scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question

Showing papers in "Canadian Psychology in 1988"


Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Rennie et al. as mentioned in this paper discussed the challenges and limitations of grounded theory in psychotherapy process research and illustrated the application of grounded approach in terms of its application to psychotherapy processes.
Abstract: There is a growing body of opinion that psychology suffers from an elaborate research technology that overemphasizes theory verification and impairs thinking and discovery. Grounded theory is advanced as an approach to research that can address this crisis of method in psychology. The grounded approach is described and illustrated in terms of its application to psychotherapy process research. The emphasis on theory creation characterizing the approach is examined within the history of induction. The challenges to and limitations of grounded theory are discussed. There are growing indications that psychology as a discipline is undergoing a crisis of confidence about its research methods. These rumblings of discontent were felt two decades ago when Bakan (1967) characterized psychologists as playing at science in much the way children play at being cowboys, imitating every aspect of cowboy life except the one which is most central — taking care of cows. In much the same way, Bakan contended, psychologists have been concerned with developing an elaborate research technology while ignoring the main work of science — thinking and discovery. The fault does not lie entirely with contemporary psychologists. It has been pointed out that psychology imitated physics with its emphasis on hypothetico-deductive research while in retrospect the fledgling science might have found astronomy or zoology, with their emphasis on Research leading to this paper was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Grants 451-83-3642 and 410-83-1264 to David Rennie. We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of D. Bakan, G. Cupchik, K. Danziger, R. Goranson, L. Heshusius, R. Kroger, A. Mahrer, J. Martin, S. Mor, A. Shaul, I. Silverman, J. Weiser, M. Westcott, and L. Wood who commented on earlier drafts of the paper. Reprint requests may be sent to David L. Rennie, Department of Psychology, York University, North York, Ontario, M3J 1P3. description, more appropriate models (Endler, 1984; Kendler, 1986). As a result, the ensuing technology has increasingly dictated the kinds of studies that are done and has made theorizing less common and less respectable than energetic but trivial research (Bakan, 1967; Brandt, 1982; Endler, 1984; Gergen, 1982; Koch, 1981; Secord, 1982; Silverman, 1977). Many theories that have been developed have been tested by disjointed investigations of irrelevant hypotheses (Arthur, 1983). Even when the hypotheses of studies have been more closely tied to theories, testing has been contaminated by research participants responding to their own inner hypotheses about the experiments in which they are engaged (Orne, 1962; Silverman, 1977). Furthermore, speculation that investigators themselves are incapable of shedding biases despite the most rigorous experimental approaches (Kuhn, 1970; Polanyi, 1969) has received empirical support

500 citations






Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: This article argued that the way we teach, research and practice organizational psychology, and teach others to do so, is based on the erroneous assumption that we exist in non-unionised environments.
Abstract: It is argued that the way we teach, research and practise organizational psychology, and teach others to do so, is based on the erroneous assumption that we exist in non-unionised environments. The influence of union membership on behaviour in organizations is illustrated, the neglect of industrial relations by organizational psychologists documented, and the need for attitudinal, behavioural and ideological change by organizational psychologists is identified. During the 1950s, industrial/organizational (I/O) psychologists frequently researched industrial relations (IR) issues, such as simultaneous loyalty to company and union (e.g., Dean, 1954; Purcell, 1954). Thereafter, the 1950s were called the "golden decade" of psychological research into unions (Straus, 1977). Since then, the reciprocal relationship between I/O psychologists and unionists has been characterized at best by neglect, at worst by mutual mistrust, suspicion and ideological differences. Numerous reasons for this situation have been documented elsewhere (e.g. Huszczo, Wiggins & Currie, 1984), and include ideological and methodological differences between the study of I/O psychology and IR, and will not be repeated here. Rather, it will be argued that avoidance of studying the effects of unions as a central aspect of industrial relations psychology results in the construction and dissemination of a truncated body of knowledge regarding organizational psychology.

25 citations



Journal ArticleDOI

20 citations