scispace - formally typeset
Open AccessJournal ArticleDOI

Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program.

Reads0
Chats0
TLDR
There was no effect of training on any measure of self-control and the implication is that training self- control through repeated practice does not result in generalized improvements inSelf-control.
Abstract
Can self-control be improved through practice? Several studies have found that repeated practice of tasks involving self-control improves performance on other tasks relevant to self-control. However, in many of these studies, improvements after training could be attributable to methodological factors (e.g., passive control conditions). Moreover, the extent to which the effects of training transfer to real-life settings is not yet clear. In the present research, participants (N = 174) completed a 6-week training program of either cognitive or behavioral self-control tasks. We then tested the effects of practice on a range of measures of self-control, including lab-based and real-world tasks. Training was compared with both active and no-contact control conditions. Despite high levels of adherence to the training tasks, there was no effect of training on any measure of self-control. Trained participants did not, for example, show reduced ego depletion effects, become better at overcoming their habits, or report exerting more self-control in everyday life. Moderation analyses found no evidence that training was effective only among particular groups of participants. Bayesian analyses suggested that the data were more consistent with a null effect of training on self-control than with previous estimates of the effect of practice. The implication is that training self-control through repeated practice does not result in generalized improvements in self-control. (PsycINFO Database Record

read more

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

This is a repository copy of Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a
six-week training program.
White Rose Research Online URL for this paper:
http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/107549/
Version: Accepted Version
Article:
Miles, E., Sheeran, P., Baird, H. et al. (3 more authors) (2016) Does self-control improve
with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 145 (8). pp. 1075-1091. ISSN 0096-3445
https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000185
This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the APA journal. It is not
the copy of record.
eprints@whiterose.ac.uk
https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/
Reuse
Unless indicated otherwise, fulltext items are protected by copyright with all rights reserved. The copyright
exception in section 29 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 allows the making of a single copy
solely for the purpose of non-commercial research or private study within the limits of fair dealing. The
publisher or other rights-holder may allow further reproduction and re-use of this version - refer to the White
Rose Research Online record for this item. Where records identify the publisher as the copyright holder,
users can verify any specific terms of use on the publisher’s website.
Takedown
If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by
emailing eprints@whiterose.ac.uk including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request.

1
Running head: EFFECTS OF SELF-CONTROL TRAINING
Does Self-Control Improve With Practice? Evidence from a 6-Week Training Program
Eleanor Miles, Paschal Sheeran, Harriet Baird, Ian Macdonald, Thomas L. Webb, and Peter R.
Harris
Word count: 11,056 (excluding references and tables)
Acknowledgements
This project was made possible through the support of a grant from the John Templeton
Foundation. The opinions expressed here are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect
the views of the John Templeton Foundation. We thank Zoltan Dienes for guidance on Bayesian
statistics.

2
Abstract
Can self-control be improved through practice? Several studies have found that repeated
practice of tasks involving self-control improves performance on other tasks relevant to self-
control. However, in many of these studies, improvements after training could be attributable to
methodological factors (e.g., passive control conditions). Moreover, the extent to which the
effects of training transfer to real-life settings is not yet clear. In the present research, participants
(N = 174) completed a 6-week training program of either cognitive or behavioral self-control
tasks. We then tested the effects of practice on a range of measures of self-control, including lab-
based and real-world tasks. Training was compared to both active and no-contact control
conditions. Despite high levels of adherence to the training tasks, there was no effect of training
on any measure of self-control. Trained participants did not, for example, show reduced ego
depletion effects, become better at overcoming their habits, or report exerting more self-control
in everyday life. Moderation analyses found no evidence that training was effective only among
particular groups of participants. Bayesian analyses suggested that the data was more consistent
with a null effect of training on self-control than with previous estimates of the effect of practice.
The implication is that training self-control through repeated practice does not result in
generalized improvements in self-control.
Keywords: self-control, self-regulation, intervention, ego depletion, self-control training

