scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question

Showing papers in "Primates in 1996"


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Oct 1996-Primates
TL;DR: 32 primate positional modes are defined, divided more finely into 52 postural sub-modes and 74 locomotor sub-Modes, and a nomenclature is recommended that is not dedicated to or derived from any one taxonomic subset of the primates.
Abstract: As quantitative studies on primate positional behavior accumulate the lack of a standard positional mode terminology is becoming an increasingly serious deficiency. Inconsistent use of traditional terms and inappropriate conflation of mode categories hamper interspecific and interobserver comparisons. Some workers use common terms without definition, allowing at least the possibility of misunderstanding. Other researchers coin neologisms tailored to their study species and not clearly enough defined to allow application to other species. Such neologisms may overlap, may completely encompass, or may conflate previously defined labels. The result is, at best, the proliferation of synonyms and, at worst, the creation of confusion where clarity had existed. Historical precedents have sometimes resulted in “catch-all” terms that conflate any number of kinematically different behaviors (e.g. “brachiation,” “climbing,” and “quadrumanous climbing”). We recognize three areas where distinction of positional modes has some current importance: (1) Modes that require humeral abduction should be distinguished from adducted behaviors; (2) locomotor modes that involve ascent or descent should be distinguished from horizontal locomotor modes; and (3) suspensory modes should be distinguished from supported modes. We recommend a nomenclature that is not dedicated to or derived from any one taxonomic subset of the primates. Here we define 32 primate positional modes, divided more finely into 52 postural sub-modes and 74 locomotor sub-modes.

348 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Apr 1996-Primates
TL;DR: These analyses and preliminary observations linking geophagy with instances of severe diarrhea, and other signs of gastrointestinal upset in the Mahale chimpanzees, suggest that one function for the ingestion of this substance by chimpanzees may be to help provide temporary relief from gastrointestinal ailments.
Abstract: Earth from a termite mound in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, eaten by chimpanzees, was analyzed to determine the possible stimulus, or stimuli, for geophagy. The termite mound sample contains relatively high aluminum (10.0%), iron (3.0%), and sodium (0.5%). This correlates well with the mineralogy of the clay (<2 µm) fraction, which is high in metahalloysite, a 1:1 (Si:Al=1:1) clay mineral similar in chemical composition to the clay mineral kaolinite, and smectite (montmorillonite), which is a 2:1 expandable clay mineral. The combination of metahalloysite and smectite produces a substance much like the pharmaceutical Kaopectate™ widely used by humans as an anti-diarrheal agent. These analyses and preliminary observations linking geophagy with instances of severe diarrhea, and other signs of gastrointestinal upset in the Mahale chimpanzees, suggest that one function for the ingestion of this substance by chimpanzees may be to help provide temporary relief from gastrointestinal ailments. Further detailed investigations into the relationship between health and geophagy should provide important insights into the diverse roles of this behavior as a form of self-medication.

91 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: High sequence variation in the target region indicated that females transfer between groups of bonobos, which is in agreement with supposition from long-term field studies.
Abstract: Matrilineal kin-relations among wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) were studied by DNA analysis. Subject individuals were the members of E1 group, living at Wamba, Zaire, which has been studied since 1974. DNA samples were extracted from wadges that bonobos spat out when feeding on sugar cane. The D-loop region of mitochondrial DNA was amplified by the PCR method, and a nucleotide sequence of 350 base pairs was determined for 17 individuals. Nucleotide variations were found at 44 positions of the sequence. Based on these variations, 13 matrilineal units were divided into seven groups, and the mother of an orphan male was determined among several females. These genetic analyses, together with behavioral observation to date, revealed the following facts. High sequence variation in the target region indicated that females transfer between groups of bonobos, which is in agreement with supposition from long-term field studies. For females, there was no relationship between genetic closeness and social closeness that is represented by frequencies of proximity or grooming. After immigration into a new group, females form social associations with senior females without regard to kin relationship.

