scispace - formally typeset
Open AccessJournal ArticleDOI

Social Origins of Self-Regulatory Competence

Dale H. Schunk, +1 more
- 01 Sep 1997 - 
- Vol. 32, Iss: 4, pp 195-208
Reads0
Chats0
TLDR
The authors reviewed the social origins of students' development of self-regulatory skill with special emphasis on observational learning through modeling, and presented a social cognitive perspective on self-regulation, and found that students' academic competence develops initially from social sources of academic skill and subsequently shifts to self sources in a series of four levels: observational, imitative, self-controlled, and self-regulated.
Abstract
This article reviews the social origins of students' development of self-regulatory skill with special emphasis on observational learning through modeling A social cognitive perspective on self-regulation is presented In this view, students' academic competence develops initially from social sources of academic skill and subsequently shifts to self sources in a series of 4 levels: observational, imitative, self-controlled, and self-regulated The effects of models on observers depend in part on perceptions of self-efficacy, or beliefs about one's capabilities to learn or perform designated behaviors Research on social influences is reviewed, and includes factors such as cognitive modeling, coping and mastery models, self-modeling, learning goals, and progress feedback Related theoretical perspectives are discussed along with suggestions for future research

read more

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

Social Origins of Self-Regulatory Competence
By: Dale H. Schunk and Barry J. Zimmerman
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1997). Social origins of self-regulatory competence. Educational
Psychologist, 32, 195-208.
Made available courtesy of Taylor and Francis: http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/
***Reprinted with permission. No further reproduction is authorized without written permission from
Taylor and Francis. This version of the document is not the version of record. Figures and/or pictures
may be missing from this format of the document.***
Abstract:
This article reviews the social origins of students' development of self-regulatory skill with special emphasis on
observational learning through modeling. A social cognitive perspective on self-regulation is presented. In this
view, students' academic competence develops initially from social sources of academic skill and subsequently
shifts to self sources in a series of 4 levels: observational, imitative, self-controlled, and self-regulated. The
effects of models on observers depend in part on perceptions of self-efficacy, or beliefs about one's capabilities
to learn or perform designated behaviors. Research on social influences is reviewed, and includes factors such
as cognitive modeling, coping and mastery models, self-modeling, learning goals, and progress feedback.
Related theoretical perspectives are discussed along with suggestions for future research.
Article:
Successful adaptation to school requires that students develop self-regulation, or processes that activate and
sustain cognitions, behaviors, and affects, and that are oriented toward goal attainment (Zimmerman, 1989,
1990). Academic self-regulatory processes include planning and managing time; attending to and concentrating
on instruction; organizing, rehearsing, and coding information strategically; establishing a productive work
environment; and using social resources effectively (Kanfer & Kanfer, 1991; Karoly, 1993; Pressley et al.,
1990; Zimmerman, 1994). Self-regulation also incorporates motivational processes such as setting performance
goals and outcomes; holding positive beliefs about one's capabilities; valuing learning and its anticipated
outcomes; and experiencing positive affects (e.g., pride, satisfaction) with one's efforts (McCombs, 1989;
Schunk, 1994).
The development of self-regulation is affected by many factors, but an important set comprises socialization
influences. Several years ago researchers hypothesized that children' s exposure to socializing agents (e.g.,
models) influences their behavioral and cognitive development to include the acquisition of concepts, attitudes,
preferences, and standards for self-reward and self-punishment (Bandura & Walters, 1963; Hartup, 1978;
Mischel, 1968). Much research shows that children readily induce and transfer concepts that underlie modeling
sequences (Bandura, 1986; Rosenthal & Zimmerman, 1978; Zimmerman & Rosenthal, 1974).
In recent years, cognitive theorists have shifted their attention to the process whereby self-regulatory
competence is internalized and have studied how children arid adolescents learn to function independently from
socializing agents in an adaptive, generative, and creative manner (Bandura, 1986; Como, 1989; Fuson, 1979;
Kopp, 1982; Mithaug, 1993; Paris & Newman, 1990; Vygotsky, 1978; Wertsch, 1979; Zimmerman, in press).
In this article we identify and review research on the social origins of students' development of self-regulatory
skill with special emphasis on observational learning through modeling. Modeling occurs when observers
pattern their behaviors, strategies, thoughts, beliefs, and affects after those of one or more models (Schunk,
1987). Recent research has demonstrated the effectiveness of modeling self-regulatory skills as a means of
promoting students' academic achievement and associated self-efficacy beliefs (Schunk & Zimmerman, 1994).
Researchers hypothesize that self-efficacy, or personal beliefs about one's capabilities to learn or perform

