scispace - formally typeset
Open AccessJournal ArticleDOI

A field study of the exposure-annoyance relationship of military shooting noise.

Mark Brink, +1 more
- 05 Apr 2010 - 
- Vol. 127, Iss: 4, pp 2301-2311
Reads0
Chats0
TLDR
The sound exposure level L(E) of shooting noise better explained variations in annoyance than other operational and/or acoustical predictors.
Abstract
This article reports a field study on noise annoyance from military shooting with small, midsize, and heavy weapons that was carried out among 1002 residents living near eight different training grounds of the Swiss army. The goal of the study was to derive the exposure-annoyance relationship for military shooting noise in communities in the vicinity of average military training grounds. Annoyance was determined in a telephone survey by means of the 5-point verbal and 11-point numerical annoyance scale recommended by the International Commission on Biological Effects of Noise. Exposure was calculated using acoustical source models of weapons and numbers of shots fired, as recorded by the army. Annoyance predictor variables investigated were LAE, LCE, LCE−LAE, number of shots above threshold, as well as individual moderators. Exposure-annoyance relationships were modeled by means of linear and logistic regression analyses. The sound exposure level LE of shooting noise better explained variations in annoyan...

read more

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

A field study of the exposure-annoyance relationship of military
shooting noise
Mark Brink
a
D-MTEC Public and Organizational Health, ETH Zurich, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Jean-Marc Wunderli
Laboratory of Acoustics, Empa Swiss Federal Laboratories for Materials Testing and Research, CH-8600
Duebendorf, Switzerland
Received 20 April 2009; revised 15 December 2009; accepted 6 February 2010
This article reports a field study on noise annoyance from military shooting with small, midsize, and
heavy weapons that was carried out among 1002 residents living near eight different training
grounds of the Swiss army. The goal of the study was to derive the exposure-annoyance relationship
for military shooting noise in communities in the vicinity of average military training grounds.
Annoyance was determined in a telephone survey by means of the 5-point verbal and 11-point
numerical annoyance scale recommended by the International Commission on Biological Effects of
Noise. Exposure was calculated using acoustical source models of weapons and numbers of shots
fired, as recorded by the army. Annoyance predictor variables investigated were L
AE
, L
CE
, L
CE
L
AE
, number of shots above threshold, as well as individual moderators. Exposure-annoyance
relationships were modeled by means of linear and logistic regression analyses. The sound exposure
level L
E
of shooting noise better explained variations in annoyance than other operational and/or
acoustical predictors. Annoyance on the 5-point scale was more closely related to noise exposure
than expressed on the 11-point scale. The inclusion of the C-A frequency weighting difference as a
second explaining variable, as suggested earlier, did not substantially enhance the predictability of
high annoyance. © 2010 Acoustical Society of America. DOI: 10.1121/1.3337234
PACS numbers: 43.50.Qp, 43.50.Pn, 43.50.Sr BSF Pages: 2301–2311
I. INTRODUCTION
A. Study rationale
Exposure-response relationships are commonly used to
assess the annoyance impact of many kinds of traffic or in-
dustrial noise. In their most common form, they relate noise
exposure to the percentage of highly annoyed persons
%HA. As military shooting noise as a result of military
training activities in times of peace is less of a problem for
the majority of the population, there exist only a few field
studies in the literature that investigated its effects. Hence the
impact of military shooting noise from training grounds of
armies is far less well understood than effects of other noise
sources. The goals of the current study were thus the estab-
lishment of a statistical model that explains variation of com-
munity annoyance by operational and acoustical descriptors
of military shooting activity and to provide an exposure-
effect function for high annoyance %HA among residents
in the vicinity of typical military training grounds in Swit-
zerland.
Despite a relatively large body of literature, which
mostly pertains to laboratory studies
Meloni and Rosen-
heck, 1995
; Schomer et al., 1994; Vos, 2001, 2003; Vos and
Geurtsen, 2003
, there have only few exposure-effect func-
tions for military shooting noise been published so far e.g.,
in
Schomer, 1985. In the real-world situation, people use
adaptive mechanisms that try to ignore noise as much as
possible, whereas in a laboratory setting they do the opposite
and inevitably concentrate on the noise. This provides a
strong rationale to investigate shooting noise effects in the
field, at the homes of the affected population.
The current study was carried out in Switzerland, where
one can find several multipurpose training grounds where
military shooting activity comprises small, middle, and
heavy weapon shooting in one and the same place, and, be-
cause plain space is very scant, often in close vicinity to
inhabited areas. This specific geographic situation therefore
appears to be well suited to investigate military shooting
noise annoyance by means of a field study. At this point, the
notion is relevant that this study is not primarily about the
effects of large army weapons such as tanks or artillery, since
the number of rounds of such types of weapons each year is
considerably lower than from small caliber arms.
B. Shooting noise descriptors and exposure-effect
relationships
While exposure assessment following the equal energy
principle has been adopted for the most distinctive noise
sources, at least pertaining to annoyance as dependent vari-
able, no commonly accepted noise descriptor for assessing
community annoyance to shooting noise has successfully es-
tablished itself to date. Of the few field studies on commu-
nity annoyance due to weapon noise at hand
Buchta and
Vos, 1998
; Bullen and Hede, 1982; Fidell et al., 1983; Lev-
ein and Ahrlin, 1988; Rylander and Lundquist, 1996;
a
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Electronic mail:
brink@ethz.ch
J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 127 4, April 2010 © 2010 Acoustical Society of America 23010001-4966/2010/1274/2301/11/$25.00
EMPA20100095

