scispace - formally typeset
Open AccessPosted ContentDOI

Meaningful Effects in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study

Reads0
Chats0
TLDR
The ABCD Study aims and design are described, as well as issues surrounding estimation of meaningful associations using its data, including population inferences, hypothesis testing, power and precision, control of covariates, interpretation of associations, and recommended best practices for reproducible research, analytical procedures and reporting of results.
Abstract
The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study is the largest single-cohort prospective longitudinal study of neurodevelopment and children9s health in the United States. A cohort of n=11,880 children aged 9-10 years (and their parents/guardians) were recruited across 22 sites and are being followed with in-person visits on an annual basis for at least 10 years. The study approximates the US population on several key sociodemographic variables, including sex, race, ethnicity, household income, and parental education. Data collected include assessments of health, mental health, substance use, culture and environment and neurocognition, as well as geocoded exposures, structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and whole-genome genotyping. Here, we describe the ABCD Study aims and design, as well as issues surrounding estimation of meaningful associations using its data, including population inferences, hypothesis testing, power and precision, control of covariates, interpretation of associations, and recommended best practices for reproducible research, analytical procedures and reporting of results.

read more

Content maybe subject to copyright    Report

NeuroImage 239 (2021) 118262
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
NeuroImage
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuroimage
Meaningful associations in the adolescent brain cognitive development
study
Anthony Steven Dick
a
, Daniel A. Lopez
b
, Ashley L. Watts
c
, Steven Heeringa
d
, Chase Reuter
e
,
Hauke Bartsch
f
, Chun Chieh Fan
g
, David N. Kennedy
h
, Clare Palmer
i
, Andrew Marshall
j
,
Frank Haist
k
, Samuel Hawes
a
, Thomas E. Nichols
l
, Deanna M. Barch
m
, Terry L. Jernigan
h
,
Hugh Garavan
n
, Steven Grant
o
, Vani Pariyadath
o
, Elizabeth Homan
p
, Michael Neale
q
,
Elizabeth A. Stuart
r
, Martin P. Paulus
s
, Kenneth J. Sher
c
, Wesley K. Thompson
e , g ,
a
Department of Psychology and Center for Children and Families, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
b
Division of Epidemiology, Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY 14642, United States
c
Department of Psychology, University of Missouri, MO, United States
d
Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, United States
e
Herbert Wertheim School of Public Health and Human Longevity Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, United States
f
Mohn Medical Imaging and Visualization Center, Department of Radiology, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
g
Population Neuroscience and Genetics Lab, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, United States
h
Department of Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, MA United States, 01604
i
Center for Human Development, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, United States
j
Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, and the Department of Pediatrics, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
k
Department of Radiology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, United States
l
Oxford Big Data Institute, Li Ka Shing Centre for Health Information and Discovery, Nuffield Department of Population Health, University of Oxford, Oxford, United
Kingdom
m
Departments of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Psychiatry and Radiology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, United States
n
Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, 05405, United States
o
Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Branch, Division of Neuroscience and Behavior, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, Department of
Health and Human Services, Bethesda, MD, United States
p
National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, Department of Heatlh and Human Services, Bethesda, MD, United States
q
Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA 23298, United States
r
Department of Biostatistics, Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
s
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, United States
Keywords:
Adolescent brain cognitive development study
Population neuroscience
Genetics
Hypothesis testing
Reproducibility
Covariate Adjustments
Eect Sizes
The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study is the largest single-cohort prospective longitudinal
study of neurodevelopment and children’s health in the United States. A cohort of n = 11,880 children aged 9–10
years (and their parents/guardians) were recruited across 22 sites and are being followed with in-person visits on
an annual basis for at least 10 years. The study approximates the US population on several key sociodemographic
variables, including sex, race, ethnicity, household income, and parental education. Data collected include assess-
ments of health, mental health, substance use, culture and environment and neurocognition, as well as geocoded
exposures, structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and whole-genome genotyping. Here, we
describe the ABCD Study aims and design, as well as issues surrounding estimation of meaningful associations
using its data, including population inferences, hypothesis testing, power and precision, control of covariates,
interpretation of associations, and recommended best practices for reproducible research, analytical procedures
and reporting of results.
1. Introduction
The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development
SM
(ABCD) Study is
the largest single-cohort long-term longitudinal study of neurodevelop-
Corresponding author.
E-mail address: wkthompson@health.ucsd.edu (W.K. Thompson).
ment and child and adolescent health in the United States. The study
was conceived and initiated by the United States’ National Institutes
of Health (NIH), with funding beginning on September 30, 2015. The
ABCD Study® collects observational data to characterize US population
trait distributions and to assess how biological, psychological, and en-
vironmental factors (including interpersonal, institutional, cultural, and
physical environments) can relate to how individuals live and develop
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118262 .
Received 13 January 2021; Received in revised form 7 May 2021; Accepted 10 June 2021
Available online 18 June 2021.
1053-8119/© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ )

A.S. Dick, D.A. Lopez, A.L. Watts et al. NeuroImage 239 (2021) 118262
in today’s society. From the outset, the NIH and ABCD scientic inves-
tigators were motivated to develop a baseline sample that reected the
sociodemographic variation present in the US population of 9–10 year-
old children, and to follow them longitudinally through adolescence and
into early adulthood.
The ABCD Study was designed to address some of the most im-
portant public health questions facing today’s children and adolescents
( Volkow et al., 2018 ). These questions include identifying factors lead-
ing to the initiation and consumption patterns of psychoactive sub-
stances, substance-related problems, and substance use disorders as well
as their subsequent impact on the brain, neurocognition, health, and
mental health over the course of adolescence and into early adulthood.
More broadly, a large epidemiologically informed longitudinal study be-
ginning in childhood and continuing on through early adulthood will
provide a wealth of unique data on normative development, as well as
environmental and biological factors associated with variation in devel-
opmental trajectories. This broader perspective has led to the involve-
ment of multiple NIH Institutes that are stakeholders in the range of
health outcomes targeted in the ABCD design. (Information regarding
funding agencies, recruitment sites, investigators, and project organiza-
tion can be obtained at https://abcdstudy.org ).
Population representativeness, or more precisely, absence of uncor-
rected selection bias in the subject pool, is important in achieving exter-
nal validity, i.e., the ability to generalize specic results of the study to
US society at large. As described below, the ABCD Study attempted to
match the diverse US population of 9–10 year-old children on key de-
mographic characteristics. However, even with a largely representative
sample, failure to account for key confounders can aect internal valid-
ity, i.e., the degree to which observed associations accurately reect the
eects of underlying causal mechanisms. Moreover, it is crucial that the
study collects a rich array of variables that may act as moderators or
mediators, including biological and environmental variables, in order
to aid in identifying potentially causal pathways of interest, to quan-
tify individualized risk for (or resilience to) poor outcomes, and to in-
form public policy decisions. External and internal validity also depend
on assessing the impact of random and systematic measurement error,
implementing analytical methods that incorporate relevant aspects of
study design, and emphasizing robust and replicable estimation of asso-
ciations.
The ABCD Study primary aims are given in the Supplementary Ma-
terials (SM) Section S.1. We describe the study design and outline an-
alytic strategies to address the primary study aims, including worked
examples, with emphasis on approaches that incorporate relevant as-
pects of study design ( Section 2 : Study Design; Section 3 : Population
Weighting). We emphasize the impact of sample size on the precision of
association estimates and thoughtful control of covariates in the context
of the large-scale population neuroscience data produced by the ABCD
Study ( Section 4 : Hypothesis Testing and Power; Section 5 : Eect Sizes;
Section 6 : Control and Confounding Variables), and we briey outline
state-of-the-eld recommendations for promoting reproducible science
(Section SM.5) and best practices for statistical analyses and reporting
of results using the ABCD Study data (Section SM.6). For readability,
more technical subject matter is also largely left to SM sections.
2. Study design
The ABCD Study is a prospective longitudinal cohort study of US chil-
dren born between 2006 and 2008. A total cohort of 𝑛 = 11 , 880 children
aged 9–10 years at baseline (and their parents/guardians) was recruited
from 22 sites (with one site no longer active) and are being followed for
at least ten years. Eligible children were recruited from the household
populations in dened catchment areas for each of the study sites dur-
ing the roughly two-year period beginning September 2016 and ending
in October 2018.
2.1. Recruitment
Within study sites, consenting parents and assenting children were
primarily recruited through a probability sample of public and private
schools augmented to a smaller extent by special recruitment through
summer camp programs and community volunteers. ABCD employed
a probability sampling strategy to identify schools within the catch-
ment areas as the primary method for contacting and recruiting eligible
children and their parents. This method has been used in other large
national studies (e.g., Monitoring the Future ( Bachman et al., 2011 );
the Add Health Study ( Chantala and Tabor, 1999 ); the National Co-
morbidity Replication-Adolescent Supplement ( Conway et al., 2016 );
and the National Education Longitudinal Studies ( Ingels et al., 1990 )).
Twins at four “twin-hub ”sites were recruited from birth registries (see
( Garavan et al., 2018 ; Iacono et al., 2017 ) for participant recruitment
details). A minority of participants were recruited through non-school-
based community outreach and word-of-mouth referrals.
2.2. Inclusion criteria
Across recruitment sites, inclusion criteria consisted of being in the
required age range and able to provide informed consent (parents) and
assent (child). Exclusions were minimal and were limited to lack of En-
glish language prociency in the children, the presence of severe sen-
sory, intellectual, medical or neurological issues that would impact the
validity of collected data or the child’s ability to comply with the pro-
tocol, and contraindications to MRI scanning ( Garavan et al., 2018 ).
Parents must be uent in either English or Spanish.
2.3. Measures
Measures collected in the ABCD Study include a neurocognitive bat-
tery ( Luciana et al., 2018 ; Thompson et al., 2019 ), mental and physical
health assessments ( Barch et al., 2018 ), measures of culture and envi-
ronment ( Zucker et al., 2018 ), biospecimens ( Uban et al., 2018 ), struc-
tural and functional brain imaging ( Casey et al., 2018 ; Hagler et al.,
2018 ), geolocation-based environmental exposure data, wearables and
mobile technology ( Bagot et al., 2018 ), and whole genome genotyping
( Loughnan et al., 2020 ). Many of these measures are collected at in-
person annual visits, with brain imaging collected at baseline and at
every other year going forward. A limited number of assessments are
collected in semi-annual telephone interviews between in-person visits.
Data are publicly released on an annual basis through the NIMH Data
Archive (NDA, https://nda.nih.gov/abcd ). Fig. 1 graphically displays
the measures that have been collected as part of the ABCD NDA 3.0.
Release. Fig. 2 depicts the planned data collection and release schedule
over the initial 10 years of the study.
2.4. Sociodemographics
ABCD sample baseline demographics (from NDA Release 2.0.1,
which contains data from 𝑛 = 11 , 879 subjects) are presented in Table 1 ,
along with a comparison to the corresponding statistics from the Amer-
ican Community Survey (ACS). The ACS is a large probability sample
survey of US households conducted annually by the US Bureau of Census
and provides a benchmark for selected demographic and socioeconomic
characteristics of US children aged 9–10 years. The 2011–2015 ACS Pub-
lic Use Microsample (PUMS) le provides data on over 8000,000 sample
US households. Included in this ve-year national sample of households
are 376,370 individual observations for children aged 9–10 and their
households.
With some minor dierences, the unweighted distributions for the
ABCD baseline sample closely match the ACS-based national estimates
for demographic characteristics including age, sex, and household size.
2

