




Did you find this useful? Give us your feedback
229 citations
101 citations
98 citations
90 citations
1,464 citations
...…to inform debates on whether Universal Grammar is fully available to older L2 learners (e.g. Lardiere, 1998a, 1998b, 2000; Prévost and White, 2000; Schwartz and Sprouse, 1996; Vainikka and Young-Scholten, 1996a) or whether it is only partially available (e.g. Hawkins and Chan, 1997; Hawkins and…...
[...]
...Such selective differences help to inform debates on whether Universal Grammar is fully available to older L2 learners (e.g. Lardiere, 1998a, 1998b, 2000; Prévost and White, 2000; Schwartz and Sprouse, 1996; Vainikka and Young-Scholten, 1996a) or whether it is only partially available (e....
[...]
920 citations
...The distinction between implicit and explicit knowledge refers to whether or not that knowledge is intuitive and available for automatic processing (implicit), or consciously available through effortful, controlled processing (explicit) (e.g. R. Ellis et al., 2006; Hulstijn, 2005)....
[...]
915 citations
...These findings fit with the view that the fronted temporal adverbial functions as a topic (Chafe, 1984; Partee, 1984; Reinhart, 1983; Virtanen, 1992) and, as such, creates a new discourse segment.4 (8) a....
[...]
[...]
859 citations
Further research is necessary to address many issues that are raised in this study. One possibility is that because lexical means for expressing temporal relations characterize early learners ’ tense/aspect productions, seemingly irrespective of the properties of the L1 ( e. g. Starren, 2001 ), then such lexical means may ultimately exert more influence in L2 learners ’ interpretations The authors leave these open questions to future research. This may be so even when the inflectional morphology appears to be in place, and can be used in real-time processing.