Schomer, 1985; Schomer et al., 1994; Sorensen and Magnus-
son, 1979
, only few exposure-effect functions explaining
annoyance due to a mixture of different kinds of army weap-
ons emerged.
In the literature, noise descriptors that were identified to
yield the highest degrees of explained variance of annoyance
from impulsive sounds vary from accumulated peak level
Bullen et al., 1991, maximum sound pressure level Levein
and Ahrlin, 1988
, A-weighted FAST maximum sound pres-
sure level
Sorensen and Magnusson, 1979, number of shots
above a C-weighted threshold level
Rylander and Lun-
dquist, 1996
, C-weighted average day-night level L
CDN
Schomer, 1985, and Schomers Schomer, 1994 “new de-
scriptor for high-energy impulsive sounds”
Buchta and Vos,
1998
, the L
Aeq
, to even surrogate measurements of ground
vibration in the case of blast noise from surface mines
Fidell
et al., 1983
. Most of these studies investigated the noise
effect from particular source weapon types, either from,
e.g., rifle shooting ranges or from large weapon training fa-
cilities. Shooting with firearms on multipurpose training
grounds with different combinations of small to very large
caliber weapons creates a complex blend of different sounds.
It therefore appears that the construction of an all-purpose
exposure-effect curve regarding military shooting noise is
much more difficult than for other more uniform noise types.
Depending on the predominant weapon type used, one or the
other noise descriptor probably better predicts community
annoyance. For example, noise annoyance from large weap-
ons which also elicit rattle and vibrations might better be
predicted using a C-weighted measure than an A-weighted
measure. The question which predictor best accounts for the
variation of military shooting noise annoyance in general,
that means for any kind and combination of weapons, cannot
easily be answered.
C. Frequency weighting
The question of the choice of frequency weighting to
best predict impulsive or weapon noise annoyance respec-
tively has received considerable attention in the literature.
Insights into the relationship between shots of weapons and
annoyance, especially with regard to impulse correction and
frequency weighting have been collected in a series of labo-
ratory studies
Meloni and Rosenheck, 1995; Schomer and
Wagner, 1995
; Schomer et al., 1994; Vos, 1990, 2001. The
use of the A-weighting is widespread in the evaluation of
gunfire noise from small arms, usually including a penalty
correction of between 5 and 12 dB for the added annoyance
of impulsive sounds
Buchta, 1990; Vos, 1990. However,
for the assessment of large caliber or high-energy weapon
noise, the C weighting and the measure L
CE
or L
CDN
have
been suggested in the past
Schomer, 1986 or are recom-
mended in ISO 1996-1
International Standards Organisa-
tion, 2003
. The assessment methodology applied in many
European countries uses L
AF,max
Germany, Switzerland or
L
AI,max
Austria, Finland, Denmark, Norway, and Sweden
for small arms, and C-weighted measures such as L
CE
Fin-
land, Norway, Sweden and L
Ceq
Germany, The Nether-
lands, and Denmark for large weapons.
For the whole set of impulse sound types produced by
various firearms ranging in caliber from 7.62 to 155 mm, the