A.S. Dick, D.A. Lopez, A.L. Watts et al. NeuroImage 239 (2021) 118262
Fig. 1. ABCD Study Assessments for NDA 2.0.1
Release Data.
The general concordance of the samples can be attributed in large part
to three factors: 1) the inherent demographic diversity across the ABCD
study sites; 2) stratication (by race/ethnicity) in the probability sam-
pling of schools within sites; and 3) demographic controls employed
in the recruitment by site teams. Likewise, the unweighted percent-
ages of ABCD children for the most prevalent race/ethnicity categories
are an approximate match to the ACS estimates for US children age
9 and 10. Collectively, children of Asian, American Indian/Alaska Na-
tive (AIAN) and Native Hawaiian/Pacic Islander (NHPI) ancestry are
under-represented in the unweighted ABCD data (3.2%) compared with
ACS national estimates (5.9%). This outcome, which primarily aects
ABCD’s sample of Asian children, may be due in part to dierences in
how the parent/caregiver of the child reports multiple race/ethnicity
ancestry in ABCD and the ACS.
3.
Population inferences
The ABCD recruitment eort worked very hard to maintain similar-
ity of the ABCD sample and the US population with respect to sex and
race/ethnicity of the children in the study. The predominantly proba-
bility sampling methodology for recruiting children within each study
site was intended to randomize over confounding factors that were not
explicitly controlled (or subsequently reected in the population weight-
ing). Nevertheless, school consent and parental consent were strong
forces that certainly may have altered the eectiveness of the random-
ization over these uncontrolled confounders.
3.1. Population weighting
The purpose of population weighting is to control for specic
sources of selection bias and restore unbiasedness to descriptive and
analytical estimates of the population characteristics and relationships
( Heeringa et al., 2017 ). Briey, construction of the population weights
required identication of a key set of demographic and socioeconomic
variables for the children and their households that are measured in
both the ABCD Study and in the ACS household interviews. For the
ABCD eligible children, the common variables include 1) age; 2) sex;
and 3) race/ethnicity. For the child’s household, additional variables
include: 4) family income; 5) family type (married parents, single par-
ent); 6) household size 7) parents’ work force status (family type by
parent employment status); 8) Census Region. A multiple logistic re-
gression model using these variables was then t to the concatenated
ACS and ABCD data to predict study membership. The construction
of the population weights for the ABCD Study is described in detail
in Heeringa and Berglund (2020) ( Heeringa and Berglund, 2020 ). R
scripts for computing the ABCD population weights and for applying
them in analyses are available at https://github.com/ABCD-STUDY/
abcd _ acs _ raked _ propensity . The population weights are available in the
NDA data releases 2.0.1 and 3.0.
3.2. Recommendations
Heeringa and Berglund (2020) ( Heeringa and Berglund, 2020 )
present regression analyses with and without using the population
weights. Although it is important not to over-generalize from a small
set of comparisons to all possible analyses of the ABCD data, the re-
sults described therein lead to recommendations for researchers who
are analyzing the ABCD baseline data. First, unweighted analysis may
result in biased estimates of descriptive population statistics. The po-
tential for bias in unweighted estimates from the ABCD data is strongest
when the variable of interest is highly correlated with socioeconomic
variables including family income, family type and parental work force
3