74 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
Masako Myowa1
30 Apr 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The present study clearly demonstrated that the infant chimpanzee could imitate human facial gestures in a particular period.
Abstract: A nursery-reared infant chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) was tested for the ability to imitate human facial gestures, following the procedure ofMeltzoff andMoore (1977). The subject was observed between 5 to 15 weeks of ages. The results showed that the subject could imitate tongue protrusion in the age of 5–10 weeks and mouth opening between the ages of 5–11 weeks. However, from 12 to 15 weeks, the infant did not imitate the facial gestures. The present study clearly demonstrated that the infant chimpanzee could imitate human facial gestures in a particular period.

67 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The Sanwa chimpanzees had similar growth patterns to those of the Yerkes chimpanzees, although they showed a slight delay in infancy, and a higher growth rate from the early juvenile phase onwards, and may be regarded as “normative” for laboratory-reared chimpanzees.
Abstract: Growth of chimpanzees reared at the Kumamoto Primates Park of Sanwa Kagaku Kenkyusho Co. Ltd. was studied cross-sectionally from the viewpoints of somatic growth and reproductive maturation. Distance and velocity curves were expressed using spline function method. Males showed adolescent growth acceleration in body weight, with a peak at 7.86 yrs of age, but not in trunk length. Females showed continuous rapid growth from mid-juvenile to adolescent phase in both body weight and trunk length, but no isolated adolescent spurt. The Sanwa chimpanzees matured at about 12.5 yrs of age for females and 15.0 yrs for males. The mean adult weights and trunk lengths were 53.2 kg and 507.8 mm for males and 42.7 kg and 481.6 mm for females. The Sanwa chimpanzees had similar growth patterns to those of the Yerkes chimpanzees, although they showed a slight delay in infancy, and a higher growth rate from the early juvenile phase onwards. Growth patterns in these two laboratories may be regarded as “normative” for laboratory-reared chimpanzees. They matured earlier than wild chimpanzees by more than two years. The major reason for the retarded maturation in wild chimpanzees is the delay of growth from infant to the early juvenile phases (0–4 yrs of age), probably owing to a limited nutritional supply from the mother. Development of the testes comprised three phases: slow growth from infant to juvenile (until 6.4 yrs); rapid growth around adolescence (until 9.2 yrs); and adult (mean testicular volume, 187 cm3). Setting the nutritional standard at 2,000–2,600 Cal/day (= Kcal/day) per adult, calories were considered for captive chimpanzees in each age class.

62 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
30 Apr 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Red uakari (Cacajao calvus ucayalii) living near the Communal Reserve Tamshiyacu-Tahuayo in northeastern Peru frequently associate with other species of primate, especially the woolly monkey (Lagothrix lagotricha).
Abstract: Red uakari (Cacajao calvus ucayalii) living near the Communal Reserve Tamshiyacu-Tahuayo in northeastern Peru frequently associate with other species of primate, especially the woolly monkey (Lagothrix lagotricha). In 42 observation follows over 14 months (April 1993 – December 1994), the uakari were in association with other species for all or some portion of time during 62% of the follows. Woolly monkeys were present during 65% of the polyspecific groupings. During 151.5 hr of timed observations, the uakari spent 29.21% of their time with other species, and 76% of their associative time with woolly monkeys. The participants of the associations may benefit from increased predator protection and more efficient use of resource. These data represent the first published notes from a long-term behavioral ecology study of red uakari.

