values typical of fMRI experimentation (e.g. 50 trials) only about 50% of eventually active voxels were significant; asymptotic values were approached by 150 trials. The variability of the estimated hemodynamic response decreased with signal averaging, becoming stable across samples of > 25 trials. Therefore, group or condition differences may result from differences in voxelwise noise exacerbated by averaging small numbers of trials. NeuroReport 12:2411‐2416 & 2001 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

85 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The results suggest that studies of change blindness can inform understanding of more general attentional processing, and that detection of change, when transient visual cues are not present, requires activation of extrastriate visual regions and frontal regions responsible for eye movements.
Abstract: We investigated using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) the neural processes associated with performance of a change-detection task. In this task, two versions of the same picture are presented in alternation, separated by a brief mask interval. Even when the two pictures greatly differ (e.g., as when a building is in different locations), subjects report that identification of the change is difficult and often take 30 or more seconds to identify the change. This phenomenon of "change blindness" provides a powerful and novel paradigm for segregating components of visual attention using fMRI that can otherwise be confounded in short-duration tasks. By using a response-contingent event-related analysis technique, we successfully dissociated brain regions associated with different processing components of a visual change-detection task. Activation in the calcarine cortex was associated with task onset, but did not vary with the duration of visual search. In contrast, the pattern of activation in dorsal and ventral visual areas was temporally associated with the duration of visual search. As such, our results support a distinction between brain regions whose activation is modulated by attentional demands of the visual task (extrastriate cortex) and those that are not affected by it (primary visual cortex). A second network of areas including central sulcus, insular, and inferior frontal cortical areas, along with the thalamus and basal ganglia, showed phasic activation tied to the execution of responses. Finally, parietal and frontal regions showed systematic deactivations during task performance, consistent with previous reports that these regions may be associated with nontask semantic processing. We conclude that detection of change, when transient visual cues are not present, requires activation of extrastriate visual regions and frontal regions responsible for eye movements. These results suggest that studies of change blindness can inform understanding of more general attentional processing.

84 citations


Journal Article
TL;DR: The findings suggest differences in cognitive appraisal and involvement of a broader network of brain regions mediating emotion during remembrance of emotional words in women compared with men.
Abstract: Studies suggest that men and women have important differences in specific cognitive functions. Men show superior spatial memory and women demonstrate superior verbal memory, and women rely on emotional content to a greater degree in the processing of information. In spite of extensive research in neural correlates of human cognition, little is known about possible gender differences or the role of emotional content in the mediation of cognition. Two sets of lists of word pairs were developed, one with neutral (e.g., school-grocery) and the other with emotional (e.g., mutilate-beat) content. Male and female subjects were asked to rate emotions related to the words on several dimensions (e.g., nervous, fearful, happy). In a second experiment, men and women underwent positron emission tomographic (PET) measurement of brain blood flow during retrieval of word pairs. Words in the "emotional" category were rated more highly on the emotional dimensions, and women rated them as having more emotional impact than did the men. During retrieval of emotional words (but not neutral words) there was a different pattern of activation among the women compared with the men, with greater activation in bilateral posterior hippocampus and cerebellum, and decreased activity in medial prefrontal cortex, which are brain areas previously implicated in emotion. There were no significant differences in retrieval of emotional versus neutral words, or in differences in memory performance between men and women. The findings suggest differences in cognitive appraisal and involvement of a broader network of brain regions mediating emotion during remembrance of emotional words in women compared with men.

72 citations


Book ChapterDOI
18 Jun 2001
TL;DR: A fast algorithm, based on a wavelet representation of the data yields detrended time-series, which can infer and remove drifts that cannot be adequately represented with low degree polynomials.
Abstract: This work provides a new method to estimate and remove baseline drifts in the fMRI signal. The baseline drift in each time series is described as a superposition of physical and physiological phenomena that occur at different scales. A fast algorithm, based on a wavelet representation of the data yields detrended time-series. Experiments with fMRI data demonstrate that our detrending technique can infer and remove drifts that cannot be adequately represented with low degree polynomials. Our detrending technique resulted in a noticeable improvement by reducing the number of false positive and the number of false negative.

4 citations