behaviors at designated levels (Bandura, 1986, 1997), is an important cognitive mechanism that mediates the
relation of social (observational) influences and adaptive self-regulatory functioning (Schenk, 1994).
We initially present a theoretical overview of self-regulation from the particular perspective of social cognitive
theory. We focus on the roles played by two constructs that are intimately linked with this theoretical
perspective and that have been rigorously investigated by researchers in this traditionmodeling and self-
efficacy. Although these constructs have been discussed in the context of other theories (Kopp, 1982; Paris &
Newman, 1990), a unique contribution of social cognitive theory is that it highlights their contributions by
explicating them in detail to include careful empirical research methods. We then describe a social cognitive
phase model of the development of self-regulatory competence, and we compare that perspective to some other
prominent views relating to the development of self-regulation. Research on modeling and self-efficacy
processes is summarized, and we conclude with suggestions for future research in educational settings.
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
Social Cognitive Theory of Self-Regulation
Bandura's (1986) social cognitive theory views human functioning as a series of reciprocal interactions between
behavioral, environmental, and personal variables (e.g., cognitions, affects). For example, research shows that
self-efficacy beliefs (personal variable) influence achievement behaviors (choice of tasks, effort, persistence) in
that efficacious students are more likely to choose to engage in tasks, expend effort, and persist to overcome
obstacles and succeed (Schunk, 1996a; Zimmerman, 1995b). Conversely, behaviors influence personal
variables. As students work on tasks (behavior) they mentally note their progress (personal variable), which
conveys to them that they are capable of learning, thereby raising their self-efficacy (Schunk, 1989).
An example of the influence of environment on behavior occurs when teachers introduce an unusual stimulus or
novel event (environmental variable) and students direct their attention toward it (behavior). Behavior can affect
environment. For example, if students act puzzled by a teacher's explanation (behavior), the teacher may reteach
the material (environmental variable).
Personal variables and environments also affect one another. For example, when students with high self-
efficacy find themselves trying to accomplish an academic task in an environment full of distractions (e.g.,
noise, others working), they may redouble their mental concentration (personal variable) to make the
environment less distracting. Related views of self-regulation discuss this type of personal influence under the
headings of volitional control (Como & Kanfer, 1993) and proximal resource allocation (Kanfer & Ackerman,
1989). The influence of environmental variables on personal variables is seen when teachers give students
verbal feedback (environmental variable; e.g., "That's right. You're really getting good at this."), which raises
their self-efficacya personal variable.
Social cognitive theory postulates that the self-regulation process comprises three major levels (subprocesses):
self-ob servation, self-judgment, and self-reaction (Bandura, 1986; Kanfer & Gaelick, 1986). Self-observation
refers to deliberate attention to specific aspects of one's behavior (Bandura, 1986). Bandura and others (e.g.,
Mace, Belfiore, & Shea, 1989) recommend assessing behaviors on dimensions such as quantity, quality, rate,
and originality. When self-observation results in perceptions of goal progress, it can motivate one to improve
(Schunk, 1989). Students with academic problems often are surprised to learn that they waste much valuable
study time on nonacademic tasks. Such knowledge can motivate students to improve their habits. Self-
observation is assisted with the use of self-recording, where instances of behavior are recorded along with their
time, place, and frequency of occurrence (Mace et al., 1989).
Self-observation is closely linked to self-judgment, which refers to comparing current performance with a
standard. Bandura (1986) places much emphasis on judgmental processes and on specifying factors affecting
judgments, which has facilitated empirical investigation. Thus, self-judgments are hypothesized to be affected
by type and importance of standards employed. Standards may be stated in absolute or normative terms.
Absolute standards are fixed (e.g., a student who attempts to finish an assignment during a class period),