3
Does Self-Control Improve With Practice? Evidence from a 6-Week Training Program
Self-control, or the ability to control thoughts, behaviors, and feelings, seems to be
important for success in most areas of life (De Ridder, Lensvelt-Mulders, Finkenauer, Stok, &
Baumeister, 2012). During the past 15 years, much of the research into self-control has been
inspired by the strength model, which draws the analogy between self-control and a physical
muscle (for a review, see Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). This model proposes that, just as using
a muscle leads to temporary fatigue, exerting self-control leads to temporary reductions in self-
control performance; a phenomenon that has been termed ‘ego depletion’ (Baumeister,
Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998). However, the strength model also suggests that if self-
control is repeatedly exerted over time (interspersed with periods of rest), then the opposite
effect should occur. In other words, just as a muscle grows stronger with exercise, so self-control
should improve over time with practice (Muraven, 2010a; Muraven, Baumeister, & Tice, 1999).
The ego depletion effect has been the subject of hundreds of empirical tests, extensive
analysis of mediating and moderating factors, and much lively theoretical debate (e.g., Hagger,
Wood, Stiff, & Chatzisarantis, 2010; Inzlicht & Schmeichel, 2012; Carter, Kofler, Forster, &
McCullough, 2015). However, the strength model’s predictions about the long-term effects of
exerting self-control have received less attention. If practicing self-control improves subsequent
self-control, as has been suggested by several studies (e.g., Muraven, 2010a; Muraven et al.,
1999), then self-control training could benefit people facing everyday self-regulatory struggles
such as controlling emotions, breaking bad habits, and overcoming impulses. Yet, there are many
things that we do not yet know about the effects of training self-control through practice (see
Berkman, in press; Inzlicht & Berkman, 2015; Inzlicht, Schmeichel, & Macrae, 2014, for
discussion). For example, how reliable are training effects? Can we be sure that the observed

4
improvements are the result of practicing self-control? Perhaps most importantly from an applied
perspective, does training self-control indeed influence real-life outcomes that depend on self-
control? The present research sought to address these unanswered questions.
What Do We Already Know About the Effectiveness of Self-Control Training?
Table 1 provides an overview of prior studies of self-control training. Studies
investigating the effect of self-control training typically ask one group of participants to perform
a task requiring self-control over a period of weeks, while another group performs either no task,
or a task that does not require self-control. The performance of the two groups is then compared
on a subsequent self-control task, usually different to the one that was trained. A wide range of
tasks has been used to train self-control, such as using one's non-dominant hand, developing and
executing a personalized study or exercise program, completing the Stroop task, squeezing a
handgrip, or performing a logical reasoning task. The effect of training has also been assessed
using a wide range of tasks, such as tolerating pain, inhibiting aggressive inclinations or
behavior, ignoring a distracting video while performing a visual tracking task, solving anagrams,
or holding a handgrip. The common feature of all of these tasks is that they are believed to
require self-control.
How effective are these interventions in improving self-control performance? Hagger et
al. (2010) meta-analyzed the findings of studies that measured the effects of training on ego
depletion (i.e., performance on the second of two sequential tasks involving self-control). Across
9 tests, taken from 7 published papers, they observed that training significantly reduced ego
depletion, with an overall effect size of d
+
= 1.07. This suggests that practicing self-control is an
extremely effective intervention. To put this effect size in context, it places the effect of self-
control training on ego depletion at roughly the 95
th
percentile in terms of both the average effect

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

The Intention–Behavior Gap

TL;DR: The authors synthesize research on intention-behavior relations to address questions such as: How big is the intention−behavior gap? When are intentions more or less likely to get translated into action? What kinds of problems prevent people from realizing their intentions? And what strategies show promise in closing the intention −behavior gap and helping people do the things that they intend to do.
Journal ArticleDOI

Persistence and Fadeout in the Impacts of Child and Adolescent Interventions

TL;DR: It is argued that skill-building interventions should target “trifecta” skills—ones that are malleable, fundamental, and would not have developed eventually in the absence of the intervention.
Journal ArticleDOI

Health Behavior Change: Moving from Observation to Intervention

TL;DR: The EM approach promises progress in answering the key question that will enable the science of health behavior change to improve public health: What strategies are effective in promoting behavior change, for whom, and under what circumstances.
Journal ArticleDOI

Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and personal problems.

TL;DR: This work provides a comprehensive theoretical review and organization of a psychologically informed approach to social problems, one that encompasses a wide-range of interventions and applies to diverse problem areas.
Journal ArticleDOI

Does Self-Control Training Improve Self-Control? A Meta-Analysis.