annoyance rating in the laboratory study of
Vo s 2001 was
almost entirely determined by the “outdoor” L
AE
of the im-
pulses, as long as the artificial laboratory situation reflected a
scenario with open windows. Similar results were reported
by
Meloni and Rosenheck 1995 who found that if shooting
noise is predominantly heard through open windows, the
A-weighted sound exposure level is appropriate for predict-
ing annoyance.
Vo s 2001 suggested to include the difference between
the C- and A-weighted levels as a second annoyance predic-
tor alongside the A-weighted level as principal predictor
Vos, 2001. Because the addition of the C-weighted level in
the regression equations in most instances only very slightly
increased the explained variance of the exposure-effect rela-
tionship, it remains arguable, whether the additional effort of
C-weighted measurements and/or calculations is justified,
particularly for the assessment of the “outside situation,” as
Vo s 2001, 2003 demonstrated in his laboratory studies. It is
therefore desirable to empirically test the advantage of the
incorporation of C-weighted measurements not only in the
laboratory but also within the scope of community reaction
surveys in the field, such as the present one.
II. METHODS
A. Sampling procedure
Depending on the site-specific combinations of
weapons/ammunition used, average distances of dwellings
from the shooting ground, the degree of visibility of army
activities in the surrounding neighborhood, involvement with
the army e.g., as employee, and many other factors, one
would expect exposure-effect relationships for annoyance to
show a rather wide variation. As the primary goal of the
study was collecting data for constructing an exposure-effect
relationship, a representative amount of residents near the
eight largest training grounds of the Swiss army, that were
located sufficiently close to inhabited areas to potentially
evoke annoyance reactions from noise, were sampled. The
corresponding sites were the army training grounds of Bière,
Thun, Wangen an der Aaare, Gehren-Erlinsbach, Krähtal-
Riniken, Walenstadt, Herisau-Gossau, and Chur. At each of
these eight sites, the exposure contours from preliminary ex-
posure calculations that did not account for elevation above
ground and shielding effects from neighboring buildings
were used to assign exposure values to building addresses
using a GIS system provided by the Swiss statistics office.
The exposure was calculated as the yearly sound exposure
level L
AE
, i.e., the total acoustic energy resulting from shoot-
ing activity during an entire year. At each of the eight sites,
the primary sampling area was defined as the area that was
enclosed by the 104 dB L
AE
exposure contour. Each address
was then assigned an exposure stratum 104–107, 107–110,
110–113, 113–116, 116–119, 119–122, 122–125, 125–128,
and 128 dB. Over all eight sites, a total of 5901 building
addresses within the 104 dBA contour were identified.
These addresses were aligned with a commercial address da-
tabase to yield all available landline telephone numbers of
2302 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 127, No. 4, April 2010 M. Brink and J. M. Wunderli: Annoyance from military shooting noise