whereas normative standards are based on the performances of others (e.g., a student who attempts to be the
first one in the class to finish an assignment). Standards often are acquired by observing models (Bandura,
1986); socially comparing one's perfornninces with those of others helps one evaluate the appropriateness of
behavior. Social comparisons are used in the self-judgment process when absolute standards are not in effect or
are unclear (Schunk, 1996a).
Self-judgments are also affected by the importance and informativeness of standards. People are more likely to
judge their task progress for tasks they value. They may not assess their performance or expend effort to
improve their skills for tasks in which they have little interest in how they perform.
Comparing one's performance against standards provides information about progress. Students who must learn
30 new spelling words in a week know they are ahead of schedule if they learn 10 words the first day.
Self reaction involves making evaluative responses to judgments of one's performance; for example, whether it
is good or bad, acceptable or not acceptable, beyond or below expectation. These evaluative reactions constitute
a critical aspect of self-regulation and represent a unique contribution of social cognitive theory (Bandura,
1986). Evaluative reactions involve students' beliefs about their progress. The belief that one is making
acceptable progress toward a goal, along with the expected satisfaction of goal attainment, enhances self-
efficacy and sustains motivation (Schunk, 1996a). Negative evaluations will not decrease motivation if students
believe they are capable of improving (e.g., by working harder or using more effective strategies). Motivation is
not enhanced if students think they lack the capability to succeed and that increased effort or better use of
strategies will not help (Schunk, 1994). Self-reactions can be influenced by tangible self-rewards, which
validate perceptions of progress and raise self-efficacy when they are linked to actual accomplishments. For
example, students who believe they are improving their study routine may reward themselves by taking a break
to watch a movie.
These three self-regulatory subprocesses interact with one another. As students observe their own performances,
they judge them against goal standards and react to those judgments. Their evaluations and reactions set the
stage for additional observations. These subprocesses also interact with environmental factors (Zimmerman,
1989). Students who judge their task progress as inadequate may react by requesting teacher assistance. Teach-
ers may help students learn a better strategy, which students then use to produce better learning. This dynamic
interaction of aspects of self-regulation is one of its central features (Schunk & Zimmerman, 1994).
Modeling Processes
Modeling often is an antecedent of self-regulation. An important contribution of social cognitive theory is to
show that modeling can serve different imitative functions: inhibition/disinhibition, response facilitation, and
observational learning (Bandura, 1986). Inhibition/disinhibition refers to the strengthening or weakening of
behavioral inhibitions that occurs as a result of observing models. For example, students who are afraid of math
and observe models engage in math activities without negative consequences may experience less fear and try
the activities themselves (Zimmerman & Kindsler, 1979). Conversely, students who observe peers punished for
classroom misbehavior may be less likely to misbehave.
Response facilitation occurs when modeled actions serve to socially prompt behavior by observers. New
students in a class are likely to follow along and perform actions comparable to those of classmates as a means
of learning the rules and routines. Modeling even has been shown to induce greater creative fluency among
observing students (Zimmerman & Dialessi, 1973).
Observational learning through modeling occurs when observers display new behaviors that prior to modeling
had no probability of occurrence, even with motivational inducements in effect (Bandura, 1986; Schunk, 1987;
Zimmerman & Rosenthal, 1974). To learn observationally, students must attend to a model, code the
information for retention, be capable of producing the demonstrated responses, and be motivated to perform the
modeled behaviors (Bandura, 1986). An important form of observational learning occurs through cognitive

