scispace - formally typeset
Open AccessJournal ArticleDOI

A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention

TLDR
The first evidence in humans for a larger parieto-frontal network in the right than left hemisphere is reported, and a significant correlation between the degree of anatomical lateralization and asymmetry of performance on visuospatial tasks is reported.
Abstract
Right hemisphere dominance for visuospatial attention is characteristic of most humans, but its anatomical basis remains unknown We report the first evidence in humans for a larger parieto-frontal network in the right than left hemisphere, and a significant correlation between the degree of anatomical lateralization and asymmetry of performance on visuospatial tasks Our results suggest that hemispheric specialization is associated with an unbalanced speed of visuospatial processing

read more

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

HAL Id: hal-00680170
https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00680170
Submitted on 18 Mar 2012
HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access
archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci-
entic research documents, whether they are pub-
lished or not. The documents may come from
teaching and research institutions in France or
abroad, or from public or private research centers.
L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est
destinée au dépôt et à la diusion de documents
scientiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non,
émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de
recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires
publics ou privés.
A Lateralized Brain Network for Visuospatial Attention
Michel Thiebaut de Schotten, Flavio Dell’Acqua, Stephanie Forkel, Andrew
Simmons, Francesco Vergani, Declan G.M. Murphy, Marco Catani
To cite this version:
Michel Thiebaut de Schotten, Flavio Dell’Acqua, Stephanie Forkel, Andrew Simmons, Francesco Ver-
gani, et al.. A Lateralized Brain Network for Visuospatial Attention. Nature Neuroscience, Nature
Publishing Group, 2011, �10.1038/nn.2905�. �hal-00680170�

1
A Lateralized Brain Network for Visuospatial Attention
Michel Thiebaut de Schotten *
1,2,3†
, Flavio Dell’Acqua
1,3,4†
, Stephanie Forkel
1
,
Andrew Simmons
3,4,5
, Francesco Vergani
6
, Declan G.M. Murphy
1
and Marco Catani
1,3
1
Natbrainlab, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of
Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
2
INSERM–UPMC UMR S 975, G.H. Pitié–Salpêtrière, Paris, France
3
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London,
London, UK
4
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health at South London and
Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London Institute of Psychiatry,
UK
5
MRC Centre for Neurodegeneration Research, King’s College London, UK
6
Department of Neurosurgery, Royal Victoria Infirmary, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
Contributed Equally to this work
*Corresponding Author: Michel Thiebaut de Schotten, Natbrainlab, Department of
Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King's College, Institute of Psychiatry,
16 De Crespigny Park, SE5 8AF London UK. Email: michel.thiebaut@gmail.com

2
ABSTRACT
Right hemisphere dominance for visuospatial attention is characteristic for most
humans, but its anatomical basis remains unknown. We report the first evidence in
humans for a larger parieto–frontal network in the right than left hemisphere; and a
significant correlation between the degree of anatomical lateralization and asymmetry
of performance on visuospatial tasks. Our results suggest that hemispheric
specialization is associated with an unbalanced speed of visuospatial processing.

3
Clinical studies in patients with brain lesions, and decades of neuropsychological
testing in split–brain patients, have led to the assumption that visuospatial attention is
a function of the right hemisphere
1, 2
. Nevertheless, right visuospatial neglect is not an
infrequent finding in patients with left hemisphere damage
3
suggesting that
visuospatial attention is probably a bilateral function; with right hemisphere
dominance in most, but not all, humans
4
. However, the anatomical basis of the
hemispheric dominance for visuospatial attention is largely unknown.
In the monkey brain the activity of neurons dedicated to visuospatial attention has
been recorded simultaneously using multiple electrodes implanted in the parietal and
frontal cortex
5
. Axonal tracing studies have shown that these neurons are directly
linked through a system of connections running longitudinally in the dorsolateral
regions of the brain
6
. Recent structural
7
and functional
8
imaging studies provide
indirect evidence of a similar system in the human brain. However, the hemispheric
lateralization of the parieto–frontal connections in the human brain and its correlation
with visuospatial performances has never been demonstrated.
In this study, we performed virtual in vivo dissections of the parieto–frontal
connections in 20 right–handed subjects (11 males, 9 females) using diffusion
imaging tractography based on spherical deconvolution
9
.
A comparison between our human tractography dissections and the corresponding
reconstructions from a monkey atlas
6
that we modified are illustrated in Fig. 1.
Overall, parieto–frontal connections of the human and the monkey brain are similarly
organized in three longitudinal parieto–frontal tracts separated into a dorsal superior