households. 5851 individual telephone numbers were identi-
fied. The telephone numbers were stored together with their
exposure level category and served as the primary sample.
The survey was carried out by computer assisted telephone
interviews CATIs. Within each household, one person over
16 years of age was selected using a modified Troldahl–
Carter method
Troldahl and Carter, 1964. The CATI soft-
ware was configured to try to sample equal amounts of sub-
jects in the different exposure strata, as far as possible. 5851
individual numbers were called. A total of 1002 interviews
could be realized. 2137 calls were either never answered or
were not valid due to technical reasons e.g., a FAX device at
the other end of the line. Of the 3714 remaining calls that
resulted in a voice contact, the following statistics apply:
Valid interviews conducted: 27%; interview scheduled, but
did not take place for unknown reasons: 8%; communication
or language problems make interview impossible: 4%; no
target person living in household: 2%; person called refused
interview: 59%.
B. Telephone interviews
Interviews lasted about 15–20 min and took place during
the evening hours of September, October, and November
2007. The schedule moved gradually from questions about
the satisfaction with the immediate environment to the topic
of military shooting noise. The true aim of the survey was
disclosed to all interviewees only after the interview was
finished and they were given the opportunity to withdraw, an
option no one exercised.
For the interviews, a questionnaire was used that first
asked about various criteria of living quality of the inter-
viewee, among them, noise exposure and annoyance from
different sources five-point verbal scale, including military
shooting noise. These were asked in random order of the
sources, followed by the items of the short form of the
“Lärmempfindlichkeitsfragebogen” LEFK; English: “Noise
sensitivity questionnaire” by
Zimmer and Ellermeier 1998
to assess noise sensitivity. In the middle of the interview, the
main block about military shooting noise exposure and an-
noyance was placed. This main block of questions included
the German version of the 11-point annoyance scale from 0
to 10 recommended by the International Commission on
Biological Effects of Noise ICBEN that were published by
Fields et al. 2001, a question about strategies to cope with
the noise, and three items about the respondent’s attitude
toward the army these items were “Switzerland does need
an army,” “The Swiss army sufficiently cares for the envi-
ronment,” and “Military shooting noise is a necessary evil”
that had do be answered ona1to5scale with the end points
“totally agree” and “totally disagree.”
C. Exposure assessment
After the selection of the eight study sites and the col-
lection of the survey data, the relevant source data for the
final high detail noise exposure calculations were collected
from army officials that were in command of the respective
training grounds. Their task basically encompassed the re-
porting of the weapons and ammunitions used, the corre-
sponding number of shots and shooting days, as well as the
distribution of shots fired between day and evening night
shootings were very rare. Each weapon/ammunition combi-
nation was assigned one of the following categories: small
caliber 10 mm, e.g., assault rifles, middle caliber 10–
100 mm, e.g., antiaircraft guns, large caliber = 100 mm,
e.g., large tank cannons, grenades and explosive charges,
mortars, and practice ammunition.
For all receiver points in the survey, the exposure from
every emplacement/weapon/ammunition combination of the
respective study site was calculated using the “WL04”
source and propagation model developed by the Swiss Fed-
eral Laboratories for Materials Testing and Research Empa.
This model delivers exposure spectra in octave bands from
31.5 Hz to 4 kHz of direct and reflected sounds as well as for
each source and receiver combination and for up to 16 dis-
tinct weather conditions that were derived for each study site
based on long-term weather statistics the 16 Hz octave band
was omitted as it does not relevantly contribute to the total
exposure, even for large weapons. The model accounts for
three types of sound sources: muzzle blasts, sonic booms,
and detonations. Receiver points were set on the facade of
the building aiming at the shooting ground. The height of the
receiver points was set to 1.8 m for detached houses and
ground floor apartments. For each additional floor, the height
was increased by 2.6 m. Exposure calculations were per-
formed separately for the years 2004, 2005, and 2006 and
separately for daytime and evening shootings. Shootings in
the night past 23:00 h were extremely rare, as were shootings
during weekends.
The total yearly exposure levels were calculated as the
sum of the energetic products of each emplacement/weapon/
ammunition sound exposure level with their corresponding
number of shots fired in the respective year.
As the timely distribution of the intensity of shooting
often varies considerably across a year, a daily average
exposure value, e.g., a 12 h L
eq
ora24hL
eq
, does not in
most cases reflect a meaningful description of the noise ex-
posure residents are affected with. Dose values in this article
are therefore simply given as L
E
values, representing the to-
tal integrated energy of shooting noise exposure in a year
or as the average over 3 years. A corresponding energy
equivalent continuous level over a particular time period can
be obtained by transforming the given L
E
value, e.g., using
L
eq
= L
E
10 logN
SD
N
HD
3600,
1
where L
eq
is equivalent sound level for a particular number
of hours of a particular number of days within a year, N
SD
is number of days in a year when shootings/trainings take
place, N
HD
is number of hours per day for which the average
sound level should be calculated e.g., 12. For example, the
average daily 12 h L
eq
would thus be L
E
10 log36512
3600.
III. RESULTS
A. Sample description
A total of 460 male 46% and 542 female 54% par-
ticipants constituted the sample of 1002 residents. Shooting
J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 127, No. 4, April 2010 M. Brink and J. M. Wunderli: Annoyance from military shooting noise 2303