modeling, which incorporates modeled explanations and demonstrations with verbalizations of the model's
thoughts and reasons for performing the actions (Meichenbaum, 1977).
Exposure to models is informative. Observer modeling is strongly affected by the functional value of behav-
iorwhether modeled behaviors result in success or failure, reward or punishment. Modeled behaviors that lead
to rewarding outcomes are more likely to be performed than behaviors that result in punishment (Zimmerman &
Koussa, 1979).
Modeled (vicarious) consequences also indicate the motivational value of behavior to observers (Bandura,
1986). By observing modeled behaviors and their consequences, people formulate outcome expectations about
the likely outcomes of actions. These expectations motivate behavior (Zimmerman, 1977).
Perceived similarity between model and observer is hypothesized to be an important source of information to
determine behavioral appropriateness and formulate outcome expectations (Schunk, 1987). Most social
situations are structured so that the appropriateness of behaviors depends on factors such as age, gender, or
status. Although exceptions exist, the more alike observers are to models, the greater the probability that similar
actions by observers are socially appropriate and will produce comparable results (Zimmerman & Koussa,
1975). Similarity is especially influential when observers have little information about the functional value of
behaviors. This is not intended to downplay the importance of diversity: People learn from models who are
unlike themselves in many attributes. Societal norms are such, however, that similarity in critical attributes is
highly predictive of appropriateness for many behaviors (Bandura, 1986).
The effects of modeled consequences on observers depend in part on self-efficacy. Similarity to models consti-
tutes an important source of vicarious information for gauging one's efficacy. Observing similar others succeed
can raise observers' efficacy and motivate them to try the task because they may believe that if others can
succeed they can as well. When similar others experience difficulty, observers may doubt their capabilities and
may not be motivated to try the task. Similarity can have profound effects in situations where individuals have
experienced difficulties and hold doubts about performing well (Schunk, 1987).
In the current view, models are important sources for conveying self-regulatory skills and for building their self-
efficacy to employ these skills effectively on their own. The academic self-regulatory skills mentioned at the
outset of this article are amenable to transmission by social models: planning and managing time; attending to
and concentrating on instruction; organizing, rehearsing, and coding information strategically; establishing a
productive work environment; and using social resources. For example, students might observe a teacher
engage in effective time management and verbalize appropriate principles. By observing models, students may
believe that they also can plan and manage time effectively, which creates a sense of self-efficacy for academic
self-regulation and motivates students to engage in these activities.
Self-Efficacy
Self-efficacy is hypothesized to influence choice of tasks, effort expenditure, persistence, and achievement
(Bandura, 1986, 1997; Schunk, 1996a; Zimmerman, 1995b). Compared with students who doubt their learning
capabilities, those with high self-efficacy for acquiring a skill or performing a task participate more readily,
work harder, persist longer when they encounter difficulties, and achieve at a higher level.
Learners obtain information about their self-efficacy from their performance accomplishments, vicarious
(observational) experiences, forms of persuasion, and physiological reactions. Students' own performances offer
reliable guides for assessing self-efficacy. Successes raise efficacy and failures lower it (Zimmerman & Ringle,
1981). Students socially acquire efficacy information by comparing their performances with those of others.
Similar others offer a valid basis for comparison (Schunk, 1987). Observing similar peers succeed (or fail) at a
task may raise (or lower) observers' self-efficacy, The effects of such vicariously induced changes in self-
efficacy can be negated by observers' subsequent performance outcomes (e.g., observers fail to perform the task
after they view successful models).