53 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: To examine the development of cooperation in a captive group of chimpanzees, an apparatus which required the simultaneous traction by two animals to get a reward was designed and visual behavior was used to try to determine what chimpanzees learned about the cooperative task.
Abstract: To examine the development of cooperation in a captive group of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), we designed an apparatus which required the simultaneous traction by two animals to get a reward. Two chimpanzees, an adult male and an infant female in a group of six, produced most of the successful responses (pulling on two handles simultaneously). Visual behavior was used to try to determine what chimpanzees learned about the cooperative task. Propositions were made to investigate what kind of learning could be attributed to chimpanzees and were confronted with results. Both subjects learned the link between the presence of fruits on the apparatus and the possibility of getting a fruit. They also learned the importance of the partner at the apparatus to make a successful response. Only the adult male learned to take into account the behavior of the partner at the apparatus before pulling a handle. From a methodological point of view, the glances made by the animals can constitute a useful behavioral indicator of what the subjects learned in a given social situation.

49 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: In this paper, a group of long-tailed macaques were observed to engage in manipulations on the same object class more frequently during times when the model was working than when it was not.
Abstract: In a captive group of long-tailed macaques, tool-using behavior by a single competent individual had a significant effect on the synchronous manipulative behavior of naive animals. Group members engaged in manipulations on the same object class more frequently during times when the model was working than when it was not. The form of their behavior, however, in no way resembled the technique used by the model. All three animals that later became successful tool users were among the few subjects that exhibited a significant increase in manipulations on the same object class while the model was working. Possible causal relationships between this stimulus enhancement and the transmission of the new behavior to other group members are analyzed and discussed.

48 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Although no significant population-level left- or right-hand bias emerged for any of the tasks, there was a tendency toward greater left-hand use in a task requiring the use of probing tools.
Abstract: Eight tasks were presented to ten tufted capuchin monkeys and hand preferences were recorded for each subject on each task. The strength of hand preferences varied accross tasks. Although no significant population-level left- or right-hand bias emerged for any of the tasks, there was a tendency toward greater left-hand use in a task requiring the use of probing tools. The data also confirm that simple reaching is of limited value as a measure of hand preference. Hand preferences in capuchin monkeys appear to be determined by a number of individual, species, and environmental factors.

48 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
31 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The skeletal remains of a wild juvenile chimpanzee,Pan troglodytes verus, of known chronological age are measured and found to be smaller than laboratory born and fed juveniles of the same age.
Abstract: The skeletal remains of a wild juvenile chimpanzee,Pan troglodytes verus, of known chronological age are measured and found to be smaller than laboratory born and fed juveniles of the same age. Other wild born immature skeletal materials of all the three subspecies ofPan troglodytes, including both known and estimated chronological ages, are also smaller than laboratory born chimpanzees when comparisons are made on corresponding age groups. Differences between wild and laboratory born chimpanzees are larger in the limb bones than in the cranium. Limb bones of laboratory individuals grow earlier than those of wild ones regardless of subspecies. Small limb bone size of wild chimpanzees is discussed in terms of life processes.

45 citations


Journal ArticleDOI
01 Oct 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Active antipredator behavior was observed in a recently found gelada population in Arsi, Ethiopia and may provide a useful insight for the understanding of the process of evolutionary adaptation to highland habitats by gelada baboons, as well as for the origin of this small southern population.
Abstract: The anti-predator behavior of gelada baboons has been observed to consist of simple flight (i.e. they flee to cliffs after becoming alarmed by potential predators). However, active antipredator behavior was observed in a recently found gelada population in Arsi, Ethiopia. This population showed frequent active counter-attacks and severe mobbing at predators. Males fought domestic dogs on the plain and exhibited a systematic mobbing behavior towards a leopard on the cliff face to chase it off. These active anti-predator behaviors are the first confirmed reports on gelada baboons and may provide a useful insight for the understanding of the process of evolutionary adaptation to highland habitats by gelada baboons, as well as for the origin of this small southern population.