noise exposure was calculated for 918 distinct receiver
points. For a small number of the receiver points, more than
one respondent were interviewed e.g., more than one family
member living in the same apartment. 232 interviews were
made in the French speaking part of Switzerland. Respon-
dents were in the age range from 16 to 94 years. The average
age of the respondents was 50 years. The age class distribu-
tion was as follows in parentheses are the percentages of the
population older than 16: between 16 and 20 years: 5%
5%; 20–40: 25% 34%; 40–60: 37% 34%; and older
than 60 years: 33% 26%.
The respondents experienced yearly military shooting
noise exposure levels at their homes between 92 and 130 dB
L
AE
or 98 and 141 dB L
CE
, respectively. Unlike the quite
simple preliminary calculations that were used for sample
stratification and definition of the address sampling areas, the
definitive exposure calculation for each respondent ac-
counted for the elevation above ground and shielding effects
from other buildings; thus yearly L
AE
levels down to 92 dB
were reached in the sample. Table
I shows the distribution of
the number of telephone interviews that were realized per
L
AE
exposure level category as 3 year energetic average
and study site.
Table
II shows the yearly average number of shots as
well as the number of shots above the 50, 60, 70, and 80 dB
L
AE
thresholds per weapon type, as experienced at the 918
receiver points in the sample. The figures given in the last
four columns represent the average number of shots above
the respective threshold, which is defined as the average
A-weighted sound exposure level of one individual shot of a
distinct source more clearly the emplacement/weapon/
ammunition combination at the receiver points within the
study sample, as the average of the 3 years 2004, 2005, and
2006.
The average shooting activity per year was about the
same for all 3 years and no substantial changes have oc-
curred at any of the eight grounds between 2004 and 2006.
B. Annoyance ratings and exposure metrics
In light of the different approaches to define high annoy-
ance and for reasons of comparability, both ICBEN scales to
assess high annoyance in the respondent
Fields et al.,
2001
were part of the interview. Concerning the five-point
verbal scale, ICBEN’s recommendation is to use the upper
two categories the verbal marks “very” and “extremely” as
indicators of high annoyance. This corresponds to a cutoff
point at 60% of the scale. No recommendation is given for
the 11-point scale, but according to common practice, the
upper three points on the numerical scale 8, 9, 10 define the
presence of “high annoyance” in the respondent. In this case,
the cutoff lies at 72.7% see Schultz, 1978. In total, on the
11-point numerical scale, 170 of 1002 respondents qualified
as highly annoyed, on the 5-point scale 241 of 1002.
The annoyance questions were asked in the following
order: the first time during the interview using the 5-point
verbal scale with the marks “not at all,” “slightly,” “moder-
ately,” “very,” and “extremely” within a block of noise an-
noyance questions for different noise sources, and the second
time later during the interview using the 11-point numerical
scale. For all further quantitative analyses, the verbal answer
alternatives of the five-point scale have been transformed to
numerical values 1–5 and treated as continuous.
TABLE I. Number of interviews conducted at each study site and per exposure category.
Study
site
90–95 dB
L
AE
95–100 dB
L
AE
100–105 dB
L
AE
105–110 dB
L
AE
110–115 dB
L
AE
115–120 dB
L
AE
120–125 dB
L
AE
125–130 dB
L
AE
Bière 14 21 50 42 51 42 10
Chur 1 15 62 56 23 2
Gehren-Erlinsbach 2 6 11 8 2
Herisau-Gossau 11 25 20 7 1 2
Krähtal-Riniken 1 5 25 16 10 1
Thun 72759925234 8
Wangen an der Aare 15 10 8 12 3
Walenstadt 25 28 46 34
Total 16 101 233 258 237 102 45 10
Percent 1.60 10.08 23.25 25.75 23.65 10.18 4.49 1.00
TABLE II. Number of shots and number of shots above threshold at the 918 receiver points in the sample all values represent the yearly average over the
years 2004, 2005, and 2006.
Type of weapon/
ammunition
No. of shots
during day
No. of shots
during evening
No. of shots
L
AE
=50 dB
No. of shots
L
AE
=60 dB
No. of shots
L
AE
=70 dB
No. of shots
L
AE
=80 dB
Large caliber 5 088 179 2 119 1 701 834 207
Middle caliber 336 351 11 808 38 141 18 954 17 194 14 699
Small caliber 8 554 533 532 128 303 277 179 902 73 783 0
Practice ammunition 32 650 4 862 0000
Grenades/explosive charges 17 163 1 065 2 356 1 712 816 471
Mortars 6 443 583 1 514 1 271 1 266 737
2304 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 127, No. 4, April 2010 M. Brink and J. M. Wunderli: Annoyance from military shooting noise