Learners often receive from teachers, parents, coaches, and peers persuasive information that they are capable
of performing a task (e.g., "You can do this."). Such information can raise efficacy but can lose its influence
from subsequent performance failure (Bandura, 1997). Students also acquire efficacy information from
physiological reactions (e.g., sweating, heart rate). Symptoms signaling anxiety may convey that one lacks skill;
lower anxiety may be construed as a sign of greater competency. It should be noted that sources may offer
conflicting information; for example, a teacher gives positive feedback, but a student feels highly anxious. We
cannot always predict which source will have the strongest effect on efficacy; it may depend on factors such as
prior experiences, source credibility, and motivational influences (e.g., peer pressure).
Self-efficacy is an important influence on achievement behavior but not the only one. High self-efficacy will
not produce competent performances when requisite knowledge and skills are lacking, Outcome expectations
are influential because students engage in activities they believe will lead to positive outcomes (Shell, Murphy,
& Bruning, 1989). Per ceived valuethe perceived importance of learning, or what use will be made of what
one learnsaffects behavior because learners show little interest in activities they do not value. Assuming,
then, that students possess adequate skill, hold positive outcome expectations, and value what they are learning,
self-efficacy is hypothesized to exert an important effect on the instigation, direction, and persistence of
achievement behavior.
Effective self-regulation depends on feeling self-efficacious for using skills to achieve mastery (Bandura, 1986,
1997; Bouffard-Bouchard, Parent, & Larivee, 1991; Schunk, 1996a; Zimmerman, 1989). As students work on a
task, they compare their performances to their goals. Self-evaluations of progress enhance self-efficacy and
maintain motivation to improve. Students who feel efficacious about learning or performing well are apt to
implement effective self-regulatory strategies, such as concentrating on the task, using proper procedures,
managing time effectively, seeking assistance as necessary, and monitoring performance and adjusting strate-
gies as needed (McCombs, 1989; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Zimmerman, 1994). The latter self-regulatory
strategies are types of volitional processes (Corno, 1993) because they involve task and self-management rather
than personal appraisals of efficacy.
Although low self-efficacy is detrimental, effective selfregulation does not require that self-efficacy be
exceptionally high. Salomon (1984) found that a slightly lower sense of self-efficacy led to greater mental effort
and better learning than did extreme confidence. There is a limit, however; a very low sense of efficacy does not
motivate. Assuming that learners feel efficacious about surmounting problems, holding some doubt about
success may mobilize effort and effective use of strategies more than does feeling overly confident.
Development of Self-Regulatory Competence
Zimmerman and Bonner (in press) advanced a social cognitive theoretical model of the development of self-
regulatory competence (Table 1). The model predicts that academic competence develops initially from social
sources and subsequently shifts to self sources in a series of levels. Novice learners acquire self-regulatory skills
and strategies most rapidly from social modeling, tuition, task structuring, and encouragement (Zimmerman &
Rosenthal, 1974). Although many learners can induce the major features of strategies from watching a model
(observational level of academic skill), most of them will benefit from actually performing the strategies to help
incorporate them into their behavioral repertoires. If a model adopts a teaching role and provides guidance,
feedback, and social reinforcement during practice, he or she can improve the observer's behavioral accuracy of

Citations
More filters
Book ChapterDOI

Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective.

TL;DR: In this paper, the structure of self-regulatory systems, social and physical environmental context influences on self-regulation, dysfunctions in selfregulation, and selfregulatory development are discussed.
Journal ArticleDOI

Academic Self-Concept and Self-Efficacy: How Different Are They Really?

TL;DR: The authors argue that self-efficacy acts as an active precursor of self-concept development and suggest that selfconcept research separate out its multiple components and subprocesses and invest more effort toward making students less preoccupied with normative ability comparisons in school.
Journal ArticleDOI

Collective Efficacy Beliefs:Theoretical Developments, Empirical Evidence, and Future Directions

TL;DR: The authors synthesize existing research to discuss how teachers' practice and student learning are affected by perceptions of collective efficacy, and develop a conceptual model to explain the formation and influence of perceived collective efficacy in schools.
BookDOI

Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance

TL;DR: Self-Regulation of learning and performance has been studied extensively in the literature as mentioned in this paper, with a focus on the role of self-regulation in the development of learners' skills and abilities.
Journal ArticleDOI

Influencing children’s self-efficacy and self-regulation of reading and writing through modeling

TL;DR: In this paper, the authors discuss students' reading and writing performances using Zimmerman's four-phase social cognitive model of the development of self-regulatory competence and suggest instructional applications based on social cognitive theory and research findings.
References
More filters
Book

Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control

TL;DR: SelfSelf-Efficacy (SE) as discussed by the authors is a well-known concept in human behavior, which is defined as "belief in one's capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to produce given attainments".
Book

Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes

TL;DR: In this paper, Cole and Scribner discuss the role of play in children's development and play as a tool and symbol in the development of perception and attention in a prehistory of written language.
Journal ArticleDOI

Social learning theory

TL;DR: In this article, an exploración de the avances contemporaneos en la teoria del aprendizaje social, con especial enfasis en los importantes roles que cumplen los procesos cognitivos, indirectos, and autoregulatorios.
Book

Thought and language

Lev Vygotsky
TL;DR: Kozulin has created a new edition of the original MIT Press translation by Eugenia Hanfmann and Gertrude Vakar that restores the work's complete text and adds materials that will help readers better understand Vygotsky's meaning and intentions as discussed by the authors.
Book

Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory

TL;DR: In this paper, models of Human Nature and Casualty are used to model human nature and human health, and a set of self-regulatory mechanisms are proposed. But they do not consider the role of cognitive regulators.
Frequently Asked Questions (2)
Q1. What are the contributions in this paper?

This article reviews the social origins of students ' development of self-regulatory skill with special emphasis on observational learning through modeling. A social cognitive perspective on self-regulation is presented. Related theoretical perspectives are discussed along with suggestions for future research. In recent years, cognitive theorists have shifted their attention to the process whereby self-regulatory competence is internalized and have studied how children arid adolescents learn to function independently from socializing agents in an adaptive, generative, and creative manner ( Bandura, 1986 ; Como, 1989 ; Fuson, 1979 ; Kopp, 1982 ; Mithaug, 1993 ; Paris & Newman, 1990 ; Vygotsky, 1978 ; Wertsch, 1979 ; Zimmerman, in press ). In this article the authors identify and review research on the social origins of students ' development of self-regulatory skill with special emphasis on observational learning through modeling. The authors initially present a theoretical overview of self-regulation from the particular perspective of social cognitive theory. The authors focus on the roles played by two constructs that are intimately linked with this theoretical perspective and that have been rigorously investigated by researchers in this tradition—modeling and selfefficacy. The authors then describe a social cognitive phase model of the development of self-regulatory competence, and they compare that perspective to some other prominent views relating to the development of self-regulation. An example of the influence of environment on behavior occurs when teachers introduce an unusual stimulus or novel event ( environmental variable ) and students direct their attention toward it ( behavior ). The academic self-regulatory skills mentioned at the outset of this article are amenable to transmission by social models: planning and managing time ; attending to and concentrating on instruction ; organizing, rehearsing, and coding information strategically ; establishing a productive work environment ; and using social resources. The source of learning of self-regulatory skill is primarily social for the first two levels of academic competence ; most research summarized in this article addresses social influences. The authors discuss some higher level research, but they refer readers to Schunk and Zimmerman ( 1996 ) for a thorough discussion of research at the self level. Although this article focuses on social cognitive theory, other theoretical perspectives address the development of self-regulation. Their purpose is not to provide a complete review of applicable self-regulation theories ; an extensive discussion would alter the article 's focus and extend beyond its intended scope. Rather, the authors discuss two well-known and often-cited perspectives bearing on the development of self-regulation: sociocultural theory and self-determination theory. The authors suggest that an optimal model of self-regulatory development needs to explain not only increasing self-motivation but also enhanced self-regulation of learning, such as children 's increasing capability to set goals, concentrate, self-monitor, and use strategies on their own ( Bandura, 1986 ; Schunk, 1994 ; Zimmerman, 1994 ). For example, self-monitored changes in learning during the self-control and self-regulation phases enhance self-reinforcement and self-efficacy beliefs as well as provide feedback about the effectiveness of particular learning strategies. This section presents a limited review of research on the social origins of self-regulatory competence. Selfinfluences, which include factors such as self-instruction, personal goal setting, self-monitoring, self-evaluation, help seeking, and time management, extend beyond the scope of this article and are discussed in detail elsewhere ( Bandura, 1986 ; Schunk & Zimmerman, 1996 ). In a typical study, participants are videotaped while performing a task and subsequently view their tapes. Children assigned to the process-goal plus progress-feedback condition periodically received social ( verbal ) feedback from the adult model that linked their use of the strategy with improved writing performance ( e. g., `` You 're doing well because you followed the steps in order. `` ). Some girls were asked to set a process goal and concentrate on executing the strategy ( e. g., sighting, throwing, follow through ) as they practiced the skill, whereas others were asked to set a product ( e. g., outcome ) goal of trying to get the most points as they practiced. However, after internalized self-control is attained, product goals may enhance learning better, and this hypothesis was tested in research. This article suggests that students ' self-regulation of learning develops from initial social modeling experiences and progresses through increasing levels of self-directed functioning. The authors have described some research results that support the importance of modeling and self-efficacy in this formulation. In this section the authors suggest three areas where they believe future research is needed to clarify and refine the hypothesized operation of social origins of the development of self-regulatory competence. The authors discuss two of these: cooperative groups and peer trainers. This type of research may require extensive naturalistic observations of groups over long periods, but the data would provide rich information on what types of models operate best in the setting. In this view, modeling is important for enhancing acquisition and motivation, and also can help provide corrective or supplementary instruction during retention and transfer. The authors might investigate what features of modeled displays are most important for transfer. First, it describes how self-regulatory skill and a sense of self-efficacy grow out of specific social learning instructional experiences, including modeling and socially reinforced attempts to imitate as well as self-controlled study or practice efforts. Research on modeling and self-efficacy processes is summarized, and the authors conclude with suggestions for future research in educational settings. The authors can not always predict which source will have the strongest effect on efficacy ; it may depend on factors such as prior experiences, source credibility, and motivational influences ( e. g., peer pressure ). Students internalize more than an academic skill from these social learning episodes ; they acquire the tools for further learning the skill more effectively on their own, such as how to self-monitor and self-adjust their study efforts. Sociocultural Theory Vygotsky ( 1962, 1978 ) suggested that children 's self-regulatory activities grow from social interactions between adults or more mature peers and learners in four stages. Although Vygotsky ' s theory could be used to explain cognitive features of self-regulatory development and self-determination theory could be used to describe motivational features, the authors suggest that an integrative explanation for both learning and motivational processes is preferable. The authors believe that an integrative model of self-regulatory learning and motivational development is essential for guiding research and instructional applications. To further focus their review, the authors summarize primarily research on observational learning through modeling. Schunk, Hanson, and Cox ( 1987 ) further explored the coping-mastery model distinction and found that observing coping models enhances children 's self-efficacy and skillful performance more than does observing mastery models. These results suggest that as strategic performing is being internalized, process goals enhance learning better than product goals. Teachers commonly make use of models in the classroom, but theory and research suggest that the particular use of models who demonstrate the cognitive skills and strategies of self-regulation is important. Theory and research suggest that transfer is not automatic and that a variety of factors affect it. The authors suggest that modeling and self-efficacy influence transfer, and they advocate increased research emphasis on the transfer of self-regulatory skills. The authors also can determine where transfer occurs in their levels of self-regulation ; for example, are verbal descriptions adequate or do learners require the more intensive feedback that typically occurs at the imitative level ? The authors suggest that social learning experiences can be planned and organized by teachers and parents to accelerate children 's self-regulatory development. 

In this section the authors suggest three areas where they believe future research is needed to clarify and refine the hypothesized operation of social origins of the development of self-regulatory competence. 

Trending Questions (1)
What are the papers about Student self-regulation in englsh language education?

The provided paper is about the social origins of students' development of self-regulatory skill, with a focus on observational learning through modeling. It does not specifically discuss student self-regulation in English language education.