A.S. Dick, D.A. Lopez, A.L. Watts et al. NeuroImage 239 (2021) 118262
Fig. 2. ABCD Data Collection and NDA Release Schedule.
participation. Second, for regression models of the ABCD baseline data,
an unweighted analysis using mixed-eects models (e.g., site, family,
individual) is the preferred choice. Presently, there is no empirical ev-
idence from comparative analyses that methods for multi-level weight-
ing ( Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal, 2006 ) will improve the accuracy or
precision of the model t, although additional research on this topic is
ongoing.
3.3. Example: Application to baseline brain volumes
As a demonstration of the implications of the weighting strategy em-
ployed in the ABCD Study, weighted and unweighted means and stan-
dard errors for ABCD baseline brain morphometry - volumes of cortical
Desikan parcels ( Desikan et al., 2006 ) - are presented in Table 2 . Missing
observations were rst imputed using the R package mice ( van Buuren
and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2011 ) before applying weights to the com-
pleted sample. Dierences between unweighted and weighted means are
quite small in the baseline sample in this case. As longitudinal MRI data
become available in ABCD (starting with the second post-baseline an-
nual follow-up visit), population-valid mean trajectories of brain-related
outcomes will also be computable using a similar population weighting
scheme, also allowing for characterization of variation of trajectories
from the population mean.
4.
Hypothesis testing and power
Developing an operational approach to evaluate the meaningfulness
of research ndings has been a subject of consistent debate throughout
the history of statistics (
Stigler, 1986 ). Even with the continued eorts
to synthesize systems of statistical inference ( Efron and Hastie, 2016 ),
the resolution of this issue is unlikely to occur any time soon. Most neu-
roscientists continue to work within the context of the classical frequen-
tist null-hypothesis signicance testing (NHST) paradigm ( Efron, 1998 ;
Lehmann, 1993 ), although non-frequentist approaches (e.g. Bayesian,
machine learning prediction ( Efron, 2013 ; Efron, 2020 )) are increas-
ingly common and may be more appropriate for large datasets like the
ABCD Study.
Despite growing enthusiasm for these alternatives, p-values continue
to be important data points in the presentation of results in the be-
havioral and neurosciences. The NHST p-value “…is the probability
under a specied statistical model that a statistical summary of the
data…would be equal to or more extreme than its observed value
(
Wasserstein and Lazar, 2016 ). The utility of NHST and the arbitrariness
of the 0 . 05 signicance threshold has been debated extensively ( Gelman,
2018 ; Wasserstein and Lazar, 2016 ; Nickerson, 2000 ; Harlow et al.,
2013 ). While we will not relitigate these issues here, we will at-
tempt to address how best to present statistical evidence that lever-
ages the ABCD Study’s large sample size (aecting statistical power),
population sampling frame, and rich longitudinal assessment proto-
col to enable meaningful and valid insights into child and adolescent
neurodevelopment.
4.1. Power
Statistical power in the NHST framework is dened as the probabil-
ity of rejecting a false null hypothesis. Power is determined by three
4