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Oct 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Comparison of chimpanzees and bonobos revealed significant differences in the variety and the complexity of object manipulation forms, with chimpanzees' object manipulations were overall more substrate-oriented than were bonobos'.
Abstract: Four types of specific objects: wooden spoons, metal bowls, plastic boxes, and cotton towels were introduced in a similar setting to two captive groups of different species in the genusPan, the bonobo and the chimpanzee. In total, 582 unique manipulation forms were distinguished by a set of variables: types of objects, motor patterns, body-parts used, the number of objects manipulated, and types of orienting manipulation. In sum, chimpanzees and bonobos were not so different in the variety and the complexity of object manipulation forms. However, comparison of the two species revealed significant differences as follows: (1) chimpanzees preferred to use only one hand during manipulation of both single- and multiple-objects, whereas in the case of multiple-objects bonobos used both hands significantly more often; (2) chimpanzees performed more orienting manipulations in single-object manipulations than did bonobos, whereas the reverse was the case in multiple-object manipulations; and (3) chimpanzees' object manipulations were overall more substrate-oriented than were bonobos'. The factors producing these differences are discussed in relation to positional behaviors and habitual tool use in the two species.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The results suggest that carrying is a stable feature inCallithrix jacchus groups, but there is a trend suggestive of an extended carrying period in natural groups, as well as adult helpers carried infants more than subadults and juveniles.
Abstract: Data on callitrichids infant carrying is highly variable, and there is some disagreement over the relative involvement of different group members. Considering that variation may result from different social and environmental conditions it is important to compare data from different environments. The purpose of this study was to compare infant carrying in 11Callithrix jacchus groups living in two colonies, in the UK and Brazil, or in a field site in the northeastern Brazil. No differences were found in the amount of time that infants were carried, in the three environmental conditions, during weeks 1 to 4, 7, and 10. Fathers and mothers carried infants for equivalent amounts of time in the three conditions, but helpers from natural groups carried infants more than their captive counterparts. In general, adult helpers carried infants more than subadults and juveniles. The results suggest that carrying is a stable feature inCallithrix jacchus groups, but there is a trend suggestive of an extended carrying period in natural groups. Mothers behaviour were remarkably stable, but fathers behaviour, although comparable, was more conspicuous in captivity than in the field, what may be attributed to paternity certainty. Finally, the greater participation of field helpers in carrying is considered as deriving mostly to age of helpers than to group size.