4
longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) I, middle SLF II and ventral SLF III (see supplementary
material)
10
.
By measuring the volumes of the tracts in both hemispheres we were able to show a
dorsal to ventral gradient of lateralization of the SLF (Fig. 2a). The SLF I is
symmetrically distributed between left and right hemispheres (t
(19)
< 1); the SLF II
shows a trend of right lateralization (t
(19)
= 1.141; p = 0.268) and the SLF III is right
lateralized (t
(19)
= 6.083; p < 0.001).
To test whether this lateralization was related to the attentional bias for one visual
hemi–field, we asked the participants to perform a line bisection test
11
. In the general
population a small left deviation in the line bisection test is observed and referred to
as the ‘pseudoneglect effect’
11
. Consistent with previous studies
12
, our participants
deviated towards the left at a group level (– 1.53 ± 2.02mm ; t
(19)
= 3.148 ; p = 0.005).
In the majority of participants the correlation analysis indicates that larger SLF II
volumes on the right hemisphere corresponded to a greater deviation to the left in the
line bisection (r = – 0.734; p < 0.001)(Fig. 2b). Importantly the three subjects
deviating to the right showed an opposite pattern of lateralization (i.e. larger volume
of the left SLF II). Correlations with the SLF I (r = 0.258; p = 0.286) and the SLF III
(r = – 0.295; p = 0.220) were not statistically significant.
It is unknown how differences between the two hemispheres in SLF II volume can
lead to asymmetrical processing of visual scenes. Larger tract in the right hemisphere
could depend on a number of factors, including greater fiber myelination, higher
number of axons and larger axonal diameter that are correlated with the conduction
speed
13, 14
. In light of these previous works, we suggest that the left deviation in the

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

The challenge of mapping the human connectome based on diffusion tractography

Klaus H. Maier-Hein, +76 more
TL;DR: The encouraging finding that most state-of-the-art algorithms produce tractograms containing 90% of the ground truth bundles (to at least some extent) is reported, however, the same tractograms contain many more invalid than valid bundles, and half of these invalid bundles occur systematically across research groups.
Journal ArticleDOI

Attentional routes to conscious perception

TL;DR: Evidence is shown that distinct sorts of spatial attention can have different effects on visual conscious perception, and Fronto-parietal networks important for spatial attention constitute plausible neural substrates for the interactions between exogenous spatial attention and conscious perception.
Journal ArticleDOI

Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems Distinct Neural Circuits but Collaborative Roles

TL;DR: It is concluded that neither of the two networks controls attentional processes in isolation and that the flexible interaction between both systems enables the dynamic control of attention in relation to top-down goals and bottom-up sensory stimulation.
Journal ArticleDOI

Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex

TL;DR: Functional MRI evidence from single-subject analyses for broad functional generality of a specific set of brain regions is presented: the same sets of voxels are engaged across tasks ranging from arithmetic to storing information in working memory, to inhibiting irrelevant information.
Journal ArticleDOI

Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts

TL;DR: An atlas of human frontal association connections is produced that is compared with axonal tracing studies of the monkey brain and reports several similarities between human and monkey in the cingulum, uncinate, superior longitudinal fasciculus, frontal aslant tract and orbito-polar tract.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain

TL;DR: Evidence for partially segregated networks of brain areas that carry out different attentional functions is reviewed, finding that one system is involved in preparing and applying goal-directed selection for stimuli and responses, and the other is specialized for the detection of behaviourally relevant stimuli.
Journal ArticleDOI

Orienting of attention

TL;DR: This paper explores one aspect of cognition through the use of a simple model task in which human subjects are asked to commit attention to a position in visual space other than fixation by orienting a covert mechanism that seems sufficiently time locked to external events that its trajectory can be traced across the visual field in terms of momentary changes in the efficiency of detecting stimuli.
Journal ArticleDOI

MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging.

TL;DR: Once Deff is estimated from a series of NMR pulsed-gradient, spin-echo experiments, a tissue's three orthotropic axes can be determined and the effective diffusivities along these orthotropic directions are the eigenvalues of Deff.
Journal ArticleDOI

Three-dimensional tracking of axonal projections in the brain by magnetic resonance imaging.

TL;DR: It is shown that neuronal pathways in the rat brain can be probed in situ using high‐resolution three‐dimensional diffusion magnetic resonance imaging and a newly designed tracking approach.
Journal ArticleDOI

In vivo fiber tractography using DT-MRI data

TL;DR: Fiber tract trajectories in coherently organized brain white matter pathways were computed from in vivo diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging (DT‐MRI) data, and the method holds promise for elucidating architectural features in other fibrous tissues and ordered media.
Related Papers (5)
Frequently Asked Questions (5)
Q1. What are the contributions mentioned in the paper "A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention" ?

HAL this paper is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of scientific research documents, whether they are published or not. 

Larger tract in the right hemisphere could depend on a number of factors, including greater fiber myelination, higher number of axons and larger axonal diameter that are correlated with the conduction speed13, 14. 

The SLF The authoris symmetrically distributed between left and right hemispheres (t(19) < 1); the SLF II shows a trend of right lateralization (t(19) = 1.141; p = 0.268) and the SLF III is right lateralized (t(19) = 6.083; p < 0.001). 

To test whether this lateralization was related to the attentional bias for one visualhemi–field, the authors asked the participants to perform a line bisection test11. 

In light of these previous works, the authors suggest that the left deviation in the5line bisection the authors observed in the majority of subjects may result from unbalanced speed of visuospatial processing between the two hemispheres along the SLF II.