A.S. Dick, D.A. Lopez, A.L. Watts et al. NeuroImage 239 (2021) 118262
Table 1
ABCD Baseline and ACS 2011–2015 Demographic Characteristics.
Characteristic Category ABCD ( n = 11,879) ACS 2011–2015
% N %
Population Total 100 8211,605 100
Age 9 52.3 4074,807 49.6
10 47.8 4136,798 50.4
Sex Male 52.2 4205,925 51.2
Female 47.8 4005,860 48.8
Race/Ethnicity NH White 52.2 4305,552 52.4
NH Black 15.1 1101,297 13.4
Hispanic 20.4 1973,827 24.0
Asian, AIAN, NHPI 3.2 487,673 5.9
Multiple 9.2 343,256 4.2
Family Income <
$25k 16.1 1762,415 21.5
$25k - $49k 15.1 1784,747 21.7
$50k - $74k 14.0 1397,641 17.0
$75k - $99k 14.1 1023,127 12.5
$100k - $199k 29.5 1685,036 20.5
$200k + 11.2 558,639 6.8
Family Type Married Parents 73.4 5426,131 66.1
Other Family Type 26.6 2785,474 33.9
Parent Employment Married, 2
in LF 50.2 3353,572 40.8
Married, 1 in LF 21.9 1949,288 23.7
Married, 0 in LF 1.3 156,807 1.9
Single, in LF 21.1 2174,365 26.5
Single, Not in LF 5.4 577,573 7.0
Region Northeast 16.9 1336,183 16.3
Midwest 20.4 1775,723 21.6
South 28.3 3117,158 38.0
West 34.4 1982,541 24.1
Household Size
2 to 3 17.3 1522,216 18.5
4 33.5 2751,942 33.5
5 24.9 2085,666 25.4
6 14.0 1025,285 12.5
7 + 10.3 826,496 10.1
LF = labor force.
ACS = American Community Survey.
factors: 1) the signicance level 𝛼; 2) the magnitude of the population
parameter; and 3) the accuracy (precision and bias) of the model es-
timates. Increasing power while maintaining a specied Type I error
rate depends largely on obtaining more precise association parameter
estimates from improved study designs, more ecient statistical meth-
ods, and, importantly, increasing sample size ( Rothman et al., 2008 ;
Button et al., 2013 ; Hong and Park, 2012 ).
The ABCD Study has a large sample compared to typical neurode-
velopmental studies, so much so that one might expect even very small
associations to be statistically signicant. In our experience, not all as-
sociations in the ABCD Study are guaranteed to have small p-values. For
example, a recent study attempting to replicate the often-cited bilingual
executive function advantage failed to nd evidence for the advantage in
the rst data release (NDA 1.0) of the ABCD Study ( 𝑛 = 4524 ) ( Dick et al.,
2019 ).
Nevertheless, even very small associations are well-powered in the
ABCD Study. Fig. 3 displays power curves as a function of sample size
for dierent values of absolute Pearson correlations |𝑟 |. The dashed line
in Fig. 3 indicates the full ABCD baseline sample size of 𝑛 = 11 , 880 .
As can be seen, Pearson correlations |r| = 0.04 and above have power
> 0 . 99 at 𝛼 = 0 . 05 . Simply rejecting a null hypothesis without reporting
on other aspects of the study design and statistical analyses (includ-
ing discussion of plausible alternative explanatory models and threats
to validity), as well as the observed magnitude of associations, is unin-
formative, perhaps particularly so in the context of very well-powered
studies ( Abadie, 2020 ).
5. Effect sizes
Because p-values may be less informative in the context of very well-
powered studies like ABCD, eect sizes become important data points
in determining the importance of the ndings. Eect sizes quantify
relationships between two or more variables, e.g., correlation coe-
cients, proportion of variance explained ( 𝑅
2
), Cohen’s 𝑑, relative risk,
number needed to treat, and so forth ( Cohen, 1988 ; Kraemer, 1992 ;
Rosenthal et al., 2000 ), with one variable often thought of as indepen-
dent (exposure) and the other dependent (outcome) ( Rothman et al.,
2008 ). Eect sizes are independent of sample size, e.g., t-tests and p-
values are not eect sizes; however, the precision of eect size estima-
tors depend on sample size as described earlier. Consensus best practice
recommendations are that eect size point estimates be accompanied
by intervals to illustrate the precision of the estimate and the conse-
quent range of plausible values indicated by the data ( Wasserstein and
Lazar, 2016 ). Table 3 presents a number of commonly used eect size
metrics ( Kirk, 1996 ; Fidler et al., 2004 ). We wish to avoid being overly
prescriptive for which of these eect sizes to employ in ABCD applica-
tions, as researchers should think carefully about the intended use of
their analyses and pick an eect size metric that addresses their partic-
ular research question.
5.1. Small effects
As much as the choice of which eect size statistic to report is driven
by context, the interpretation of the practical utility of the observed ef-
fect size is even more so. While small p-values do not imply that reported
eects are inherently substantive, “small ”eect sizes might have prac-
tical or even clinical signicance in the right context ( Rosenthal et al.,
2000 ).
As described in SM Section S.2, known problems of publication
bias and incentives for researchers to nd signicant associations
( Button et al., 2013 ; Simonsohn et al., 2014 ) combined with the pre-
dominantly small sample sizes of most prior neurodevelopmental stud-
5