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The results showed the squirrel monkey to prefer lower sugar concentrations compared to other non-human primates and thus support the assumption that this species may use sweetness as a criterion for food selection.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to determine taste preference thresholds for four different food-associated sugars inSaimiri sciureus. Using a two-bottle test three male squirrel monkeys were found to significantly prefer 10 mM sucrose, 40 mM fructose, 90 mM glucose, and 100 mM lactose over tap water. Presentation of suprathreshold sugar solutions led to marked concentration-dependent and monotonically increasing polydipsia. The results showed the squirrel monkey to prefer lower sugar concentrations compared to other non-human primates and thus support the assumption that this species may use sweetness as a criterion for food selection.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Recognition of facial expressions by a Japanese monkey and two humans was studied and suggested that the important cues in recognizing facial expressions of monkeys were “thrusting the mouth” and “raising the eyebrows.”
Abstract: Recognition of facial expressions by a Japanese monkey and two humans was studied. The monkey subject matched 20 photographs of monkey facial expressions and 20 photographs of human facial expressions. Humans sorted the same pictures. Matching accuracy by the monkey was about 80% correct for both human and monkey facial expressions. The confusion matrices of those facial expressions were analyzed by a multi-dimensional scaling procedure (MDSCAL). The resulting MDS plots suggested that the important cues in recognizing facial expressions of monkeys were “thrusting the mouth” and ‘raising the eyebrows.” Comparison of the MDS plots by the monkey subject with those by human subjects suggested that the monkey categorized the human “happiness” faces. This may suggest that the monkey has an ability to recognize human smile face even though it is learned. However, the monkey did not differentiate the human “anger/disgust” faces from the human “sad” faces, while human subjects clearly did. This may correlate with the lack of eyebrow movement in monkeys.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: A strong correlation between prefeed association patterns and spatial proximity during clumped feeding sessions suggests that chimpanzees' main concern is to be allowed to feed near individuals who are able to monopolize food sources.
Abstract: Captive chimpanzees appear to anticipate the occurrence of conflict during feeding by grooming and being in proximity at increased rates during the hour prior to feeding. The effect is more marked when food is clumped than when it is dispersed, suggesting that the proximate cause is the anticipation of increased levels of competition. Chimpanzees did not choose high ranking individuals more often as prefeed grooming partners; rather, they preferred to associate with their normal grooming partners (as reflected in post-feed grooming preferences) and close kin. A strong correlation between prefeed association patterns and spatial proximity during clumped feeding sessions suggests that their main concern is to be allowed to feed near individuals who are able to monopolize food sources.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: In three sets of experiments, a group of captive Ring-tailed lemurs ranging at liberty were presented with sticks scent-marked with their own scent, or with scent from unrelated animals or unmarked sticks, and the role of scent-marking and the concept of group scent are discussed.
Abstract: In three sets of experiments, a group of captive Ring-tailed lemurs ranging at liberty were presented with sticks scent-marked with their own scent, or with scent from unrelated animals or unmarked sticks. The results from experiments which involved a choice between scents indicated stronger responses, in terms of greater intensity of scent-marking to scent from “strange” animals. In the experiment where individual scents were presented one at a time, the length of time spent sniffing was greater for “strange” scent but this did not transfer to additional scent-marks. Most of the responses were elicited from males in the group and involved wrist-marking only, but timing of experiment influenced both responses and type of scent-mark probably in relation to the onset of oestrus. The role of scent-marking and the concept of group scent are also discussed.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Oct 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The results of this study suggest task-specific demands like posture and/or whether reaching was visually or tactually guided to be the major correlates of hand preferences in food-reaching tasks in squirrel monkeys.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to test the influence of sex, age, social rank, matriline membership, posture, and visual and tactual motor control on manual preferences inSaimiri sciureus. A well-established social group of 12 squirrel monkeys, aged 2 to 14 yrs and consisting of two matrilines with social rank known for each animal, was presented with four different food-reaching tasks and assessed for hand preferences with a minimum of 100 reaches per animal. Frequency of occurrence of hand preferences at the group level and degree of hand preferences at the individual level depended on posture and on whether the reaching act took place under visual or tactual guidance. Sex, age, social rank, and matriline membership were not found to determine frequency of occurrence, direction or degree of hand preferences with the exception of one task in which a significant negative correlation between the degree of hand preference and age was found. Nine out of 12 monkeys showed task-dependent changes in the hand they used preferentially while only three animals preferred the same hand in all four tasks. Significant preferences for the use of right or left hand on a given task were distributed almost equally between individuals. Thus, the results of this study suggest task-specific demands like posture and/or whether reaching was visually or tactually guided to be the major correlates of hand preferences in food-reaching tasks in squirrel monkeys.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Apr 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The triad was very likely a by-product of male-infant caretaking, which was probably shaped to compensate heavy maternal investment to young offspring in harsh conditions, and the long-term arguments about the triad in M. sylvanus can be united to a model of the way in which “male-infants caretaking” hypothesis works ultimately, and “regulating social relations’ hypothesis does proximately.
Abstract: Event-sampling and scans were used for collecting data on male-infant-male triadic interactions, and their effects on member spacing respectively in a group ofMacaca thibetana at Mt. Emei in 1989. The group was partially provisioned by human visitors in seasons other than winter, and could be observed closely. In addition, a stable linear male-hierarchy among five males existed for two years since the end of 1987, providing a good social condition for this topic. The triadic interactions were specific to the birth season, and recognized as three types being on a continuum functionally changing from passive “agonistic buffering” (4.8%) to active spatial cohesion, which resulted in a significant decline of intermale distances. Positive correlations were documented between the triad initiation rate and the number of females in consort with the males in the mating season (MS), and between the triad reception rate and the number of infants in proximity to the males in the MS when maternal care was significantly reduced. Thus the male's mating effort and kin/sexual selection may deeply be involved in the triad of this species. Considering that the two triad-species,M. sylvanus andM. thibetana, had different levels of paternity, but shared similar foraging conditions, and showed similar intensities of male-infant caretaking, the triad was very likely a by-product of male-infant caretaking, which was probably shaped to compensate heavy maternal investment to young offspring in harsh conditions. Accordingly, the long-term arguments about the triad inM. sylvanus can be united to a model of the way in which “male-infant caretaking” hypothesis works ultimately, and “regulating social relations” hypothesis does proximately.