Using linear regression models, it was first assessed
which exposure metrics appear to be the best predictors of
annoyance. The following potential predictors were investi-
gated: Total energetic and arithmetic average over 3 years
sound exposure levels L
AE
and L
CE
, energetic and arith-
metic average sound exposure level L
AE
and L
CE
during
day and during evenings, energetic average sound exposure
level L
AE
and L
CE
of small caliber shots and of large caliber
shots, total number of small caliber shots over 50 dB L
AE
,
and total number of large caliber shots over 98 dB L
CE
. From
these preliminary analyses, it became evident that the basic
energetic dose measures L
AE
and L
CE
are the best predictors
for shooting noise annoyance. Table
III tabulates the mean
annoyance rating per exposure level category as well as the
percentage of highly annoyed persons %HA in each cat-
egory, according to the “standard” cutoff points 5-point:
60%; 11-point: 72.7% on the scales.
Annoyance is an increasing function of the sound expo-
sure level up to the exposure level category of 115–120 dB
L
AE
. Contrary to expectation, within the higher level catego-
ries 120–125 and 125–130 dB L
AE
, mean annoyance as
well as the percentage of highly annoyed persons %HA
drop to a level close to the level reported by respondents that
are 15 or even 20 dB less exposed. This could be explained
by some types of self-selection process being at work insofar
as people not being annoyed by military shooting noise are
over-represented in areas close to military shooting grounds,
maybe because they are less sensitive to noise and/or have a
more positive attitude toward the army, e.g., because they are
army employees that live in the vicinity of their employer.
This explanation appears feasible since a noise sensitivity
as measured by the LEFK is a significant negative predictor
of the exposure, as expressed in the L
AE
in linear regression
analysis
=−0.11, t1000 = 2.33, p = 0.02; b annoy-
ance, as measured using the five-point verbal scale, and atti-
tude toward the army an index value between 1 and 5 with
higher values denominating a more positive attitude, derived
from items of the questionnaire, see Sec. II B is negatively
correlated within the sample r=−0.28; p 0.0001. b
Furthermore, in general linear modeling of annoyance five-
point verbal scale, both L
AE
and attitude independently pre-
dict annoyance L
AE
:F1= 94.23, p 0.0001; attitude:
F1= 89.64, p 0.0001, whereas attitude is negatively re-
lated to annoyance in this model.
The annoyance ratings showed considerable variability
as can bee estimated from the confidence intervals reported
in Table
III. Linear regression results of the individual data
not the grouped data for the 11-point numerical scale
yielded R
2
values of less than 0.05, and the 5-point verbal
scale yielded an adjusted R
2
value of 0.08 for both L
AE
and
L
CE
as predictor. While with transportation noise, on the in-
dividual level, R
2
values between 0.1 and 0.2 are common,
the marginal relationship found with military shooting noise
is no surprise, assuming that individual moderators more
strongly influence the annoyance rating than would be the
case with transportation noise.
TABLE III. Mean annoyance and percent highly annoyed %HA for different degrees of exposure. The
categories are defined based on the L
AE
metric, the average exposure values -L
AE
and -L
CE
pertain to the
arithmetic average of all cases within the category boundaries. N refers to the number of cases in each exposure
level category.
Level category
range of L
AE
values Scale Mean annoyance CI −95% CI +95% St. dev. %HA
90–95 N =16 11-point 0,,10 3.38 2.12 4.63 2.36 6.25
-L
AE
=93.64 5-point 1,,5 1.81 1.46 2.16 0.66 0.00
-L
CE
=114.87
95–100 N =101 11-point 0,,10 2.83 2.27 3.40 2.86 7.92
-L
AE
=98.07 5-point 1,,5 2.10 1.89 2.31 1.05 7.92
-L
CE
=111.50
100–105 N =233 11-point 0,,10 3.83 3.47 4.19 2.77 10.73
-L
AE
=102.67 5-point 1,,5 2.41 2.26 2.55 1.12 15.45
-L
CE
=116.37
105–110 N =258 11-point 0,,10 4.14 3.77 4.51 3.00 16.28
-L
AE
=107.53 5-point 1,,5 2.53 2.38 2.68 1.20 23.26
-L
CE
=119.66
110–115 N =237 11-point 0,,10 4.65 4.26 5.03 3.03 22.78
-L
AE
=112.30 5-point 1,,5 2.97 2.82 3.13 1.24 32.07
-L
CE
=123.77
115–120 N =102 11-point 0,10 5.35 4.78 5.92 2.91 28.43
-L
AE
=117.32 5-point 1,,5 3.41 3.18 3.65 1.20 43.14
-L
CE
=129.21
120–125 N =45 11-point 0,,10 5.09 4.30 5.88 2.63 22.22
-L
AE
=122.21 5-point 1,,5 3.11 2.78 3.44 1.09 35.56
-L
CE
=131.40
125–130 N =10 11-point 010 3.90 1.68 6.12 3.11 10.00
-L
AE
=127.74 5-point 1,,5 2.60 2.00 3.20 0.84 10.00
-L
CE
=134.09
J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 127, No. 4, April 2010 M. Brink and J. M. Wunderli: Annoyance from military shooting noise 2305