TL;DR: There is not enough evidence to conclude that the repeated control of dominant responses is the critical element driving training effects, but a random-effects meta-analysis based on robust variance estimation of the published and unpublished literature on self-control training effects revealed a small-to-medium effect.
References
More filters
Posted Content

The Satisfaction with Life Scale

TL;DR: The Satisfaction With Life Scale is narrowly focused to assess global life satisfaction and does not tap related constructs such as positive affect or loneliness, but is shown to have favorable psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and high temporal reliability.
Journal ArticleDOI

The Satisfaction With Life Scale.

TL;DR: The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) as mentioned in this paper is a scale to measure global life satisfaction, which does not tap related constructs such as positive affect or loneliness, and has favorable psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and high temporal reliability.
Journal ArticleDOI

A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure

TL;DR: In this paper, a simple and widely accepted multiple test procedure of the sequentially rejective type is presented, i.e. hypotheses are rejected one at a time until no further rejections can be done.
Journal ArticleDOI

The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis.

TL;DR: The results suggest that it is important to recognize both the unity and diversity ofExecutive functions and that latent variable analysis is a useful approach to studying the organization and roles of executive functions.
Journal ArticleDOI

Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test.

TL;DR: An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute when instructions oblige highly associated categories to share a response key, and performance is faster than when less associated categories share a key.
Related Papers (5)
Frequently Asked Questions (16)
Q1. What contributions have the authors mentioned in the paper "Running head: effects of self-control training does self-control improve with practice? evidence from a 6-week training program" ?

In this paper, a 6-week training program of either cognitive or behavioral self-control tasks was used to test the effects of practice on a range of measures of self control, including lab-based and real-world tasks. 

As self-control influences the extent to which individuals can overcome habits and enact intentions (De Ridder et al., 2012; Neal, Wood, & Drolet, 2013), it is possible that self-control training could reduce habitual control, and increase intentional control, over behaviors. 

Sixteen participants who identified themselves as Asian were excluded from theanalysis of the effects of training on prejudice and four participants who reported that they were unable to eat chocolate were excluded from the analyses of the effects of training on chocolate consumption. 

The authors also analyzed the effects of training on outcomes using condition as a four-levelvariable (behavioral training, cognitive training, active control, no-contact control), as opposed to collapsing across training and control conditions. 

Greater self-control is associated with improvedwell-being (e.g., De Ridder et al., 2012) and training self-control might therefore be expected to enhance well-being. 

Inzlicht et al. (2014) also offer a number of other promising suggestions for improving self-control that go beyond the conception of effortful inhibition, such as changing goal appraisals and responding to self-control failures with acceptance. 

Power analyses based on the effect size estimate of d+ = 1.07 from Hagger et al.’s (2010)meta-analysis of training effects on ego depletion indicated that 40 participants would be required, split between the training and control conditions, to achieve 90% power (two-tailed). 

Training participants also reported that their tasks required more effort, but only when rated retrospectively, t(171) = 3.70, p < .001, d+ = 0.56, and not when rated immediately after each task, t(132) = 0.00, p = 1.00, d+ = 0.00. 

Although the central idea of programs designed to train self-control involves improvingpeoples’ ability to inhibit a dominant response, self-control involves more than just the effortfulinhibition of impulses (Fujita, 2011) and, as such, there may be multiple ways to improve this skill (Inzlicht, Legault, & Teper, 2014). 

The Bayes factor was 0.23, which indicates that their data are more consistent with a null effect of training on ego depletion than with the experimental hypothesis. 

A MANOVA on these four variables found no effect of condition on behavior, F(4, 168) = 0.53, Wilk's 】 = 0.99, p = .71, partial さ2 = .01. 

As such, the present research measured participants’ performance of two behaviors that have been shown to depend upon self-control resources, but have not yet been tested as training outcomes; eating chocolate and displaying prejudice. 

As correlations are not normally distributed, the authors tested for differences in the magnitude of these correlations between conditions using Mann-Whitney U tests. 

The authors also conducted hierarchical regression analyses to examine whether the association between implicit attitudes and behavior was moderated by training. 

The present findings also have implicationsfor the other key prediction of the strength model – that exerting self-control temporarily reduces self-control performance. 

Data for each of the behaviors was positively skewed, such that most participants performed the behaviors relatively infrequently.