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Wild populations ofAlouatta belzebul and A. seniculus from Brazil were surveyed in relation to 20 protein loci and estimates of genetic variability demonstrated that A.seniculus presents the highest level of heterozygosity among several New World monkey species studied for the same loci.
Abstract: Wild populations ofAlouatta belzebul andA. seniculus from Brazil were surveyed in relation to 20 protein loci. Estimates of genetic variability demonstrated thatA. seniculus presents the highest level of heterozygosity among several New World monkey species studied for the same loci. Additional information from DNA and chromosomes suggest thatA. seniculus andA. belzebul are not the closest species in theHershkovitz’sAlouatta seniculus group.

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Predatory behavior of Pan t.
Abstract: Predatory behavior ofPan t. troglodytes in the Ndoki Forest was confirmed by both direct observation and fecal evidence. Eight out of 214 fecal samples (3.7%), collected during 16 months, contained vertebrate tissue. The prey species were a terrestrial bird, two monkey species including crowned guenon, a squirrel, and probably a pangolin. This rate suggested that predation in the Ndoki population can occur as frequently as in other populations. Chimpanzees were also directly observed to eat an infant crowned guenon, a hornbill, and a duiker. An adult female used a branch apparently in an attempt to drive out a hornbill from its nest hole, though no bird was observed to come out. Chimpanzees were attracted to meat, and were observed begging and sharing over the meat. Predatory behavior is common toPan andHomo, but not toGorilla, implying that the common ancestor of the former two genera acquired this behavior after separating from gorillas.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Apr 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The results suggest that the presence of older siblings as helpers affects the parents' participation in caregiving by altering maternal investment through the control of nursing and by reducing the costs of carrying for the father.
Abstract: Cooperative care of the young by the father, older siblings, and unrelated group members is observed in a number of species, but inCallithrix jacchus parental care by the father is considered to be part of the reproductive behavior of the species. Nevertheless, the way the presence of potential helpers influences the costs of caring for the parents is not well understood. In this study we describe the suckling phase of youngC. jacchus, in families with and without older siblings, assessing the effect of siblings on the amount of care given by the father and mother. Six family groups were observed: three consisting of the mated pair and two newborn infants, and three with one or two additional sub-adult siblings. Our results suggest that the presence of older siblings as helpers affects the parents' participation in caregiving by altering maternal investment through the control of nursing and by reducing the costs of carrying for the father.