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

WHO Environmental Noise Guidelines for the European Region: A Systematic Review on Environmental Noise and Annoyance

TL;DR: The increase of %HA in newer studies of aircraft, road and railway noise at comparable Lden levels of earlier studies point to the necessity of adjusting noise limit recommendations.
Journal ArticleDOI

The association between road traffic noise exposure, annoyance and Health-Related Quality of Life (HRQOL)

TL;DR: Investigating the relationships between road traffic noise exposure, annoyance caused by different noise sources and validated health indicators in a cohort of 1375 adults revealed that the association between physical noise exposure and health-related quality of life (HRQOL) is strongly mediated by annoyance and sleep disturbance.
Journal ArticleDOI

Human response to vibration in residential environments

TL;DR: The main aim of this study was to derive exposure-response relationships for annoyance due to vibration from environmental sources, and use of relevant frequency weightings was found to improve correlation between vibration exposure and annoyance.
Journal ArticleDOI

Effect of situational, attitudinal and demographic factors on railway vibration annoyance in residential areas.

TL;DR: It was found that annoyance scores were strongly influenced by two attitudinal factors: Concern of property damage and expectations about future levels of vibration, which indicate that future railway vibration policies and regulations focusing on community impact need to consider additional factors for an optimal assessment of railway effects on residential environments.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

Maximising response rates in household telephone surveys

TL;DR: The use of a combination of approaches, such as an advance letter, interviewer training, establishment of researcher credentials, increasing call attempts and targeted call times, remains a good strategy to maximise telephone response rates.
Journal ArticleDOI

On the annoyance caused by impulse sounds produced by small, medium-large, and large firearms.

TL;DR: The present results showed that for the entire set of impulse sounds rated indoors with windows closed, the rating sound level, Lr, is given by Lr=LAE +12dB+beta(LCE-LAE)(LAE-alpha), with alpha=45dB and beta=0.015dB(-1).
Journal ArticleDOI

A field survey on the annoyance caused by sounds from large firearms and road traffic.

TL;DR: Results from the present highly controlled field survey provided a new opportunity to optimize the parameter values in Schomer's rating procedure y = (1/beta)(LCE-PNSE) + PNSE, in which the noise exposure for impulsive sounds (y) is expressed as the A-weighted SEL of equally annoying vehicle sounds.
Journal ArticleDOI

A- and C-weighted sound levels as predictors of the annoyance caused by shooting sounds, for various façade attenuation types

TL;DR: It was concluded that for the determination of the rating sound level, the acoustic parameters ASEL and CSEL are very powerful.
Related Papers (5)
Frequently Asked Questions (3)
Q1. What are the contributions in "A field study of the exposure-annoyance relationship of military shooting noise" ?

This article reports a field study on noise annoyance from military shooting with small, midsize, and heavy weapons that was carried out among 1002 residents living near eight different training grounds of the Swiss army. The goal of the study was to derive the exposure-annoyance relationship for military shooting noise in communities in the vicinity of average military training grounds. The inclusion of the C-A frequency weighting difference as a second explaining variable, as suggested earlier, did not substantially enhance the predictability of high annoyance. 

The inclusion of this second predictor is based on the idea that for large weapons with considerable low frequency content, the A-weighted level alone does not sufficiently account for the variation in annoyance. 

This main block of questions included the German version of the 11-point annoyance scale from 0 to 10 recommended by the International Commission on Biological Effects of Noise ICBEN that were published by Fields et al. 

Trending Questions (1)
Which is better Skullcandy or noise?

The sound exposure level L(E) of shooting noise better explained variations in annoyance than other operational and/or acoustical predictors.