Citations
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

New insights into the dynamic development of the cerebral cortex in childhood and adolescence: Integrating macro- and microstructural MRI findings.

TL;DR: In this paper, a review of recent findings of typical postnatal macro-and microstructural development of the cerebral cortex from early childhood to young adulthood is presented, integrating imaging studies with cortical gene expression findings to further understand the underlying neurobiology of the developmental changes, bridging the gap between ex vivo histological-and in vivo MRI studies.
Journal ArticleDOI

Development of white matter microstructure and executive functions during childhood and adolescence: a review of diffusion MRI studies

TL;DR: In this article, the authors provide a brief overview of diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (dMRI) methods and how they can be used to study white matter and connectivity and review the extant literature examining the links between dMRI indices and executive functions during development.
Journal ArticleDOI

Understanding Vulnerability and Adaptation in Early Brain Development using Network Neuroscience.

TL;DR: Early adversity influences brain development and emerging behavioral phenotypes relevant for psychiatric disorders as discussed by the authors, which has implications for contextualizing current public health crises and pervasive health inequities, and highlight targets for future research.
Journal ArticleDOI

A history of previous childbirths is linked to women's white matter brain age in midlife and older age.

TL;DR: The authors investigated the association between previous childbirths and white matter brain age in 8,895 women in the UK Biobank cohort (age range = 54-81 years) and found that the number of previous childbirth events was negatively associated with white brain age, potentially indicating a protective effect of parity on white matter later in life.
References
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI

mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R

TL;DR: Mice adds new functionality for imputing multilevel data, automatic predictor selection, data handling, post-processing imputed values, specialized pooling routines, model selection tools, and diagnostic graphs.
Journal ArticleDOI

Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience

TL;DR: It is shown that the average statistical power of studies in the neurosciences is very low, and the consequences include overestimates of effect size and low reproducibility of results.
Journal ArticleDOI

Statistical Power Analysis

TL;DR: The application of positron emis sion tomography (PEM) to the study of panic disorder was discussed in this paper, where a focal brain abnormal ity in panic disorder, a severe form of anxiety, was discussed.

Why Most Published Research Findings Are False

TL;DR: In this paper, the authors discuss the implications of these problems for the conduct and interpretation of research and suggest that claimed research findings may often be simply accurate measures of the prevailing bias.
Related Papers (5)

Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study.

Donald J. Hagler, +144 more
- 15 Nov 2019 - 
Frequently Asked Questions (1)
Q1. What are the contributions in "Meaningful associations in the adolescent brain cognitive development study" ?

The study approximates the US population on several key sociodemographic variables, including sex, race, ethnicity, household income, and parental education. Here, the authors describe the ABCD Study aims and design, as well as issues surrounding estimation of meaningful associations using its data, including population inferences, hypothesis testing, power and precision, control of covariates, interpretation of associations, and recommended best practices for reproducible research, analytical procedures and reporting of results.