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: This study did not support the hypothesis of pharmacological self-medication via leaf swallowing in chimpanzees, and the hypothesis that chemical components in swallowed leaves have negative effects on intestinal nematodes was not supported.
Abstract: ChimpanzeesPan troglodytes in Kibale National Park, Uganda occasionally swallow entire leaves ofRubia cordifolia (Rubiaceae) without chewing them. The leaves are subsequently egested with minimal damage and no sign of any significant digestion. Similar behavior reported elsewhere has been proposed to have medicinal effects. Here we test the hypothesis that chemical components in swallowed leaves have negative effects on intestinal nematodes. We used anin vitro assay to detect the effects of methanol extracts ofR. cordifolia leaves on nematodes,Strongyloides spp., cultured from feces. Control extracts were distilled water, methanol solution, and methanol extracts of food items that were chewed, rather than swallowed, by chimpanzees. Effects of experimental or control solutions were assayed by nematode motility. There was no difference in nematode motility among control and experimental extracts. This study therefore did not support the hypothesis of pharmacological self-medication via leaf swallowing.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Two births of wild proboscis monkeys took place during a 1991/1992 field study at Tanjung Puting National Park, Indonesia, where the infant was born unassisted while the members of the group watched the birthing process.
Abstract: Two births of wild proboscis monkeys took place during a 1991/1992 field study at Tanjung Puting National Park, Indonesia After conception, both females continued copulating with their group’s male One of the births took place during the night, the other during mid-morning In the latter case, the infant was born unassisted while the members of the group watched the birthing process After parturition, the mother consumed the placenta

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Oct 1996-Primates
TL;DR: E. fulvus agonism was in fact not rare except in comparison to baboons and macaques, and compared rates reported in other research for brown lemurs, other lemuriform species, and some New and Old World anthropoid species.
Abstract: Agonistic behaviour was studied in three groups each of free-ranging and semi-free-ranging brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus) at Berenty, Madagascar and the Duke University Primate Center (DUPC) respectively. The purpose of the study was to answer questions arising from the work of other researchers regarding the frequency and intensity of agonism in this species. Authors of field studies generally concluded that agonism was rare and mild, whereas those who had studied semi-free-ranging or captive animals at the DUPC reported intense agonism during the peaks of the mating and birth seasons, with sometimes fatal wounding occurring among captive animals. I recorded 30 agonistic behaviours or “signals” which I grouped into seven general categories — cuffs, other physical contact, threats, chases, third party intervention, unprovoked submissive signals, and reciprocal aggression. The seven categories represent the types of signals which initiated or otherwise defined agonistic interactions, regardless of whether or not there was a submissive response to aggression. The relative percentages of all agonism constituted by the seven categories were not found to be significantly different between study sites. Agonistic signals were also classified as either subtle or obvious, a classification which crosscut the seven categories. At both study sites, the majority of agonistic signals initiating or defining interactions were subtle. Rates of agonism for the Berenty groups, studied during the birth season only, were significantly lower than those for the DUPC groups during the birth season, possibly due to (1) easier observation conditions at the DUPC, and (2) the impossibility of successful emigration at the DUPC, which might have resulted in social stresses translating into higher rates of agonism. In only one DUPC group was there significant variation in rates of agonism between seasons. I found agonistic behaviour to be mild, at both study sites, in the senses of subtlety of both aggressive and submissive signals, unlikelihood of response to aggression, and virtual absence of wounding; and I noted that serious wounding during other studies at the DUPC involved animals captive in caged runs. Comparing rates of the study groups with rates reported in other research for brown lemurs, other lemuriform species, and some New and Old World anthropoid species, I concluded thatE. fulvus agonism was in fact not rare except in comparison to baboons and macaques.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: This study of one captive pigtail macaque group demonstrates that rank was not a predictor of reproductive success and there was no preferential attraction for one's own offspring by males or one's Own sire by offspring.
Abstract: Paternity of 16 pigtail macaque offspring was determined using a DNA profile analysis and was based on two independent assays of the genome of each individual using multilocus DNA probes. The offspring were members of a group of 59 pigtail macaques, including 5 adult males, 1 subadult male, and 37 adult and subadult females. Rank was unrelated to paternity as the first ranking male sired 0, the second ranking male sired 3; the third ranking male sired 0, the fourth ranking male sired 8, and the fifth ranking male sired 2 offspring. The subadult male sired 0 offspring. The DNA analysis was effective in excluding possible sires of 3 offspring whose mothers had become pregnant by another male before being introduced to the study males. Subsequent semen evaluation revealed an absence of sperm in the semen of the alpha male, but revealed a sperm count within normal limits in the third ranking male, who also sired no offspring. Behavioral data focusing on male-offspring interactions found that offspring did not preferentially affiliate with their sire and that males did not affiliate with their offspring frequently enough for analysis. Thus, this study of one captive pigtail macaque group demonstrates that: (1) rank was not a predictor of reproductive success; and (2) there was no preferential attraction for one's own offspring by males or one's own sire by offspring.

Journal ArticleDOI
31 Jul 1996-Primates
TL;DR: In this article, two groups of captive macaques (M. fascicularis) were studied at Kassel University, Germany, and two groups were split into two mutually hostile conalitions: a despotic community and an egalitarian community.
Abstract: Two groups of captive macaques (M. fascicularis) were studied at Kassel University, Germany. One included animals whose mothers were high-ranking, another, those whose mothers were low-ranking. The first group was a despotic community in which conflicts were severe and occurred mainly between single individuals; the reconciliation tendency was weak, the male leader was the controlling animal, and the affiliative preferences were marked. The second group was an egalitarian community split into two mutually hostile conalitions; the conflicts were less severe, the tendency for reconciliation was strong, the male leader could control only his own bloc and had no strong affiliative ties with other group members.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Oct 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Temporary differences in the temporal sequence of specific developmental events in the embryonic period are important factors to be taken into consideration in any embryological and teratological studies when using M. fascicularis as a primate model.
Abstract: The external characteristics and successive morphological changes of the brain and its derivatives were studied in 69 long-tailed monkey embryos representing developmental stages 8 through 16. This morphogenesis follows a similar pattern to those of the rhesus, baboon, and human. Minor differences in the temporal sequence of specific developmental events include: 1) otic disc, adenohypophyseal pouch, and hippocampal internal sulcus formation in the long-tailed macaque occur at stage 10, stage 11, and after stage 16 respectively, which is comparable to human stages 9, 10, and 16; 2) formation of the trigeminal primordium and the motor root of the trigeminal nerve and evagination of the neurohypophysis occur at stage 12, stage 14, and stage 15, while in the human embryo these features are observed at stage 14, stage 15, and stage 16, respectively; and 3) closure of the lens pore, like in the rhesus monkey, occurs during stage 15, while in the baboon and human it takes place during stage 14. These temporal differences in the embryonic period are important factors to be taken into consideration in any embryological and teratological studies when usingM. fascicularis as a primate model.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Apr 1996-Primates
TL;DR: Analysis of error patterns and latency indicated that the rhesus monkeys had a larger asymmetry in response bias and were significantly slower in responding than the chimpanzees.
Abstract: This study describes video-task acquisition in two nonhuman primate species. The subjects were seven rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and seven chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). All subjects were trained to manipulate a joystick which controlled a cursor displayed on a computer monitor. Two criterion levels were used: one based on conceptual knowledge of the task and one based on motor performance. Chimpanzees and rhesus monkeys attained criterion in a comparable number of trials using a conceptually based criterion. However, using a criterion based on motor performance, chimpanzees reached criterion significantly faster than rhesus monkeys. Analysis of error patterns and latency indicated that the rhesus monkeys had a larger asymmetry in response bias and were significantly slower in responding than the chimpanzees. The results are discussed in terms of the relation between object manipulation skills and video-task acquisition.

Journal ArticleDOI
01 Jan 1996-Primates
TL;DR: The observation of seed predation on Cariniana estrellensis (Lecythidaceae) fruits by the black howler monkey Alouatta caraya in south-east Brazil during an intense dry season suggests that even non-specialized seed predators such as howlers can use seeds in critical periods of dry season.
Abstract: Seed predation by howler monkeys is poorly documented. Here we report the seed predation onCariniana estrellensis (Lecythidaceae) fruits by the black howler monkeyAlouatta caraya in south-east Brazil during an intense dry season. This observation suggests that even non-specialized seed predators such as howlers can use seeds in critical periods of dry season.